Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 November 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Children's Drawings: Evidence-based Research and Practice View all 18 articles

Draw yourself: How culture influences drawings by children between the ages of two and fifteen

\r\nSophie RestoySophie Restoy1Lison MartinetLison Martinet1Cdric Sueur,Cédric Sueur1,2Marie Pel*&#x;Marie Pelé3*†
  • 1Université de Strasbourg, CNRS, IPHC UMR 7178, Strasbourg, France
  • 2Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
  • 3Anthropo-Lab, ETHICS EA 7446, Université Catholique de Lille, Lille, France

The place children live strongly influence how they develop their behavior, this is also true for pictorial expression. This study is based on 958 self-portraits drawn by children aged 2–15 years old from 35 countries across 5 continents. A total of 13 variables were extracted of each drawing allowing us to investigate the differences of individuals and environment representations in these drawings. We used a principal component analysis to understand how drawing characteristics can be combined in pictorial concepts. We analyzed the effect of age, gender, socioeconomic, and cultural factors in terms of complexity and inclusion of social (human figures) and physical (element from Nature and man-made elements) environments, their frequencies, size, and proportions of these elements on each drawing. Our results confirm the existence of cultural variations and the influence of age on self-portrait patterns. We also observed an influence of physical and socio-cultural contexts through the level of urbanization and the degree of individualism of the countries, which have affected the complexity, content and representation of human figures in the drawings studied.

Introduction

Drawing is one of the different ways that human beings express themselves (Lange-Küttner, 2020). This was already the case thousands years ago, as shown by the Sulawesi warty pig painting found on the walls of a cave in Indonesia and dated to at least 45,500 years ago (Brumm et al., 2021). Long seen as the prerogative of modern humans, the findings of older traces cast doubt on the identification of the first species to show drawing behavior. To date, human lineage alone has the capacity to produce figurative drawings (DeLoache, 2004). In young children, drawing is a fundamental early activity as they have not mastered verbal language at this age (Wallon et al., 1998). The analysis of children’s drawings became a very active and pluridisciplinary research area from the end of the 19th century onward, and are now a central interest for many researchers who hypothesize that children’s drawing productions represent their state of mind, thus providing a doorway into their internal world (Rübeling et al., 2011).

But how should we define drawing? This term simultaneously defines an action (a behavior) and an artifact (the result of this behavior) (Kourkoulis, 2021). Drawing behavior makes it possible to represent objects, thoughts or feelings through visible graphic elements, regardless of whether they are interpretable or not (Martinet et al., 2021). Humans therefore use drawing as a projective tool that can make their internal life, perceptions and experiences visible (Ouedraogo, 2015). This “mirror of the soul” reflects the representativeness of a drawing, and results from the perspectives of both, the one who produces it and the one who observes it. Thus, the child who draws possess an internal representativeness of his or her work, while the adult who analyses the drawing only perceives its external representativeness (Martinet et al., 2021). The more similar the representativeness of both perspectives is, the more convinced we are that children world can be understood from their drawing. A drawing is therefore described as figurative when it can be read without ambiguity by others and permits communication (Kourkoulis, 2021), whatever its degree of realism (Willats, 2005). The analysis of this drawing allows us to understand certain aspects of representations by children, and namely their self-image.

In this respect, it is therefore essential to take into account the development of drawing behavior in humans (Toku, 2001; Lange-Küttner, 2008, 2014). Pictorial expression is made possible by the interaction of three ontogenic stages. The first is the improvement of the child’s motor coordination. The second is the development of perceptual skills, which increase with improved levels of attention. And finally, the third corresponds to cognitive skills, notably when the child understands the symbolic meaning of objects and establishes pictorial repertoires (Toomela, 2002). The neurological development occurring during early childhood (0–4 years) allows children to understand themselves and others, at around the age of three, and gives them the ability to communicate with the help of visual symbols (DeLoache, 2004). Children motor and cognitive development is therefore the root of the progressive complexity of their drawings.

Many researchers (Luquet, 1927; Lowenfeld and Brittain, 1987; Baldy, 2005; Royer, 2005; Marcilhacy and Demirdjian, 2011) concur that regardless of the environment in which the drawing child has grown, there is a progression that begins with scribblings, then figurative sketches and finally detailed drawings (Golomb, 1992). Toward the age of two, children begin to draw and include certain graphical elements that they seek to reproduce (Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014). These first artistic productions are referred to as scribbles (Kellogg, 1970). At the age of 3–4 years, the first representations of human figures appear in the form of tadpole figures. Subsequently, entering infant school provides children with an opportunity to draw, write and understand socially shared meanings (Cox, 1992). This training increases their drawing experience and their use of figurative signifiers (Martinet et al., 2021). Thus, children add external representativeness to their drawing from the age of 4–5 years (Baldy, 2005). Their productions then become more and more differentiated and complex with age (Golomb, 1992), before reaching a critical period at puberty (i.e., period of oppression) (Cohn, 2012) when their drawing activity ceases in favor of verbal language, which is more flexible and economical (Baldy, 2011). An important point is that this critical period as well as other stages of graphic development are not always found at the same ages or in the same way from one culture to another (Toku, 2001; Cohn, 2014).

Drawing behavior is therefore composed of an innate element–a blind individual not exposed to a graphic universe will have resilient capacities to produce a rudimentary figurative drawing (Millar, 1975; Golomb, 1992; Andersson and Andersson, 2009; Cohn, 2012)- but also has another culturally acquired component (Cohn, 2012; Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014). Indeed, the existence of the above general models does not rule out the influence of environmental factors (Rübeling et al., 2011). The recognized importance of cultural variations have changed the way drawing behavior was perceived; it would henceforth be seen in more flexible and diversified terms, some researchers going so far as to speak of graphic language (Goodnow et al., 1995; Baldy, 2011). Verbal language and graphic expression therefore share many similarities and particularly their culturally specific character (Kourkoulis, 2021). The UNESCO defines culture as a set of distinctive spiritual, material, intellectual, and emotional features that characterize a society or a social group. An organized social unit, whether familial, societal or media-based, thus holds its own understanding of the world which is transmitted throughout generations. In this way, children gradually develop a culturally informed understanding of themselves and others (Rübeling et al., 2011; Kourkoulis, 2021). The eco-cultural approach defines two major developmental strategies. In the first, each person sees themselves as part of an economically and socially interdependent whole: this is called the interdependence strategy. Conversely, the self is central in the second strategy, and the individuals see themselves as unique and separate from others: This is called the independence strategy (Keller, 2007). Children therefore have a different perception of themselves and others according to their culture (Markus and Kitayama, 1991), and this can be observed through their drawings (Rübeling et al., 2011). These two strategies will influence many human behaviors as the way pedestrians cross the road (Pelé et al., 2017), how they share rewards (Kim et al., 1990) or knowledge (Moss et al., 2007). Then, if asked to realize a self-portrait, we may hypothesize that a child from an individualistic culture will represent him/herself in bigger proportions compared to a child who grows in a more collectivist one. In this latter case, we can imagine that the child will implement other people or others elements in his/her self-portrait.

Paying attention to the influence of culture on drawings is relatively recent (Jolley et al., 2010), but some authors have confirmed the existence of cultural variations (Lindström, 2000; Cox et al., 2001; Toku, 2001; Cohn, 2014; Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014). When children draw human figures, they do not seek to represent an anatomically correct body but rather draw people (Rübeling et al., 2011) by varying the height, facial details (Gernhardt et al., 2015, 2016), and facial expressions (La Voy et al., 2001) of these representations. Studies based on qualitative data give examples of the influence of culture on young people who draw. Cox et al. (2001) observed that Japanese children draw motionless figures that are seen from the front or in profile, immobile or running, and that these drawings were of better quality than those drawn by children in the United Kingdom. The authors attribute this superiority to the influence of manga comics on Japanese children. Nomoto (2007) compares the Rey complex figures drawn by French and Japanese children and how these figures evolve over time. He found that while French children attach importance to the overall effect of their drawing and constantly improve the proportions of the figures they draw; Japanese children show meticulous attention to detail and draw increasingly detailed figures. Existing researches suggest that these trends may reflect the differences between the two education systems: The Japanese tradition encourages imitation and insists on the importance of paying attention to details, whereas French education encourages a more global and spontaneous cognition style (Cohn, 2014). The self-portrait is a good means to observe the cultural influence on drawing. Indeed, the predominant cultural model is imprinted on our sense of identity, and reflects partly the degree of attachment we have for the elements surrounding us (Kourkoulis, 2021). The presence or absence of environmental details in the drawings therefore appears to testify their value to the child (Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014).

Studies have observed the understanding of the self and of the social world from the details of drawn human figures, but few have closely examined the elements of environment (elements from Nature and man-made objects) depicted in these drawings. On the same way, only few studies focused on the influence of the level of urbanization on the drawings of children (Rübeling et al., 2011). Moreover, the number of cultures and countries considered in cross-cultural studies is also very limited. Current research mostly focuses on Joseph Henrich’s W.E.I.R.D. acronym, which designates Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic societies (Henrich et al., 2010); it is therefore important to study other societies in order to obtain a clear and global picture of drawing behavior in our species. Our study sought to observe the differences in the representation of the individual and of their environments (social and physical) in self-portraits drawn by children from a large number of countries. We analyzed a total of 958 scanned and available self-portraits produced by children aged 2–15. These children were from 35 countries located on 5 different continents (Porte, 2012). In total, we measured 13 indices-based on classical studies of drawing in children (level of complexity, Baldy, 2005; representation of human figures, Rübeling et al., 2011) but also representation of non-human elements (from Nature and man-made objects)-that we implemented in a principal component analyses (PCA). PCA is used to extract and visualize important information contained in a multivariate data table by combining metrics to form a biologically or psychologically significant dimension, as already shown for personality (Wolf and Weissing, 2012; Bousquet et al., 2015), sociality (Viblanc et al., 2016) or even for drawing (Sueur et al., 2021). This method is expected to combine indices in dimensions linked to important psychological or cultural concepts as the self-representation, the family, or the importance of elements from Nature or man-made objects. We expected that these dimensions will be directly linked to the socioeconomic and cultural environment of children. We also expected drawings to be exclusively self-portraits in individualistic societies (i.e., independence strategy) and to be opened to the family or to elements of Nature in collectivist ones (i.e., interdependence strategy). Children from urban areas may have access to a more graphical environment (architecture, advertising, etc.) which can contribute to the way they represent themselves. Considering that self-representation develops from scribbling to the tadpole figure and finally to more detailed and realistic drawings, we also sought to determine whether any aspects of the drawings could be associated with the age and the gender of the children.

Materials and methods

Collection of drawings

The drawings are 958 self-portraits by children (468 girls; 475 boys; 15 not stated) aged 2–15, from 35 countries spread over 5 continents. In each country, drawings were collected in one to four towns (details given in Supplementary Table 1). They are taken from the Early Pictures online archive and were collected between 2005 and 2012 by the French photographer Gilles Porte1. Analysis concerns only one drawing per child. The drawings presented by Gilles Porte were produced according to the following procedure: Children were given a sheet of black paper in format A5 and a white pencil (crayon). The crayon was prepared (sharpened) in advance. Children were asked to position the paper in “portrait” format when drawing. After such preparation, the children were asked to draw themselves. No further comment was given, and the time of drawing was free. When they finished, they gave back the papers, and names and ages were indicated on the back of drawings by Gilles Porte and the adults in charge on site. The satisfactory balance of this database allowed us to consider both age and gender of children (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Numbers of drawings according the age and gender of children.

Ethical note

Children’s participation in the creation of this database was on a voluntary basis and was subject to school approval and parental consent. The photographer, Gilles Porte, was given help and permission from Non-Governmental Organizations and the UNESCO to approach certain populations. We have respected the license of this database insofar which was strictly used for research purposes at the Hubert Curien Pluridisciplinary Institute (CNRS), a state-recognized research institute. The database was solely used to discuss the pictorial development of children, with no individual psychological or psychoanalytic interpretation. The ethical rules of this database have therefore been respected.

Drawing analysis

All measures were made by SR and double-checked by MP following ethological sampling (Pelé et al., 2021; Sueur et al., 2021). For each drawing, the following stages of analysis were performed.

Interpretation of the drawing

Each drawing was coded as figurative if it had an unambiguously recognizable external representation, non-figurative if no graphic element could be interpreted by an outside observer, or mixed if it contained both figurative and non-figurative elements.

As previously said, children can represent elements or people that are valuable for them in their drawings (Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014; Kourkoulis, 2021). So, in figurative and mixed drawings, we specifically looked at representation of human figures but also at non-human elements (Nature, man-made objects, and symbols).

Representation of human figures

Concerning the representations of human figures, we first used a recognized classification model that could determine their complexity. This is known as Baldy classification (Baldy, 2005), which defines six stages of transformation of the morphology of the human figure: The round and enumerated human figure (stage 1), the tadpole (stage 2), the intermediary (stage 3), the conventional man that is first filiform (stage 4) then tube-shaped man (stage 5), and finally the outline (stage 6). A mixed intermediate stage (stage 4b) has been added here to define the men composed of single and double lines. A lower (stage 0) has been created for non-figurative level drawings (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Examples of the different types of human figures examined in our study.

As the proportion and the level of details can differ from culture to another (Cox et al., 2001; Nomoto, 2007; Cohn, 2014), we then noted the number of human figures present in each drawing (for one individual, see Figures 3A–C,E,F; for several individuals see Figures 3D,G,H) and the presence (and the number) of sensory organs in the human figures (eyes, nose, mouth, ear, and hand).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Examples of drawings. Four types of non-human figures have been considered: elements representing nature (e.g., tree, flowers, sun; A,B), man-made objects (e.g., house, car, and boat, B–E,G), symbols (F), and characters (i.e., letters or numbers, E).

Considering proportion of the human figures in the drawings, we measured the size of each head and body drawn by children using the GIMP 2.10.22 software (Peck, 2006). The precision of measurements was improved through the use of a 10 × 10 grid (Casti, 2016) as well as compass and selection tools. GIMP was also used to determine the minimum convex polygons (MCP) which is the smallest polygon that can be drawn around the extreme location points, and which has angles that all measure less than 180 degrees. Commonly used to estimate the home range of animals (Nilsen et al., 2008), MCP was calculated here to measure the coverage of the human figure on the sheet. MCP varies from 0 (no drawing at all) to 100% (the drawing covered the entire sheet). All of these analyses allowed us to add four new indices from our raw data, namely the complexity of the face, i.e., the sum of the observed facial elements ranging from 0 to 5; the proportion of the head to the body size for drawings of human figures (head-to-body ratio in centimeters); the size of drawings and the size of human figures as a percentage of the drawing sheet’s coverage. We achieved this by dividing the minimum convex polygon by the total number of pixels of the sheet.

Representation of non-human elements

Four types of non-human figures have been considered: Elements representing Nature (e.g., tree, flowers, and sun; see Figures 3A,B), man-made objects (e.g., house, car, boat, and figure; see Figures 3B–E,G), symbols (e.g., heart and stars, see Figure 3F), and characters (i.e., letters or numbers, see Figure 3E). For each of these categories, the number of elements present in each drawing have been noted and since these different elements can be found on the same drawing, they are not exclusive. We also observed whether these drawings included a personification of Nature by noting the presence of facial details on the natural elements represented (see Figures 3B,G).

At the end of drawing analyses, a total of 13 variables were analyzed for each drawing, namely: (1) Its level of complexity [based on Baldy’s (2005) classification] and (2) its type (figurative, non-figurative, and mixed); the presence of (3) human faces and hands, (4) elements from Nature and (5) man-made objects, and (6) the personification of 4 and 5; (7) representations of animals, (8) additional human figures, (9) symbols, (10) letters and numbers, and finally the size of (11) head of human figure (ratio head/body), (12) human figures (MCP), and (13) entire drawings (MCP) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Metric loading for the five PCA dimensions of our data.

Specific indices of culture

As our study aimed to observe the influence of socioeconomic and cultural conditions on drawing behavior of children, data for three specific indices were collected to categorize each country. The first is the Individualism Index Value (IDV), which assesses the links between individuals and the members of their community as a degree measured on a scale of 0–100. A high IDV value indicates that the society can be considered more individualistic (i.e., an independent strategy), while a low value indicates a more community-based society (i.e., an interdependent strategy) (for details about factors included into this metric, see Hofstede, 2010). The second is the Urban Development Index (UDI) which corresponds to the percentage of the population living in urban areas compared to the total population of a country (Molinaro et al., 2020). Indeed, a child living in urban areas has access to a more graphical environment (architecture, advertising, etc.) which can contribute to the way he or she will draw and represent him/herself. However, a child in a largely rural country could have been recruited in a large city, while a child from a largely urban country from a small village whilst children were recruited in schools for which we can assume that they were not only passing through. Since urbanization of a country is not dichotomous but better corresponds to a scale we also and lastly consider the Inequality-Adjusted Human Development Index (IHDI). This index was developed by the United-Nations Development Program and reflects life expectancy, level of education and standard of living (access to culture, goods, services, and transport), and takes the country’s inequalities into account.

In order to facilitate our data analysis and observe the influence of the main cultural trends, we split the 35 countries into six groups of culturally similar countries (CSC): Central and South America (CSA), Western countries (W), North and West Africa (NWA), Southern Africa (SA), Middle East (ME), and Asian countries (A) (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. World map showing the countries and cultures studied.

Statistical analyses

Once collected for each drawing, our 13 variables were grouped into interpretable dimensions via principal component analysis (PCA) performed with the R FactoMineR package (Lê et al., 2008). PCA is used to extract and visualize important information contained in a multivariate data table by combining metrics to form a biologically or psychologically significant dimension. Here, we aimed and expected metrics to be combined and form dimensions that correspond to cultural (CSC groups) or biological (i.e., age and gender) aspects of the drawing and show performance or aestheticism in the drawing (Dissanayake, 2001; Matthews, 2003). The dimensions obtained were used as response variables, and the coordinates of each drawing in each dimension allowed us to compare the drawings according to the age, gender, IHDI-IDV-UDI, and CSC group of the children. For this, we removed some missing values of our data set (27 drawings from Roma community because the living country of the children were undetermined, four drawings for which gender was not known, and four drawings for which age was not known).

In order to test the potential effect of our multiple independent variables (gender-age-CSCgroup-IHDI-IDV-UDI), we performed a multivariate linear regression model for each dimension of our PCA using the lm() function from the R car package (Fox and Weisberg, 2019). The potential collinearity between our predictor variables was tested by calculating the variance inflation factor (VIF) of the car package (Fox and Weisberg, 2019). This calculation enabled us to remove the IHDI variable that was too strongly correlated with the CSC group variable (VIF > 11) and confirm the absence of problematic (multi) collinearity with other variables (VIF < 4). Our models thus included two factors, namely gender (2 levels) and CSC group (6 levels), and 3 numerical variables, i.e., age, IDV, and UDI. Interaction between age and gender was tested but was not found to be significant and was therefore excluded from the model (0.3 < p < 0.99). Given the absence of a normal distribution and homogeneity in our values, we decided to carry out non-parametric tests. The preconditions for these tests were met by our numeric dependent variables, our balanced samples and weakly correlated predictor variables. The p-value was then calculated by sampling, performing a Monte-Carlo test with 1,000 permutations for each model with the PermTest() of the R pgirmess package (Giraudoux et al., 2018). Tests were run three times to check the statistical stability. The significant factors in our models were then observed more closely using post-hoc pairwise comparison with the pairwisePermutationTest() function of the R rcompanion package (Mangiafico, 2019). Consequently, a Benjamini–Hochberg correction was applied. Finally, we observed the force and direction of the correlations between the significant numerical variables and the PCA dimensions.

All the statistical models were carried out using the R software, version 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2013) with α = 0.05.

Results

Out of a total n = 958 drawings analyzed, 33% showed environmental elements as well as the self-portrait. Amongst these, 54% contained natural elements (9.5% of which had been personified), 12% showed animals, 13.5% man-made objects, 10% supplementary human figures, 4% symbols, et 3% letters or numbers. On average, the children drew on 35 ± 20% of the total paper sheet. Human figures took up an average 16 ± 20% of the drawing space and 87% of these figures included facial elements, with an average of 2.9 ± 1.5 facial details included per drawing.

Five dimensions were retained from the 13 variables for our PCA, with eigenvalues of at least 1. These dimensions explained 63% of the total data variance (dimension 1 = 18.8%, dimension 2 = 15.4%, dimension 3 = 11.4%, dimension 4 = 9.2%, and dimension 5 = 8.1%). Each metric showed a higher loading (r > 0.5) in one dimension compared to the others, thus enabling us to classify them (see Table 1).

Dimensions 1, 2, 3, and 4 show significant variables to which they are positively correlated, while the fifth dimension is positively correlated with the head-to-body ratio but is negatively correlated with the number of man-made objects. We were therefore able to determine that the first PCA dimension represented the complexity of the drawing but also the meaning, the second was characterized by the inclusion of the living environment, the third corresponded to the space attributed to human representation, the fourth represented the characters grouping together the symbols, letters and numbers, and finally the fifth dimension showed the construction of identity, i.e., the importance that children attribute to what they are, built on their own experience and in relation with others, or at least the relative importance of the self and of objects. Of course, this interpretation of the dimensions are quite subjective and we come up to this later in the discussion.

Following the removal of the 35 missing value lines from our dataset, n = 923 drawings were considered in our analyses based on the use of 10 variables (i.e., the five PCA dimensions; the gender; the age; the CSC group; the IDV; and the UDI). Three prediction variables were found to significantly influence the values of dimension 1: Age, CSC group, and the UDI (gender p = 0.129; age p < 0.001; culture p < 0.001; IDV p = 0.885; and UDI p = 0.024). However, pairwise comparisons revealed that there was no significant difference between countries groups (p > 0.5). We also observed that the complexity of the drawings increased with the age of the children (t = 11.81; R2 = 0.130; r = 0.361) and to a lesser extent with the UDI (t = 2.278; R2 = 0.004; r = 0.067) (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Children’s drawing behavior for dimension 1 (i.e., complexity) according to age.

Two variables were significant in the second dimension (gender p = 0.053; age p < 0.001; culture p < 0.001; IDV p = 0.946; and UDI p = 0.887). These two variables are age and CSC group. Post-hoc comparisons revealed ten significant differences among pairwise comparisons between CSC groups (Table 2). More specifically, children from Central and South America showed more elements of the environment in their drawings than those from other cultures. Drawings by children from Western countries also included elements of their environment more frequently than those drawn by children from African countries and the Middle East. We also note that children living in Southern Africa tend to include fewer elements of the environment in their work than children from countries in North and West Africa, the Middle East and Asia (Figure 6A). The tendency to include elements of the environment decreases with the age of the children (t = −6.423; R2 = 0.041; r = 0.204).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Adjusted p of pairwise cultural similar countries (CSC) comparisons for dimensions 2–5 (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; and ***p < 0.001).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Comparison of children’s drawing behavior for (A) the dimension 2 (i.e., environment), (B) the dimension 3 (i.e., individual), (C) the dimension 4 (i.e., character) and (D) the dimension 5 (i.e., identity). The countries groups that have no letters in common differ significantly.

Three variables stand out significantly for the third dimension, namely age, CSC group, and IDV (gender p = 0.954; age p < 0.001; culture p < 0.001; IDV p < 0.001; and UDI p = 0.907). Post-hoc comparisons revealed seven significant differences between CSC groups (Table 2). As a result, we note that the space attributed to humans in children’s drawings was significantly larger in Asia than in African countries and the Middle East. In addition, children from Southern Africa used less space for human representations than children from Central and South America, North and West Africa or–above all–Western countries (Figure 6B). It was also evident that the space attributed to human representations tended to decrease with age (t = −3.40; R2 = 0.011; r = 0.106). Conversely, the amount of space increased with the IDV of the countries (t = 3.683; R2 = 0.013; r = 0.115).

In the fourth dimension, two variables were significant, namely CSC group and UDI (gender p = 0.084; age p = 0.415; culture p = 0.005; IDV p = 0.688; and UDI p = 0.042). Post-hoc comparisons revealed significant differences between three CSC groups (Table 2). Children from the Middle East included significantly more characters in their drawing (0.3 ± 1.2) than Southern African (−0.1 ± 0.2) and North and West African (0.1 ± 0.9) children did (Figure 6C). The inclusion of characters also increases with the UDI (t = 3.127; R2 = 0.009; r = 0.097).

Finally, the fifth dimension had three significant variables: CSC group, IDV, and UDI (gender p = 0.689; age p = 0.739; culture p < 0.001; IDV p = 0.008; and UDI p = 0.002). Post-hoc comparisons then revealed seven significant differences among CSC groups when compared two by two (Table 2). For example, Central and South American children drew human figures with larger heads in relation to their bodies and included fewer man-made objects in their drawings than children in all other cultures except those of Western countries. Children in Western societies drew proportionately larger heads and fewer man-made objects than Asian and Southern African children. This tendency was higher in children in African countries than those in Asian countries (Figure 6D). Our analyses also showed that the UDI (t = 5.464; R2 = 0.030; r = 0.097) and the IDV (t = 2.907; R2 = 0.008; r = 0.895) were positively correlated with identity construction.

Our results do not indicate any significant effect of child gender on the specific drawing variables studied.

Discussion

Our study sought to verify the existence of cultural differences in graphical representations of the self and the social and physical environments, drawn by children aged 2–15 years and originating from different countries. Using a Principal Component Analysis, self-portraits drawn by the children were evaluated in terms of their complexity and meaning, the frequency with which children included elements of their environment, the proportions used to draw human figures and the space attributed to it in terms of the self (i.e., individualism) or the presence of objects (i.e., materialism) and finally the presence or absence of characters (i.e., letters, numbers, and symbols). This is the first time that pictorial elements described in many previous studies were analyzed and combined in this way, revealing patterns and structures that could be universally studied in drawings. The cultural differences that we observed in these self-portraits confirmed that drawing is partly an acquired behavior that is widespread in humans (Kourkoulis, 2021). The physical and sociocultural environments of children appear to shape many aspects of the graphical representations they produce.

Firstly, there was no significant difference in drawing complexity between the different cultures; we could therefore note a worldwide characteristic for the development of human figure composition, with the presence of general models that evolve as the child grows older (Golomb, 1992). The observation of similar scribbles in all the CSC groups evaluated suggests that this expression is a universal step in the development of graphism (Baldy, 2009). Indeed, cognitive and motor capacities must be developed before children can create representations; graphic complexity including the meaning of the production is therefore positively correlated with the growth of the child (Toomela, 2002). However, the influence of culture on this complexity strengthens the hypothesis that the acquisition of graphic shapes and skills differs according to each child’s cultural environment. Contrary to certain studies, we have not noted an earlier development of complexity in drawings produced by Asian children who generally produce more detailed graphic compositions through the sustained learning of drawing behavior in their education systems and the common presence of manga drawings in their societies (La Voy et al., 2001). To remind, the Inequality-Adjusted Human Development Index (IHDI) we removed from analyses because of collinearity is linked with the CSC groups as with the urbanization level.

The urbanization level of children’s living environments also affects their graphical production in different ways. Our findings show that children from urbanized environments produced more complex drawings with more facial details (Gernhardt et al., 2015) that are even present in their very first representations of human figures (Rübeling et al., 2011). The influence of urbanization is explained by the child’s access to drawing materials and opportunities, in particular through the teaching of art at school, and the wide availability of graphical models that children can copy, particularly through the media. A child’s graphical capacity is therefore likely to depend on the stimulation provided by their environment, which defines their experience of the pictorial world (Picard and Zarhbouch, 2014). Our findings also suggest that children in urban societies include characters (symbols, numbers, and letters) more frequently in their drawing. As education level is linked to urbanization, our result may be due to the wider presence of educational structures in urbanized societies, thus providing a favorable environment for the learning of verbal language and writing that are then reflected in the child’s drawings. Linguistic differences could also explain the presence or absence of symbols or letters in drawings, because the complexity of certain native languages requires a faster evolution of the cognitive capacities that children also use to draw (Toku, 2001). Children who do not attend school and live in social environments with sparse sociocultural means have very limited graphical language and poor levels of inventiveness. Children who attend school and live in areas that are rich in sociocultural and artistic models develops a rich graphical language that facilitates creation. Finally, we observed that the level of urbanization also influences the construction of identity in children: In more urban societies, they are more likely to over-represent themselves in their drawings, with a larger head and the absence of man-made elements. Intercultural studies and the few available historical comparisons globally suggest that diverse cultural environments may provide different evolutive pathways leading to a wide range of drawing types (Baldy, 2009). Children from villages in developing countries are not “behind” Western children in terms of graphical development, but develop distinct graphical styles derived from different cultural patterns. They are on different developmental paths. As highlighted by Merleau-Ponty (2001), what we consider to be the “normal” endpoint of graphical development is actually one of many possible cultural achievements.

These parameters are also influenced by the degree of individualism inherent to each culture. Indeed, societies promoting individualism lead children from childhood toward independence and autonomy, both of which require the early development of confidence and self-esteem (Keller, 2007). This self-esteem is associated with a decrease in materialistic values in childhood (Chaplin and John, 2007), thus influencing graphical representations. Our results show that this individualism also influences the size and number of human representations in the drawings, thus confirming the conclusions of previous studies (Rübeling et al., 2011). The more individualistic a society is, the greater the child’s perception of his own value within his culture will be and the more he will maximize his existence as an individual, in particular by representing himself graphically as a large figure that is alone on the paper (La Voy et al., 2001). The space attributed to human representations appears to decrease as the children grow older. This change could be explained by the increased precision and dexterity of graphical representations as the individual gets older, thus making possible to successfully produce smaller representations (Cohn, 2012).

Unlike some authors (Burkitt et al., 2004), we observed that the tendency to include environmental elements in self-portraits decreased with age. We believe that children’s school and social experiences could gradually encourage the standardization of representations (Toku, 2001). As they grow older, children may therefore seek to shape their drawing to what their cultural model defines as a “good” representation, thus reducing the number of elements they include in their drawing (Rübeling et al., 2011). Gilles Porte’s instruction to “Draw yourself” could also encourage children to use less elements to draw themselves (Martinet et al., 2021): The more detailed the instructions are, the more specific the drawings will be (Smith, 1993). Older children may therefore focus more on the instruction due to the evolution of their attention capacity and ability to retain information (Sutton and Rose, 1998), thus leading them to include fewer environmental elements in their drawings (Toomela, 2002). American, European and Asian children seem to give more importance to depicting living elements of their environment in their pictures. This brings us back to the idea that it is more common to see the personification of environmental elements in some cultures than in others (Court, 1992) but, in contrast to other studies, Asian children do not seem to depict more contextual elements than their American and European counterparts in this study (Masuda et al., 2008). Contrary to our expectations, we did not see evidence of a greater consideration of family or Nature in any of the self-portraits produced by children in collectivist societies. As the education level is linked to the CSC groups, our result may be explained by factors such as the exclusion of nature in educational practices (Toku, 2001), the influence of the media presenting more details and backgrounds in some cultures (Kourkoulis, 2021) or the imitation of traditional aesthetic styles of representation of the environment (Masuda et al., 2008). Contrary to other studies (Iijima et al., 2001; Lange-Küttner, 2011; Lange-Küttner and Ebersbach, 2013; Bozzato and Longobardi, 2021; Bozzato et al., 2021), the gender of children does not appear to influence their drawings of self-portrait. However, it is difficult to demonstrate gender-related differences, and a more thorough analysis of further aspects and types of environmental details in drawings could provide us with a clearer picture.

The preliminary results of our study confirm that to understand drawing behavior, it is both possible and necessary to take its context of emergence into account in order to understand the various cultural influences that affect it. Also, we have shown that it is possible to observe an indication of a child’s sense of identity in their drawings through the importance they assign to representations of the elements within their environment. It is, however, important to note that without data specifying what the child intended to draw, our understanding of their drawing remains subjective. Although not completely reliable on its own, the child’s verbally expressed intention can be paralleled by what the observer perceives and allows the experimenter to get a more accurate picture of the child’s drawing (Martinet et al., 2021). Additionally, our choice to group the 35 countries together to observe the major cultural influences ruled out the possibility of considering the diversity of cultural practices and educational methods between countries of the same group and between regions of the same country, which could affect drawing behavior differently. It would therefore be interesting to make more specific comparisons. The dataset being limited for some age and countries categories, it should be also important to increase the number of drawings, per age, per country but also per cities vs. villages to increase the statistical power of analyses and so the rigor of our explanations. Continuing our research would allow us to discover the wealth of information contained in children’s works in greater depth. Our methodology could be applied to examine psychological and emotional traits measured through questionnaire (Kallitsoglou et al., 2021) or direct observation (Shiakou, 2012).

In his masterpiece The Little Prince, Antoine de Saint-Exupéry stated that adults never understand anything by themselves, and that children always have to explain to them. What would happen if adults took their turn and tried to understand the messages children convey, by pushing the limits of their own vision through the observation of the child’s culturally specific graphical productions? From this perspective, the analysis of drawings is essential and could allow the development of new communication strategies, particularly in our world of constantly evolving cultural diversity.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Strasbourg University Research Ethics Committee (Unistra/CER/2019-11). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

MP and CS contributed to conception and design of the study. SR and MP organized the database. SR and CS performed the statistical analysis. SR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors wrote sections of the manuscript and contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project has received financial support from the CNRS through the MITI Interdisciplinary Programs and an IDEX Exploratory Research Program from Strasbourg University.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Gilles Porte for giving us access to the children’s drawings. We also thank Noé Freyssinge and Joanna Munro for their precious help on drawings’ analysis and English language editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.940617/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.early-pictures.ch/porte1/archive/en/

References

Andersson, I., and Andersson, S. B. (2009). Aesthetic representations among Himba people in Namibia. Int. Art Early Child. Res. J. 1, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Baldy, R. (2005). Dessin et développement cognitif. Enfance 57, 34–44.

Google Scholar

Baldy, R. (2009). Dessine-moi un bonhomme. Universaux et variantes culturelles. Gradhiva Rev. Anthropol. Hist. Arts 9, 132–151. doi: 10.4000/gradhiva.1432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldy, R. (2011). Fais-moi un beau dessin. Regarder le dessin de l’enfant, comprendre son évolution. Paris.

Google Scholar

Bousquet, C. A. H., Petit, O., Arrivé, M., Robin, J.-P., and Sueur, C. (2015). Personality tests predict responses to a spatial-learning task in mallards. Anas platyrhynchos. Anim. Behav. 110, 145–154.

Google Scholar

Bozzato, P., and Longobardi, C. (2021). Cross-cultural evaluation of children’s drawings of gender role stereotypes in Italian and Cambodian students. J. Psychol. Educ. Res. 29, 97–115.

Google Scholar

Bozzato, P., Fabris, M. A., and Longobardi, C. (2021). Gender, stereotypes and grade level in the draw-a-scientist test in Italian schoolchildren. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 43, 2640–2662. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2021.1982062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brumm, A., Oktaviana, A. A., Burhan, B., Hakim, B., Lebe, R., Zhao, J., et al. (2021). Oldest cave art found in Sulawesi. Sci. Adv. 7:eabd4648. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abd4648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkitt, E., Barrett, M., and Davis, A. (2004). The effect of affective characterizations on the use of size and colour in drawings produced by children in the absence of a model. Educ. Psychol. 24, 315–343. doi: 10.1080/0144341042000211670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casti, A. B. (2016). Reaction to stimulus figures in chimpanzee drawings. Master Thesis. Ellensburg, WA: Central Washington University.

Google Scholar

Chaplin, L. N., and John, D. R. (2007). Growing up in a material world: Age differences in materialism in children and adolescents. J. Consum. Res. 34, 480–493. doi: 10.1086/518546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, N. (2012). Explaining ‘I can’t draw’: Parallels between the structure and development of language and drawing. Hum. Dev. 55, 167–192. doi: 10.1159/000341842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, N. (2014). Framing ‘I can’t draw’: The influence of cultural frames on the development of drawing. Cult. Psychol. 20, 102–117.

Google Scholar

Court, E. (1992). Researching social influences in the drawing of rural kenyan children. Harlow: Harlow Longman.

Google Scholar

Cox, M. (1992). Children’s drawings, 1st Edn. London: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Cox, M. V., Koyasu, M., Hiranuma, H., and Perara, J. (2001). Children’s human figure drawings in the UK and Japan: The effects of age, sex and culture. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 275–292.

Google Scholar

DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 66–70. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissanayake, E. (2001). Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.

Google Scholar

Fox, J., and Weisberg, S. (2019). An R companion to applied regression, 3 Edn. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Gernhardt, A., Keller, H., and Rübeling, H. (2016). Children’s family drawings as expressions of attachment representations across cultures: Possibilities and limitations. Child Dev. 87, 1069–1078. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gernhardt, A., Rübeling, H., and Keller, H. (2015). Cultural perspectives on childrens tadpole drawings: At the interface between representation and production. Front. Psychol. 6:812. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giraudoux, P., Antonietti, J.-P., Beale, C., Lancelot, R., Pleydell, D., and Treglia, M. (2018). pgirmess: Spatial analysis and data mining for field ecologists. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/pgirmess/index.html (accessed April 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

Golomb, C. (1992). The child’s creation of a pictorial world. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Goodnow, J. J., Miller, P. J., and Kessel, F. (1995). Cultural practices as contexts for development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. (2010). Hofstede cultural dimensions. Available online at: https://geert-hofstede.com (accessed May 6, 2020).

Google Scholar

Iijima, M., Arisaka, O., Minamoto, F., and Arai, Y. (2001). Sex differences in children’s free drawings: A study on girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm. Behav. 40, 99–104. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolley, R. P., Knox, E. L., and Foster, S. G. (2010). The relationship between children’s production and comprehension of realism in drawing. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 557–582.

Google Scholar

Kallitsoglou, A., Repana, V., and Shiakou, M. (2021). Children’s family drawings: Association with attachment representations in story stem narratives and social and emotional difficulties. Early Child Dev. Care 192, 1337–1348. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2021.1877284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Kellogg, R. (1970). Analyzing Children’s Art. Palo Alto. CA: Mayfield Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Kim, K. I., Park, H. J., and Suzuki, N. (1990). Reward allocations in the United States, Japan, and Korea: A comparison of individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Acad. Manag. J. 33, 188–198.

Google Scholar

Kourkoulis, L. (2021). The network of influences that shape the drawing and thinking of fifth grade children in three different cultures. Ph.D thesis. New York, NY: Columbia University.

Google Scholar

La Voy, S. K., Pedersen, W. C., Reitz, J. M., Brauch, A. A., Luxenberg, T. M., and Nofsinger, C. C. (2001). Children’s drawings: A cross-cultural analysis from Japan and the United States. Sch. Psychol. Int. 22, 53–63. doi: 10.1007/BF01542882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange-Küttner, C. (2008). Drawing and the Non-Verbal mind: A life-span perspective. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511489730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange-Küttner, C. (2011). Sex differences in visual realism in drawings of animate and inanimate objects. Percept. Mot. Skills 113, 439–453. doi: 10.2466/04.10.24.PMS.113.5.439-453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange-Küttner, C. (2014). Do drawing stages really exist? Children’s early mapping of perspective. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 8, 168–182. doi: 10.1037/a0036199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange-Küttner, C. (2020). ‘Drawing’ in the encyclopedia of child and adolescent development. Hoboken NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd, doi: 10.1002/9781119171492.wecad145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange-Küttner, C., and Ebersbach, M. (2013). Girls in detail, boys in shape: Gender differences when drawing cubes in depth. Br. J. Psychol. 104, 413–437. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lê, S., Josse, J., and Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: An R package for multivariate analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 25, 1–18.

Google Scholar

Lindström, L. (2000). The cultural context: Comparative studies of art education and children’s drawings. Stockholm: Stockholm University Press.

Google Scholar

Lowenfeld, V., and Brittain, W. L. (1987). Creative and mental growth, 8th Edn. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin. Paris: Alcan.

Google Scholar

Mangiafico, S. (2019). rcompanion: Functions to support extension education program evaluation. R package version, 2.

Google Scholar

Marcilhacy, C., and Demirdjian, F. B. (2011). Le Dessin et l’écriture dans l’acte clinique. Paris: Elsevier Masson.

Google Scholar

Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98:224.

Google Scholar

Martinet, L., Sueur, C., Hirata, S., Hosselet, J., Matsuzawa, T., and Pelé, M. (2021). New indices to characterize drawing behavior in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Sci. Rep. 11:3860. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83043-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., and Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Culture and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1260–1275.

Google Scholar

Matthews, J. (2003). Drawing and painting: Children and visual representation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Merleau-Ponty, M. (2001). Psychologie et pédagogie de l’enfant: Cours de sorbonne 1949 – 1952. Lagrasse: Verdier.

Google Scholar

Millar, S. (1975). Visual experience or translation rules? drawing the human figure by blind and sighted children. Perception 4, 363–371.

Google Scholar

Molinaro, R. K., Najjar, M. W. A., Hammad, A., Haddad, A., and Vazquez, E. (2020). Urban development index (UDI): A comparison between the city of Rio de Janeiro and four other global cities. Sustainability 12:823. doi: 10.3390/su12030823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, G., Kubacki, K., Hersh, M., and Gunn, R. O. D. (2007). Knowledge management in higher education: A comparison of individualistic and collectivist cultures. Eur. J. Educ. 42, 377–394.

Google Scholar

Nilsen, E. B., Pedersen, S., and Linnell, J. D. (2008). Can minimum convex polygon home ranges be used to draw biologically meaningful conclusions? Ecol. Res. 23, 635–639.

Google Scholar

Nomoto, T. (2007). Etude du développement cognitif du traitement des propriétés d’objet?: Apport de la Figure Complexe de Rey B?: Le développement du jeune enfant au travers de la Figure Complexe de Rey B. Doctoral thesis. Saint-Denis: Paris 8 University.

Google Scholar

Ouedraogo, W. D. (2015). Le dessin du bonhomme chez des enfants autistes?: Quatre études de cas à l’association burkinabè d’accompagnement psychologique et d’aide à l’enfance. ABAPE Report. Ouagadougou: Université Ouaga.

Google Scholar

Peck, A. (2006). Beginning GIMP: From novice to professional. Berkeley, CA: Apress.

Google Scholar

Pelé, M., Bellut, C., Debergue, E., Gauvin, C., Jeanneret, A., Leclere, T., et al. (2017). Cultural influence of social information use in pedestrian road-crossing behaviours. Open Sci. 4:160739. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelé, M., Thomas, G., Liénard, A., Eguchi, N., Shimada, M., and Sueur, C. (2021). I wanna draw like you: Inter- and intra-individual differences in orang-utan drawings. Animals 11:3202. doi: 10.3390/ani11113202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picard, D., and Zarhbouch, B. (2014). Le dessin comme langage graphique. Approch. Rev. Sci. Hum. 14, 28–40.

Google Scholar

Porte, G. (2012). Digital edition of the picture archive of Gilles Porte. Available online at: https://www.early-pictures.ch/porte1/en/ (accessed March 5, 2020).

Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Core Team.

Google Scholar

Royer, J. (2005). Que nous disent les dessins d’enfants?. Marseille: Hommes et perspectives.

Google Scholar

Rübeling, H., Keller, H., Yovsi, R. D., Lenk, M., Schwarzer, S., and Kühne, N. (2011). Children’s drawings of the self as an expression of cultural conceptions of the self. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 406–424. doi: 10.1177/0022022110363475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiakou, M. (2012). Representations of attachment patterns in the family drawings of maltreated and non-maltreated children. Child Abuse Rev. 21, 203–218. doi: 10.1002/car.1184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. M. (1993). Young children’s depiction of contrast in human figure drawing: Standing and walking. Educ. Psychol. 13, 107–118. doi: 10.1080/0144341930130202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sueur, C., Martinet, L., Beltzung, B., and Pelé, M. (2021). Making drawings speak through mathematical metrics. Available online at: http://arxiv.org/abs/2109.02276 (accessed November 9, 2021).

Google Scholar

Sutton, P. J., and Rose, D. H. (1998). The role of strategic visual attention in children’s drawing development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 68, 87–107. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toku, M. (2001). Cross-cultural analysis of artistic development: Drawing by Japanese and U.S. children. Vis. Arts Res. 27, 46–59.

Google Scholar

Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: A study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 234–247. doi: 10.1080/01650250143000021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viblanc, V. A., Pasquaretta, C., Sueur, C., Boonstra, R., and Dobson, F. S. (2016). Aggression in Columbian ground squirrels: Relationships with age, kinship, energy allocation, and fitness. Behav. Ecol. 27, 1716–1725. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallon, P., Cambier, A., and Engelhart, D. (1998). Le dessin de l’enfant, 2nd Edn. Paris: Presses Univ. de France.

Google Scholar

Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children’s drawings. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Google Scholar

Wolf, M., and Weissing, F. J. (2012). Animal personalities: Consequences for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 452–461. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: representation, self-portrait, cross-cultural study, drawings, child development

Citation: Restoy S, Martinet L, Sueur C and Pelé M (2022) Draw yourself: How culture influences drawings by children between the ages of two and fifteen. Front. Psychol. 13:940617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.940617

Received: 10 May 2022; Accepted: 24 October 2022;
Published: 08 November 2022.

Edited by:

Monica Shiakou, European University Cyprus, Cyprus

Reviewed by:

Olesya Blazhenkova, Sabancı University, Turkey
Evelin Ingrid Witruk, Leipzig University, Germany
Christina J. Roussi, University of Thessaly, Greece

Copyright © 2022 Restoy, Martinet, Sueur and Pelé. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marie Pelé, marie.pele@univ-catholille.fr

Present address: Marie Pelé, Lille Catholic University, Lille, France

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.