Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 12 September 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Children's Drawings: Evidence-based Research and Practice View all 18 articles

Family drawing for assessing attachment in children: Weaknesses and strengths

  • University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy

Introduction

The drawing represents a projective technique widely used by clinical and developmental psychologists to access a child’s inner world (Falk, 1981; Pianta and Longmaid, 2010; Procaccia et al., 2014). Particularly, the Family Drawing (FD) developed by the psychoanalyst Corman (1967) is one of the most used tests to explore perceptions of relationships in children aged 5−⁣−15 years and, thanks to Kaplan and Main (1986) and Fury (1996) contributions, it can also assess children’s attachment representations.

The Family Drawing (FD) with an attachment-based coding system includes three levels: a checklist of individual markers (Kaplan and Main, 1986), which comprises 24 features of drawing whose presence or absence is assessed; four global attachment classifications, i.e., ABCD classifications, namely, Secure (B), Avoidant (A), Ambivalent (C), and Disorganized (D), assigned based on a global evaluation of marker combinations (Kaplan and Main, 1986); eight global rating scales scored from 1 (absent) to 7 (very high) points, added by Fury (1996), who also redesigned criteria of assignment of ABCD categories considering both markers and scale scores. Table 1 details the characteristics of these attachment-based coding systems.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Three levels of attachment-based coding system on Family Drawing.

The administration of the FD follows the procedure described by Kaplan and Main (1986) and Fury et al. (1997). It requires an 8.5 × 11 cm white paper placed horizontally in front of the child and a set of colored markers. Different from other methods, such as the gold-standard Strange Situation Procedure (SSP; Ainsworth et al., 1978; van Ijzendoorn et al., 1992), the child is not subjected to stressful situations or stimuli but is asked to draw a picture of his/her family. As a projective open-ended technique, no other information is given. When the drawing has been completed, the child is asked to identify all the people in the drawing and explain their relationship with the child. The assessment is made according to some indicators that suggest certain patterns of attachment (e.g., lack of color or distance between family members suggests an avoidant attachment, and unusually small figures or exaggeration of body parts suggests an ambivalent attachment, as detailed in Table 1).

Because drawing is non-verbal communication, FD is thought to be useful when it is not possible or unreliable to rely on verbal communication. For example, with internationally low-language proficiency adopted children (Pace et al., 2015), children with selective mutism, or other clinical conditions that involve language difficulty (e.g., too fast and confused speech in ADHD, Clarke et al., 2002), as well as when the child may be frightened, reluctant or not used to communicate about family relationships, as in the case of abused children, children exposed to family problems (Leon and Rudy, 2005), or children of depressed mothers (Fihrer and McMahon, 2009).

As shown by a recent systematic review and meta-analysis on this topic (Pace et al., 2021), the FD has been increasingly used to assess attachment in the last decade, either with community, clinical, or at-risk children from 5 to 13 years old. Most of the studies employed a double coding system, i.e., both the Main and Kaplan’s ABCD classifications, and Fury’s scales (detailed in Table 1), with the former being suggested as maybe less accurate than the latter, especially in clinical and at-risk samples. Other findings also suggested a culture-fair potentiality of FD in non-Western collectivistic cultures, because of the possibility offered by this method to assess attachment representations toward multiple attachment figures in the drawing, overcoming the exclusive focus on mother and father typical of Western cultures.

In sum, the scarce studies and review findings opened questions concerning FD psychometric properties and culture-fair potential in non-Western collectivistic cultures, which still need more investigation.

Sub-section 1: How are the psychometric properties of the Family Drawing attachment-based coding systems?

Focusing on the psychometric properties of the FD with an attachment-based coding system, studies provided values of inter-rater reliability being from acceptable to good for ABCD classifications, i.e., Cohen’s k between 0.64 and 0.80 (Madigan et al., 2003; Pianta and Longmaid, 2010; Behrens and Kaplan, 2011), and good to optimal for Fury’s global scales, i.e., Cohen’s k between 0.75 and 1.00 (Fury et al., 1997; Madigan et al., 2003; Pianta and Longmaid, 2010), and Pearson’s r from 0.54 to 0.95 (Fury et al., 1997; Madigan et al., 2003). These results suggest if independent and blinded evaluators of the FD employ the same parameters, they usually assign the same classification or score, which indicates that the coding guidelines of both ABCD and Fury scale systems are clear and well-explained, understandable by different possible raters.

Regarding FD discriminant validity, some studies found relations between children’s IQ and both ABCD categories (Pianta and Longmaid, 2010) and Fury scales scores in community and at-risk children (Dallaire et al., 2012). However, other studies did not find any relationship between FD and IQ or children’s fine motor skills (Fury, 1996; Madigan et al., 2003; Leon et al., 2007; Pace et al., 2015). Surprisingly, no studies explored the discriminant validity of the child’s ability to draw.

Concerning the concurrent validity, studies in different populations of children (i.e., community, clinical, adopted, etc.) showed the attachment-based FD results partially converged with those of the gold-standard SSP and the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task (MCAST; Goldwyn et al., 2000), a completion task used to assess attachment representations in 4–8 years old children and rated both through four ABCD classifications and 1-to 9-point scales (Jin et al., 2018; Pace et al., 2020; Kallitsoglou et al., 2021).

Particularly, the results of the meta-analyses with the SSP (van Ijzendoorn et al., 1992) and with FD (Pace et al., 2021) converge toward the higher prevalence of secure classifications over the insecure ones in community children and security scores as the highest in Fury’s scales. Moreover, with both instruments, the community children showed higher security than clinical and at-risk ones. However, the meta-analytic rate of C categories with the Family Drawing ABCD system was markedly higher than the rate in the meta-analysis of SSP, so the authors have suggested a possible overestimation of the C pattern employing the Kaplan and Main (1986) system on FD which should be further investigated. The authors have also observed higher convergence of results between SSP and Fury scales than with the ABCD system (Pace et al., 2021).

Few studies explored the convergence of FD results with those of the MCAST (Goldwyn et al., 2000), reporting contrasting results across samples, which suggest further investigation. Specifically, Jin et al. (2018) suggested convergence in both classifications and scales of community and (especially) clinical children. Pace et al. (2020) reported convergence of more scales in communities than in at-risk children, and Kallitsoglou et al. (2021) suggested no convergence of scales among the communities. Overall, these results are too heterogeneous to assume that FD can be as trustable as other more validated attachment measures in assessing attachment, and further studies are needed.

Regarding clinical validity, several studies showed that FD attachment-based coding systems can discriminate against higher attachment insecurity in clinical and at-risk children (i.e., ADHD, adopted, abused, etc.) using Fury’s global scales (Clarke et al., 2002; Dallaire et al., 2012; Pace et al., 2015; Howard et al., 2017; Jin et al., 2018). If future psychometric studies will prove the reliability of the results obtained with the FD, these findings suggest practitioners employ the FD as a simple, economic, and useful method to assess attachment in vulnerable groups.

Last but not least, Pace et al. (2021) rated the quality of studies included in the systematic review, revealing fair to moderate quality for most of them, which mostly did not check the influence of demographics on results which should be further investigated.

Sub-section 2: Can the Family Drawing be considered a culture-fair method to assess attachment in children around the world?

As detailed in the review by Pace et al. (2021), attachment of pre- and school-aged children has been mainly evaluated through observational procedures, e.g., the SSP, or narrative completion tasks, e.g., the MCAST. Both of them have limitations at this age: the former because it is mostly based on behaviors that children show with their parents up to 2 years of age and the latter because results can be influenced by the child’s verbal abilities or cultural stereotypes transmitted through language (Burgers and Beukeboom, 2020).

Drawing instead has been reported as a culture-fair option to assess psychological abilities, e.g., (Weiss, 1980), and the cited meta-analysis seems to recognize this potential also in FD, as cultural differences did not completely overlap with those hypothesized based on general literature (Mesman et al., 2016). Specifically, the distribution of any ABCD category did not significantly differ between community children from Western (i.e., Canada, United States, Italy, and Greece) or non-Western (i.e., Israel, Japan, Korea, and Cameroon) cultural backgrounds. However, differences emerged employing Fury’s scales, with Western community children scoring higher than non-Western ones on all scales related to insecure patterns, revealing a counterintuitive trend. A limitation of this investigation is that cultural differences in clinical and at-risk samples have been not explored despite potentially informative, e.g., by exploring the differences between community and at-risk internationally adopted children who have different cultural backgrounds.

Discussion

This opinion deepened two open questions raised by current literature on Family Drawing with attachment-based coding systems leading to ABCD classifications (Kaplan and Main, 1986) or scales (Fury, 1996).

The first question was about psychometric properties. Current findings highlight the main strength of FD in the inter-rater reliability, almost always reported and with good values for both systems across different samples. Instead, the scarcity of studies suggests a great need for research on discriminant validity. Particularly, it appears important to investigate if the results obtained with the FD are independent of the child’s ability to draw, enlarging the potential number of children assessable. If the investigation is routed concerning IQ, on which anyway more studies are needed, there is still a lot to do concerning the ability to draw. Few efforts focused on fine motor skills as a measurable parameter of the ability to draw, to understand if the FD runs the risk to classify as secure children more able to drawn, and less able children are more likely to be classified as insecure. However, a child’s drawing abilities depend on different skills besides fine motor ability, the topic which is still uninvestigated, e.g., visuospatial skills (Toomela, 2002). One option can be to use a tool for evaluating drawing abilities (e.g., Clark, 1995), including an evaluation of the same child’s abilities in drawing different contents, such as the family and another not-attachment-related topic, to also check if the content of the FD may elicit emotional arousal impacting visual-sensory skills (Costanzi et al., 2019).

Besides, drawing abilities varied according to gender and age (Wright and Black, 2013). Their influence should be further investigated, especially given that scarcity of available data hindered a meta-analytic investigation of their role in a study by Pace et al. (2021). This would help to understand whether gender differences suggested by some findings reflect those recognized in the wider literature on attachment, or whether the drawing ability is influential. Concerning age, existing studies included children in large age ranges, while more research on clustering age groups would help to define the optimal age range where the FD led to more reliable and accurate results.

Concerning concurrent validity, contrasting findings, and limitations of the few existing studies seem to suggest the informative utility to design mixed-method studies employing the SSP and/or the MCAST with the FD, analyzed with both coding systems and possibly including either community, clinical, or at-risk samples. In this regard, authors of studies employing a double method are also encouraged to publish data on convergent validity, e.g., SSP and FD (Fihrer and McMahon, 2009).

The second research question inquired about the culture-fair potential of the FD. On the one side, results with both scoring systems support the universality of pattern B, as expressed without marked differences between Western and non-Western children (Mesman et al., 2016). On the other side, unexpected results raised doubts about the universality of indicators of insecure patterns, particularly the insecure-preoccupied pattern. These indicators of insecurity are based on a typically Western conception of parent–child dyads, and they could not capture the contribution of multiple sources of attachment security typical of some non-Western cultures where multiple adults contribute to raising children (e.g., African countries like Cameroon; Eloundou-Enyegue and Shapiro, 2004; Amos, 2013). In this case, the FD has the potential to leave the child free to draw all the significant figures he/she considers part of his/her family. However, it poses the problem of how to compare the results with those obtained with other measures based on the dyad, e.g., SSP and MCAST.

Beyond inter-country differences, the FD would help in those situations where the reliance on a child’s verbal abilities is limited, e.g., inter-country adopted or migrant or asylum seeker or refugee children, or clinical ones, e.g., children with selective mutism or social anxiety.

However, all these enthusiastic purposes urge to be substantiated by future investigations providing empirical support or disconfirming the FD culture-fair potential, for instance, through inter-country investigations or with mixed-method studies designed as proposed above, specifically selecting the previously mentioned subgroups of children as at-risk and clinical participants.

In conclusion, the convoluted and heterogeneous state-of-art of research on FD with attachment-related systems is probably due to a lack of continuous development and control of coding systems starting from the same developers, which led to multiple adaptations of the coding system (e.g., Crittenden’s model in Carr-Hopkins et al., 2017) and fragmented contributions affecting the recognition of FD potential. Hopefully, this Opinion Article provides a valuable resource and an important starting point to guide future lines of research to advance the knowledge on this topic.

Author contributions

CP conceptualized, structured the opinion, revised, and rewrote some parts. SM and FV wrote the first draft of the manuscript and made all further revisions until the final version. FV had performed Table 1 based on CP’s indications. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Amos, P. M. (2013). “Parenting and culture–evidence from some African communities,” in Parenting in South American and African contexts, ed. M. L. Seidl-de-Moura (London: IntechOpen). doi: 10.5772/56967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, K. Y., and Kaplan, N. (2011). Japanese children’s family drawings and their link to attachment. Attach. Hum. Dev. 13:437450. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2011.602252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgers, C., and Beukeboom, C. J. (2020). How language contributes to stereotype formation: Combined effects of label types and negation use in behavior descriptions. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 39, 438–456. doi: 10.1177/0261927X20933320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr-Hopkins, R., De Burca, C., and Aldridge, F. A. (2017). Assessing attachment in school-aged children: Do the school-age assessment of attachment and family drawings work together as complementary tools? Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 23, 402–420. doi: 10.1177/1359104517714589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, G. A. (1995). CDAT clarks drawing abilities test. Bloomington, IND: Arts Publishing C., Inc.

Google Scholar

Clarke, L., Ungerer, J., Chahoud, K., Johnson, S., and Stiefel, I. (2002). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is associated with attachment insecurity. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 7, 179–198. doi: 10.1177/1359104502007002006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corman, L. (1967). Le test du dessin de famille dans la pratique medico-pedagogique. Paris: P.U.F.

Google Scholar

Costanzi, M., Cianfanelli, B., Saraulli, D., Lasaponara, S., Doricchi, F., Cestari, V., et al. (2019). The effect of emotional valence and arousl on visuo-spatial working memory: Incidental emotional learning and memory for object-location. Front. Psychol. 10:2587. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallaire, D. H., Ciccone, A., and Wilson, L. C. (2012). The family drawings of at-risk children: Concurrent relation with contact with incarcerated parents, caregiver behavior, and stress. Attach. Hum. Dev. 14, 161–183. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.661232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eloundou-Enyegue, P. M., and Shapiro, D. (2004). Buffering inequalities: The safety net of extended families in Cameroon. Cornell food and nutrition policy program working paper. Available online at: http://www.saga.cornell.edu/images/wp177.pdf

Google Scholar

Falk, J. D. (1981). Understanding children’s art: An analysis of the literature. J. Pers. Assess. 45, 465–472. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4505_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fihrer, I., and McMahon, C. (2009). Maternal state of mind regarding attachment, maternal depression and children’s family drawings in the early school years. Attach. Hum. Dev. 11, 537–556. doi: 10.1080/14616730903282498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fury, G. (1996). The relation between infant attachment history and representations of relationships in school-aged family drawings. Ph.D. thesis. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.

Google Scholar

Fury, G., Carlson, E. A., and Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Children’s representations of attachment relations in family drawings. Child Dev. 68, 1154–1164. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01991.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldwyn, R., Stanley, C., Smith, V., and Green, J. (2000). The Manchester child attachment story task: Relationship with parental AAI, SAT and child behaviour. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2, 71–84. doi: 10.1080/146167300361327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, A. R. H., Razuri, E. B., Call, C. D., DeLuna, J. H., Purvis, K. B., and Cross, D. R. (2017). Family drawings as attachment representations in a sample of post-institutionalized adopted children. Arts Psychother. 52, 63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2016.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, M. K., Chung, U., and Hazen, N. (2018). Attachment representations of school-aged Korean children: Comparing family drawing and narrative assessments in a clinical and a community sample. Attach. Hum. Dev. 20, 43–61. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1371781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kallitsoglou, A., Repana, V., and Shiakou, M. (2021). Children’s family drawings: Association with attachment representations in story stem narratives and social and emotional difficulties. Early Child Dev. Care. 192, 1337–1348. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2021.1877284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1986). Instruction for the classification of children’s family drawing in term of representation of attachment. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Google Scholar

Leon, K., and Rudy, D. (2005). Family processes and children’s representations of parentification. J. Emot. Abuse 5, 111–142. doi: 10.1300/J135v05n02_06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leon, K., Wallace, T., and Rudy, D. (2007). Representations of parent-child alliances in children’s family drawings. Soc. Dev. 16, 440–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00392.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madigan, S., Ladd, M., and Goldberg, S. (2003). A picture is worth a thousand words: Children’s representations of family as indicators of early attachment. Attach. Hum. Dev. 5, 19–37. doi: 10.1080/1461673031000078652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Sagi-Schwartz, A. (2016). “Cross-cultural patterns of attachment,” in Handbook of attachment: Theory research, and clinical applications, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: Guildford press), 852–877.

Google Scholar

Pace, C. S., Guerriero, V., and Zavattini, G. C. (2020). Children’s attachment representations: A pilot study comparing family drawing with narrative and behavioral assessments in adopted and community children. Arts Psychother. 67:101612. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2019.101612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pace, C. S., Muzi, S., Madera, F., Sansò, A., and Zavattini, G. C. (2021). Can the family drawing be a useful tool for assessing attachment representations in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Attach. Hum. Dev. 24, 477–502. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2021.1991664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pace, C. S., Zavattini, G. C., and Tambelli, R. (2015). Does family drawing assess attachment representations of late-adopted children? A preliminary report. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 20, 26–33. doi: 10.1111/camh.12042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pianta, R., and Longmaid, K. (2010). Attachment-based classifications of children’s family drawings: Psychometric properties and relations with children’s adjustment in kindergarten. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 28, 244–255. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2802_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Procaccia, R., Veronese, G., and Castiglioni, M. (2014). The impact of attachment style on the family drawings of school-aged children. Open Psychol. J. 7, 9–17. doi: 10.2174/1874350101407010009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: A study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 234–247. doi: 10.1080/01650250143000021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Golberg, S., Kroonenberg, P. M., and Frenkel, O. J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and. Child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Dev. 63, 840–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01665.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S. C. (1980). Culture fair intelligence test and draw-a-person scores from a rural Peruvian sample. J Soc. Psychol. 111, 147–148. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1980.9924286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, L., and Black, F. (2013). Monochrome males and colorful females: Do gender and age influence the color and content of drawings? SAGE Open. 3, 1–9. doi: 10.1177/2158244013509254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: family drawing, attachment representations, children, ABCD system, Fury’s global rating scales, psychometric properties, culture-fair method

Citation: Pace CS, Muzi S and Vizzino F (2022) Family drawing for assessing attachment in children: Weaknesses and strengths. Front. Psychol. 13:980129. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.980129

Received: 28 June 2022; Accepted: 12 August 2022;
Published: 12 September 2022.

Edited by:

Claudio Longobardi, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Nicola Carone, University of Pavia, Italy
Rekha Negi, University of Delhi, India

Copyright © 2022 Pace, Muzi and Vizzino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cecilia Serena Pace, cecilia.pace@unige.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.