Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 September 2023
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

Acoustic perception and emotion evocation by rock art soundscapes of Altai (Russia)

Samantha Lpez-Mochales,Samantha López-Mochales1,2Raquel Aparicio-Terrs,Raquel Aparicio-Terrés1,2Margarita Díaz-Andreu,,Margarita Díaz-Andreu3,4,5Carles Escera,,,
Carles Escera1,2,4,6*
  • 1Brainlab – Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Departament d’Història i Arqueologia, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats (ICREA), Barcelona, Spain
  • 5Institut d’Arqueologia de la Universitat de Barcelona (IUAB), Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Barcelona, Spain
  • 6Institut de Recerca Sant Joan de Déu (IRSJD), Esplugues de Llobregat, Spain

The major goal of psychoarchaeoacoustics is to understand the psychology behind motivations and emotions of past communities when selecting certain acoustic environments to set activities involving the production of paintings and carvings. Within this framework, the present study seeks to explore whether a group of archaeological rock art sites in Altai (Siberia, Russia) are distinguished by particular acoustic imprints that elicit distinct reactions on listeners, in perceptual and emotional terms. Sixty participants were presented with a series of natural sounds convolved with six impulse responses from Altai, three of them recorded in locations in front of rock art panels and three of them in front of similar locations but without any trace of rock art. Participants were interrogated about their subjective perception of the sounds presented, using 10 psychoacoustic and emotional scales. The mixed ANOVA analyses carried out revealed that feelings of “presence,” “closeness,” and “tension” evoked by all sounds were significantly influenced by the location. These effects were attributed to the differences in reverberation between the locations with and without rock art. Despite results are not consistent across all the studied rock art sites, and acknowledging the presence of several limitations, this study highlights the significance of its methodology. It stresses the crucial aspect of incorporating the limitations encountered in shaping future research endeavors.

1. Introduction

An emerging avenue of inquiry posits that investigating sound in the past from different perspectives –acoustics, psychoacoustics, ethnomusicology, etc.– may provide crucial information to better understand past societies (Scarre and Lawson, 2006). In particular, archaeology has given rise to a sub-field of research, archaeoacoustics, a novel perspective interested in sound in the past. Some researchers working in archaeoacoustics are focusing their attention on the acoustics of rock art sites in order to answer questions such as how and why past communities chose certain acoustic environments to decorate and most likely to set social and ritual activities related to the production of paintings and carvings in them (Díaz-Andreu and Mattioli, 2015). Sound affects the way people engage with, transform and create environments or places (Blake and Cross, 2015). Environmental acoustics can affect listeners’ emotional reactions and sound perception (Västfjäll, 2012) and, although sound is an ephemeral event, acoustic environments persevere over time (Rainio et al., 2018). Acoustic and perceptual sciences can be applied together to better understand the sensory potential of a soundscape, by reproducing the conditions of the auditory experience endured by ancient communities through experimentation on modern-day participants (Kolar, 2013). When these approaches combine with the experimental methods of psychology, arguably they give rise to an emerging discipline, that of psychoarchaeoacoustics (Valenzuela et al., 2020).

By recording the so-called impulse response or IR, the acoustic signature of a space of archaeological interest can be recreated (Alvarez-Morales et al., 2020; Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a,b; Alvarez-Morales et al., 2023; Santos da Rosa et al., 2023). The IR method measures sound propagation between an emission point and a receiver device (Farina, 2007), and allows the recreation of an acoustic space in laboratory conditions through the process of auralization (Vorländer, 2008). Auralization consists in convolving a sound with the IR of interest and presenting listeners with the resulting sound, in order to immerse them in the recorded sonic space (Farina and Ayalon, 2003). Hence, the listeners’ individual reactions, subjective interpretations and affective responses to the sound properties of spaces can be measured (Västfjäll et al., 2002; Pätynen and Lokki, 2016) to ultimately extract conclusions about the election process of the most suitable places for the production of rock art, as in the present study. Hence, the aim of the present research is to investigate from a psychoarchaeoacoustical point of view (Valenzuela et al., 2020), the acoustic environment of a particular rock art area in the Republic of Altai (Russia).

There is a tight relationship between rock art, sound and the landscape with links to the beliefs of the people who produced and experienced the art. These connections have been seen in many cultures around the globe (Díaz-Andreu and Mattioli, 2017; Díaz-Andreu et al., 2021). In Altai, soundscapes and acoustic phenomena had a significant cultural role, at least in historical times, and presumably in prehistoric times. The ethnographic accounts of Altai describe how natural and supernatural entities are related through sound in an ordinary basis, which shapes music esthetics: songs and singing styles imitate natural sounds and, at the same time, nature interacts and imitates human sound with phenomena such as echoes (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a,b). Although no explicit mention of rock art is made in the ethnographic sources, in some areas of Altai it is still considered wrong to modify the environments where carved stones are found. This supports the idea that the marking of the stones was part of the described spiritual and ritualistic imaginary, involving sound production and interaction with the acoustic environment (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a,b). The rock art of this area is characterized by carvings made in a broad period ranging from, probably, the Neolithic to the Turkic era and even later. The major period of production was from 3,000 BCE to ca 500 CE. All the sites mentioned in this article had rock art motifs from all the different periods, indicating that they were repeatedly visited by many generations since prehistory (Miklashevich and Bove, 2010; Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a,b). This is the case of Kalbak-Tash I, Kalbak-Tash II, Grand Yaloman and Torgun I-5, The only exception is the panel of Urkosh IVb with only a few carvings of undiagnostic chronology. Still, it is important to note that it is found in a more extensive are with many other rock art panels distinguished, as the previous sites, by having a large quantity of motifs made during a long period of time.

As discussed, some acoustic phenomena are linked to the consideration of a place as a specific location in which social and ritual activities may have taken place (Díaz-Andreu and Mattioli, 2017; Díaz-Andreu et al., 2021). They were not dwelling places, but locales where people undertook activities that had a special sacred or social character and marked the space with paintings or carvings. In our case study, excavations have shown that at the time the rock art was produced there were episodic periods in which fires were lit, pots were used, and sacrifices were performed close to the decorated panels (Surazakov, 1996; Borodovskiĭ et al., 2016). In the present study, the point of departure is the proposal that by exposing listeners to natural sounds in presence of the acoustics from the rock art sites, and studying its effects –in terms of auditory subjective experience and emotion-, we might better understand the auditory experience lived by the communities who took part on the production of rock art and engaged with it later on. The objective of the present study was hence to observe which specific psychoacoustic and emotional dimensions were experienced differently in the presence of the soundscape of a spot close to a rock art panel, compared to a spot located away from it.

For our experiment, we selected a set of 17 natural sounds present in the natural landscapes of Siberia (including sounds from autochthonous animals and weather phenomena). We decided to use natural sounds to avoid cultural biases that could be caused by the use of music (Zentner et al., 2008; Hagman, 2010; Carew and Ramaswami, 2020). In particular, the music from the region of Altai could be esthetically unpleasant for modern-day listeners not familiar with the cultural tradition in Altai and neighboring areas. Sounds were convolved with six different IRs from Altai, three of them recorded in front of wall marked with carvings on the rocks, and three from wall of similar physical characteristics but not marked with rock art carvings. Experiment participants were presented with these sounds and were interrogated about their feelings and perception using 10 psychoacoustical and emotional descriptors. The sounds were presented in a spatial audio format –3rd order Ambisonics– to recreate the acoustic environments more accurately and to enhance stronger emotional reactions (Västfjäll, 2003).

Not only sounds (Bradley and Lang, 2000) play a role in emotion evocation, but also other-than-content features of sound –for example, physical attributes of sound propagation in space (Västfjäll, 2003). Therefore, in our experiment we expected to observe significant variations in all the psychoacoustical and emotional dimensions studied when our participants listened to the sounds convolved with the different IRs. Moreover, we expected to find disparities in these dimensions when listening to the sounds convolved with the IRs recorded in front of the rock art panels (from now on, art+) compared to the ones recorded away from the rock art panels (from now on, art−). Finding contrasting perceptions between the art+ and art− sounds, and commonalities within each group, would be a first step toward considering that acoustics played some role in the selection of the most suitable places for rock art production in the study area in Altai and its related activities. Furthermore, it would reveal which dimensions of perception and emotion we should keep exploring to better understand the experience endured by individuals who took part on the production of rock art carvings.

Considering the numerous limitations inherent in the present study, it becomes imperative to acknowledge and address these constraints from the outset. Firstly, the IRs used were recorded long after rock art creation, potentially impacting landscape accuracy. Geomorphological and climatic changes might have occurred in the course of hundreds of years, as well as changes in the vegetation of the area, influencing sound absorption and, consequently, acoustical properties. Secondly, the accuracy of soundscape reconstructions is uncertain due to unavailable anechoic or quasi-anechoic recordings from natural sources for convolutions. Lastly, the empirical approach to study emotions in ancient societies raises concerns about cultural differences. Despite it presents limitations, and its interpretations should be cautious, this study provides valuable insights into acoustic considerations in the process of rock art production by past societies. However, interpretations should be cautious, recognizing the constraints of the present perspective.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Sixty healthy human volunteers −44 women and 16 men of ages between 18 and 31 years old– participated on this experiment. The recruitment was carried out via students and research staff mail lists, blogs and social media. The exclusion criteria for participants included hearing impairments, psychiatric or neurological illnesses, ages below 18 or above 35 years and consumption of drugs or pharmaceuticals acting on the central nervous system. The study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Barcelona. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants gave written informed consent in compliance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

2.2. Stimuli

2.2.1. Sounds

The stimulus set was composed of 17 natural sounds of 20 s of duration, including animals (a bear, a crane, a plover, a cricket, an eagle, an ibex, a leopard, a lynx, a sheep and a pack of gray wolves) and other natural phenomena (a bonfire, the rain, a river, a snowstorm, a thunderstorm, a waterfall and the wind). The sounds were extracted from several sources; details are provided in Supplementary Table S1.

Five additional sounds (a dunnock, a baboon, a river, an impala and a forest atmosphere) were used in the first five trials of each block, but the data obtained from these trials were not included in the analysis. These five trials were used to allow participants to get immersed in the two different acoustic conditions (art+ and art−), and to ensure that the task to be carried out was clearly understood (Bradley et al., 1999; Witew et al., 2005; Vigeant and Celmer, 2010; Marquis-Favre et al., 2019). These additional sounds were extracted from the BBC sound effects repository; details are provided in Supplementary Table S2. The amplitude of sounds was normalized using Audacity1 version 2.3.2.

2.2.2. Impulse responses

Each sound of the stimulus set was convolved with six different IRs recorded in different natural landscapes across the Republic of Altai. All the IRs were recorded and processed using the MIMO IR technique (Farina and Chiesi, 2016; Farina, 2020). The position of the source and the microphone with respect to the rock faces was constant; the microphone was placed in front of the rock, at least 1 m away from it, and the loudspeaker was placed 10–12 m away from the microphone, aligned with it, and perpendicular to the direction of the panel (Figure 1). Three of the IRs were recorded in front of panels with rock art motifs (art+), and the other three were recorded in front of rock panels without any presence of rock art, at least 30 meters away from any wall with carvings on it (art−). The objective of this methodological approach was to isolate the effects of the critical variable presence of rock art, which would allow attribution of the eventual effects to the presence of the carvings. The six resulting versions of the stimulus set were grouped in three bundles (one, two and three), each including one version convolved with an IR from an art+ recording position -in front of a rock art panel- and an art− recording position -convolved with an IR from a recording position in front of a rock face away from the rock art panels.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schema of the recording position of the Ambisonics microphone (A) and the loudspeaker (B) with respect to the rock faces (C).

The first bundle included a version of the stimulus set (art+ 1) convolved with an IR recorded in front of one of the rock art panels at Kalbak-Tash I (Figure 2, E 86° 49′ 10″, N 50° 24′ 05″), in the area of the Lower Chuya river. Kalbak-Tash I is a rocky flattened hill, located in the lower part of the Chuya River valley, close to the confluence with the Katun river (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a). The corresponding art− version of the stimulus set (art− 1) was convolved with an IR recorded in the area of Urkosh (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a, 2022b) called “rock face 2,” in the coordinates E 086° 33′ 36″, N 50° 32′ 50″ and not that far from the rock art site of Grand Yaloman (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a), situated in E 86° 34′ 09″, N 50° 33′ 01″.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Rock art with the “hanging monster”, in Kalbak-Tash I. The impulse response recorded in front of this rock art panel was employed in the art+ version of the first bundle of IRs. (A) General view with a rectangle where the view provided on the photograph (B) is located. Photo (A) by Margarita Díaz-Andreu. Photo (B) (detail of panel) by Andrej Rozwadowski.

The second bundle included a version of the stimulus set (art+ 2) convolved with another IR from the area of Kalbak-Tash II (in the Lower Chuya River valley; Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a), recorded in front of one of the rock art panels of Kalbak-Tash II, panel 2 (Figure 3, E 86° 41′ 20″, N 50° 24′ 13″). The corresponding art− version of the stimulus set (art− 2) was convolved with an IR recorded in the area of Urkosh, next to the Katun River, in front of a small wall next to a rock panel without rock art, named “rock face 1” (coordinates E 86° 34′ 15″, N 50° 32′ 12″).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Rock art panel in Kalbak-Tash II, panel 2. The impulse response recorded in front of this rock art panel was employed in the art+ version of the second bundle of IRs. Photo by Margarita Díaz-Andreu.

The third bundle included a version of the stimulus set (art+ 3) convolved with an IR recorded in the lower part of the Karakol River valley, before the junction with the river Ursul, in the area of Torgun –an area composed by several rock art panels located south of the locality of Bichiktu-Boom. The panel chosen was Torgun I5 (Figure 4, E 85° 55′ 18″, N 50° 46′ 13″). The corresponding art− version of the stimulus set (art− 3) was convolved with another IR from the area of Urkosh (Díaz-Andreu et al., 2022a), close to the rock art panel of Urkosh IVb, but in front of a wall several meters away from the rock art (E 86° 33′ 35″, N 50° 32′ 51″).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Rock-art panel of Torgun I5, in the Karakol valley. The impulse response recorded in front of this rock art panel was employed in the art+ version of the third bundle of IRs. (A) General view; (B) detail of panel with the representation of a shaman with a drum. Photos by Margarita Díaz-Andreu.

Participants were randomly assigned to three groups of 20, and each group was presented with one of the three bundles of stimuli. From the six described IRs, several acoustic parameters [included in ISO 3382-1 standards, Asociación Española de Normalización y Certificación (AENOR), 2010] were retrieved. These parameters were calculated using the ARTA2 acoustic software, version 1.9.4.1. A selection of the parameters that show some differences between the art+ and art− IRs is presented in Table 1. These are speech clarity (C50, measured in decibels), music clarity (C80, measured in decibels) and reverberation time (EDT, T20 and T30, measured in seconds). Figure 5 shows the spectral analysis of the six IRs, obtained using Audacity version 2.3.2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Values for the acoustic parameters: speech clarity (C50, dB), music clarity (C80, dB), and reverberation time (EDT, T20, and T30, seconds), calculated from the six impulse responses employed in the study.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Spectral analysis of each one of the six impulse responses, presented in pairs of art+ and art− corresponding to each bundle.

2.2.3. Convolution

The convolution of the stimuli with the six impulse responses selected was performed using the software Bidule,3 by Plogue Art et Technologie and the X-MCFX plugin, version 0.5.11.4 The format of the original audio files was mono WAV, and the format of the IRs was 3rd order Ambisonics (Schacher and Kocher, 2006). After the convolutions, it was verified that any of the resulting sounds produced any clipping. The amplitude of the sounds was not normalized after the convolutions; this way, we preserved any amplifications of particular frequency bands that could be generated from the convolution and that could have effects on the affective responses of listeners.

2.3. Rating scales

In order to identify the most suitable emotional and psychoacoustical descriptors for participants to rate the sensations experienced during the listening, a pilot experiment was performed. A pre-selection of items was first made, with 14 items from several validated scales, such as the Circumplex Model of Affect or the Geneva Emotional Musical Scale, that have been employed in many previous behavioral studies with auditory stimuli (Russell, 1980; Watson and Clark, 1988; Zentner et al., 2008; Barron, 2009; Trost et al., 2012; López-Mochales et al., 2022). These items were: presence, spaciousness, envelopment, deepness, closeness, darkness, fright, alert, tension, peace, power, transcendence, calm, and pleasure. Other items from the mentioned scales, such as joy, nostalgia or wonder, were disregarded, because were considered irrelevant for describing sounds without any cultural meaning. Fourteen individuals participated in the pilot experiment (eight women and six men) of ages between 22 and 30 years. They listened to five excerpts of 20 s duration of natural sounds, not convolved with any impulse response, including an insect, a mammal, a bird, a river and the ambient sound of a storm. These sounds were selected from the BBC sound effects repository, and the details are provided in Supplementary Table S3. Pilot participants had to rate how they felt during the listening of each one of the sounds using the 14 pre-selected psychoacoustical and emotional descriptors in scales from 1 to 10, 1 being “not at all” and 10 being “very much.” At the end, the pilot participants had to indicate which three descriptors had been the most “clearly ratables” for each one of the sounds, to determine the most relevant psychoacoustical and emotional dimensions to interrogate in our actual experiment in order to study the reactions of participants to this type of natural sounds.

From the results of the pilot study, we selected the descriptors that pilot participants classified as the top three most “clearly ratable.” We came out with the following final list of 10 descriptors: presence, spaciousness, envelopment, deepness, closeness, alert, tension, peace, calm, and pleasure. The other four descriptors, darkness, fright, power, and transcendence, were disregarded, because did not appear in any of the pilot participants’ top three.

2.4. Procedure

The experiment was carried out in the Immersive Psychoacoustics Laboratory (immpaLAB, Figure 6), located in the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Barcelona (Spain). This facility, consists of a control room and a soundproofed chamber. The latter has a 16 loudspeakers array, disposed in a 3D sphere and equidistant from the participant’s head, that allow rendering sound in third order Ambisonics format. Experiment participants were asked to sit in the center of the soundproofed chamber.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Listening room of the Immersive Psychoacoustics Laboratory of the Faculty of Psychology of the University of Barcelona.

Before starting the experimental session, participants were requested to fill in a series of questionnaires. Since in Western culture, individuals tend to associate long reverberation times with sacred spaces (Baumann and Niederstätter, 2008; Ghaffari and Mofidi, 2014), and reverberation is one of the differential features of some of the stimuli employed in the present study, the sense of religiosity and spirituality of participants was accounted by means of a questionnaire [the Centrality of Religiosity Scale (Huber and Huber, 2012)]. As participants’ level of anxiety could interfere with some of their responses –especially to those sounds and descriptors proven to cause such feeling (Toprac and Abdel-Meguid, 2010)-, this was taken into account, by means of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Spielberger et al., 1983). For each individual we calculated an index of religiosity (ranging 0 to 28) and the two scores derived from the STAI: a state score, s-STAI, to assess anxiety associated to specific situations, and a trait score, t-STAI, to assess the presence of the personality trait that reflects a tendency to suffer state anxiety (Spielberger et al., 1983).

After responding to all questionnaires, participants read the instructions of the experiment, describing the task to be performed, and a definition of each one of the psychoacoustical and emotional labels to be rated. Each participant was assigned to one of the three groups and, therefore, presented with one of the three bundles of stimuli described previously. Stimuli were presented in random order, first in one of the two conditions (art+ or art−) and then with the stimuli in the same order in the other condition. Half participants listened to the sounds presented in the art+ condition first and in the art− condition second, and the other half, the other way around. Participants listened to a total of 44 sounds of 20 s length (a set of 17 sounds, plus five additional sounds played at the beginning, all presented in two conditions). The volume was adjusted at a comfortable level by experimenters, and sounds were presented at the same level to all participants. After listening to each sound, participants had to rate the 10 descriptors mentioned above, according to how they perceived the sound and how it made them feel. Each descriptor was rated from 0 (meaning “not at all”) to 10 (meaning “very much”).

The experiment was carried out using an electronic tablet. The ratings were obtained using two customable spider charts, one for the psychoacoustical labels and another for the emotional labels, with five radii each (Figure 7). Each radius represented one of the descriptors, with the corresponding label at the end. Participants had to drag and drop a pointer to a specific position along the radius, indicating the punctuation they wanted to assign to each descriptor, from 1 (dragging and dropping the pointer to the center of the circle) to 10 (dragging and dropping the pointer to the end of the radius). This method for rating behavioral scales was chosen over a text version so that the process of selecting/confirming options with multiple assessments were speeded and undertaken in a short time. A total of 44 screens, one for each sound, were presented in the tablet, and the results were automatically saved in a CSV file. The experiment had an approximate duration of 75–90 min.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Tablet screen with customable spider charts for rating the descriptors of the experiment.

2.5. Statistical analysis

In order to consider the scores religiosity, sSTAI and tSTAI, derived from the questionnaires presented to participants, as covariates in the models, each score’s effect on the response variables was studied using linear regression. Observing the outcome of the analyses, no effects of the scores were found on the ratings of the descriptors (see Supplementary Table S4). Therefore, the scores were not included as covariates in the models.

A total of 10 mixed ANOVA analyses were carried out, one for each psychoacoustical and emotional descriptor. This approach considered the analysis of the ratings for each one of the 10 descriptors as responding to a different research question separately, addressing participants’ perception of auralizations from art+ spots as more or less present, spatious, enveloping, deep, close, and evoking more or less alert, tension, peace, calm, and pleasure. Although the scorings of the descriptors were ordinal, they were considered as intervals as recommended by Carifio and Perla, 2007. Even though some of the data groups did not distribute normally, we opted to use parametric methods, which are deemed robust for the analysis of data from scales with ordinal scorings (Norman, 2010), and have been used in previous similar studies (Miu and Balteş, 2012; Pearce and Halpern, 2015; Hanser et al., 2016). Other reasons to disregard the normality assumption are the large sample size and that the number of observations was constant across the groups (Feir-Walsh and Toothaker, 1974; Blanca et al., 2017). On each one of these 10 models, the response variable was the descriptor rating, and two within-subject factors -sound (with 17 levels, corresponding to each of the sounds that formed the stimulus set), and art (with two levels, art+ and art−)- and one between-subject factor -group (with three levels)- were included.

Post-hoc comparisons, using Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks tests, were performed for an in-depth study of the factors’ interactions. All the p-values were corrected for multiple testing using the Benjamini-Hochberg (FDR) method. In all cases, the significance level considered was α = 0.05. All the analyses were carried out using the software R (version 3.6.2) and the ez package (Lawrence, 2016).

Since some of the significant results obtained in the post-hoc comparisons concerned the ratings attributed to specific sounds from the stimulus set, the frequency spectrum of these sounds after their convolution was analyzed, using Audacity1 version 2.3.2, to help in the interpretation of the results.

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral experiment

After carrying out the mixed ANOVA analyses on our experimental data, including the within-subject factors sound and art, and the between-subject factor group, all the descriptors’ ratings were influenced by the single factor sound. However, for the interests of the present study, the implications of the isolated effect of the factor sound were not further developed. Instead, the focus was put on the interactions between all the factors of the experimental design, including sound as well as art and group.

A significant effect of the factor of interest (art) was observed over the ratings of the descriptors present [F(1,57) = 4.845, p = 0.0454], close [F(1,57) = 15.872, p = 0.0010], alert [F(1,57) = 8.131, p = 0.0101], tension [F(1,57) = 27.876, p = 2.1·10−5], peace [F(1,57) = 13.026, p = 0.0020], calm [F(1,57) = 12.550, p = 0.0020] and pleasure [F(1,57) = 10.503, p = 0.0040]. However, the low values of η2 indicate that this factor, isolated, does not explain much of the variability in the response variable. Therefore, the focus was put on studying the effect of factors’ interactions by carrying out post-hoc tests. The summary of all mixed ANOVA analyses is presented in Supplementary Table S5.

A significant influence of the interaction of the factors art and group was found in the ratings of the descriptors present [F(2,57) = 6.415, p = 0.0154], close [F(2,57) = 11.565, p = 0.0006] and tension [F(2,57) = 5.653, p = 0.0192]. Post-hoc analyses were performed for the ratings of each one of these descriptors, using Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks tests. We analyzed the effect of the factor art on each group separately.

For the descriptors present and close, significant differences were found between the values attributed to the art+ and art− versions of the stimuli, in the third experimental group solely. For the descriptor tension, significant differences were found between the values attributed to the art+ and art− versions of the stimuli, in the first and third experimental groups. Details of the analyses are presented in Supplementary Table S6. Participants from the third experimental group rated the sounds convolved with the impulse response recorded in front of the rock art panel of Torgun I5 (art+) as more present (M = 7.338, SD = 2.118), closer (M = 7.182, SD = 2.124), as well as evoking more tension (M = 5.747, SD = 3.174) in comparison to the art−, in which the sounds were rated as less present (M = 6.511, SD = 2.065), less close (M = 6.171, SD = 2.193) and evoking less tension (M = 4.774, SD = 3.047). Participants from the first experimental group also rated the sound convolved with the art+ IR recorded in front of the rock art panel of Kalbak-Tash I as evoking more tension (M = 5.279, SD = 3.265) compared to the art− version of the sounds (M = 4.915, SD = 3.388; Figure 8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Post-hoc comparisons of the ratings of the descriptors present, close and tension in the different experimental groups, using Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks tests to study the differences between the art+ and art− conditions. The p-values displayed have been corrected for multiple testing.

The ratings of the tension descriptor were significantly influenced by the triple interaction of the factors art, group and sound [F(32,912) = 2.241, p = 0.0001]. Thus, post-hoc analyses were performed analyzing the factor art in each group and sound, using Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks tests. A total of 51 tests were carried out, and after correcting the p-values for multiple comparisons, some significant differences were found between the ratings of the art+ and art− versions of the sounds of the plover, the snow and the waterfall (Supplementary Table S7). Participants from the first group rated the sound of the plover in the art+ condition –convolved with the IR recorded in front of the rock art panel of Kalbak-Tash I- as significantly less tension-evoking (M = 3.350, SD = 2.368) than in the art− (M = 5.650, SD = 2.834). On the contrary, in the third group, participants rated the sound of the plover in the art+ condition -convolved with the IR recorded in front of the panel of Torgun I5- as more tension-evoking (M = 6.050, SD = 2.564) than in the art− (M = 4.150, SD = 1.725). Participants from the third group also rated the sounds of the snow and the waterfall as more tension-evoking in the art+ condition (snow: M = 6.150, SD = 1.981; waterfall: M = 4.200, SD = 2.895) than in the art− (snow: M = 4.600, SD = 2.437; waterfall: M = 2.250, SD = 2.573; Figure 9).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Post-hoc comparisons of the ratings of the descriptor tension in the different experimental groups, for the sounds of plover, snow and waterfall, using Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks tests to study the differences between the art+ and art− conditions. The p-values displayed have been corrected for multiple testing.

3.2. Spectral analysis

Several studies have suggested a relationship between emotional valence elicited by sound and the energy distribution along the frequency spectrum (Scherer, 1986; Banse and Scherer, 1996; Waaramaa et al., 2010; Patel et al., 2011). Hence, we studied the spectrum of the sound of plover convolved with the impulse responses conforming the first and third bundles (Figure 10). We expected to observe an amplification of particular frequencies in some of its versions that could explain the effect of art acting in opposite direction in the first and third groups, as described in the previous section.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Spectral amplitude (dBFS) in the frequency range between 2000-5000 Hz of the sound of plover convolved with the four impulse responses forming the bundles 1 (dark colours) and 3 (light colours), listened by participants from groups 1 and 3 respectively, in art− (in front of a wall away from rock art panels, colour blue) and art+ (in front of a panel with rock art, colour pink) conditions.

The peak frequency of the sound of plover was localized at 3188 Hz. The differences in amplitude at this frequency between the four versions of the sound -thus, convolved with the IRs- were studied. The art+ version of the sound of plover that participants from the first group were presented with was 11.65 dB lower than the art− one at the peak frequency of 3,188 Hz. On the contrary, the art+ version of the sound of plover that participants from the third group listened to was 1.08 dB louder than the art− one at the peak frequency.

The analysis of the ratings of tension attributed to the sounds of snow and waterfall did not provide more information than those obtained previously, in the analysis of the effect of the interaction between the factors art and group, which showed that participants from the third group experienced more tension when listening to the art+ version of sounds. For this reason the frequency spectrum of the sounds of snow and waterfall was not studied.

4. Discussion

In this study, we have examined the perceptual and emotional reactions elicited by sounds from natural sources convolved with six impulse responses recorded in several rock art emplacements in Altai (Russia). We have observed the effect of the different acoustic prints in participants’ self-reported impressions using emotional and psychoacoustical scales, and studied if the perception of the acoustics of the site could have played any role in the selection for rock art production.

Participants from the third experimental group reported hearing the sounds as more present, closer and more tension-evoking when convolved with the impulse response recorded in front of the rock art panel of Torgun I5, compared to its art− version. Participants from the first group also reported their art+ version of the sounds, convolved with the IR recorded in front of the rock art panel of Kalbak Tash I, as more tension-evoking than the corresponding art−. These results, at first sight, support the hypothesis that there are some perceivable differences between the art+ and art− acoustics and that these differences are consistent across rock art sites, but it conflicts with the results from the post-hoc analysis of the tension evoked by each sound separately. While participants from the third experimental group reported feeling greater tension when listening to the sounds of plover, snow and waterfall convolved with the art+ IR, participants from the first experimental group reported the opposite about the sound of plover: feeling less tension when listening to this sound convolved with the art+ IR, compared to the art−.

The ratings of sounds convolved with the IR recorded in front of the rock art panel of Torgun I5, by participants from the third group, as closer and more present, can be related with reverberation parameters. Although the reverberation times of all the IRs used in the present study are considerably short, they are nevertheless longer in all the IRs recorded in front of rock panels without rock art (art−) than in the IRs recorded in front of rock art panels (art+), and the differences are above the Just Noticeable Difference (JND) for reverberation time (Bork, 2000; Martellotta et al., 2009). Since at constant sound level and distance, a longer reverberation entails a greater perceived distance from the source [Bronkhorst and Houtgast, 1999; Kolarik et al., 2016; Strutt Help. Reverberant Sound Level (From RT), 2020], this may explain why sounds convolved with the art+ IR of the panel of Torgun I5, with shorter reverberation than its art−, are perceived as closer and more present. Reverberation could also explain the stronger feeling of tension elicited to participants from the first and third experimental groups by sounds convolved with the IRs recorded in front of the rock art panels of Kalbak-Tash I and Torgun I5. Reverberation is suspected to be inversely related to fear-like emotions (Mo et al., 2016) although this has not been strongly demonstrated. In the case of the IR recorded in Kalbak-Tash I, the stronger feeling of tension elicited can be also explained by the natural sound amplification produced in the rock art sites of the area, according to Díaz-Andreu et al. (2022a), since tension-like feelings are directly related with loudness (Granot and Eitan, 2011; Ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux, 2016).

The results revealed by the analysis of the ratings of tension for each sound separately, in the case of the sounds of snow and waterfall seem consistent with this argument. However, we observed an odd behavior of the results of the sound of plover. While the third group reported feeling greater tension when listening to this sound convolved with the art+ IR from the panel of Torgun I5, in line with the results described so far, the first group reported the opposite: feeling greater tension when listening to the art− version of the sound. If we observe the spectral density of this sound convolved with each IR from bundles 1 and 3 (see Figure 10), the amplitude at the peak frequency of 3,188 Hz is 1.08 dB greater in the art+ version of the third bundle (convolved with the IR from the rock art panel of Torgun I5) compared to its corresponding art− IR. This difference is short but above the JND (Long, 2014). Nevertheless, in the case of the first bundle, the opposite happens: amplitude of the frequency peak at 3188 Hz is 11.65 dB greater in the art− version of the sound than in the corresponding art+ (convolved with the IR from the rock art panel of Kalbak Tash I). As we stated previously, tension-like feelings are directly related with loudness (Granot and Eitan, 2011; Ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux, 2016) and this relation is especially prominent when sounds are characterized by high frequencies (Ilie and Thompson, 2006; Juslin and Västfjäll, 2008; Granot and Eitan, 2011). Thus, in this case, the marked high-frequency peak amplification, especially in the first bundle of IRs, might be masking the slight difference in reverberation, and positioning as the main cue responsible for eliciting greater tension in the art− version of the sound of plover, in participants from the first experimental group. As we have confirmed, this one single result does not influence the general tendency observed when analyzing the effect of the factor art in the tension attributed to all the sounds by participants from the first experimental group.

As a first attempt to investigate the impact of archaeological sites acoustics on the selection of specific places for rock art paintings or carvings by past pre-state societies in modern listeners, and as previously alluded to in the introduction, the present study has a number of limitations. The first limitation is related to the IRs employed, as these were recorded a few thousand years after the actual moment of the rock art production, so that the landscapes might have suffered a series of modifications due to natural or human factors. Some sophisticated solutions to overcome this problem may involve 3D modeling to represent the sites of interest and to simulate the acoustic reflections to obtain virtual IRs. This process is feasible when dealing with ancient buildings such as odea and theaters (Manzetti, 2016; Weinzierl and Lepa, 2017), but much more difficult when dealing with landscapes.

A second limitation relates to the nature of the actual stimulus sets used in the present experiment. In order to reproduce a soundscape accurately, the sounds employed in the convolutions must be anechoic or quasi-anechoic (Farina and Ayalon, 2003). In the present study, anechoic recordings from natural Siberian sources, such as animals, were impossible to record, but a quasi-anechoic version may be obtained by the deconvolution of the recorded sound with the IR from the place where it was recorded (Gemba, 2014). This approach should also be considered in further research, particularly when natural sounds are employed. For these reasons, the sounds obtained from the convolutions might not be a completely accurate representation of the soundscapes in the moment when rock art was produced, and this must be taken into account when extracting conclusions regarding the possible reasons why these places, and not others, were decorated.

A final concern regards reductionism. The present experiment was set as an empirical investigation in the present of a far-reaching question: how past societies may have considered acoustic cues to place their sacred spaces, and spaces for social interaction, thousands of years ago. It is essential to pay close attention to the conclusions that can be obtained from such an approximation of the past in the present. Emotion, one of the main objects of study of this work, must be studied through the lens of culture. Certain acoustics might exalt certain emotions in modern-day listeners that might have been experienced by individuals in the past as well, but their cultural meanings, starting from what we consider positive and negative emotions, are probably very different. It is still not clear which emotions are liable to historical inquiry (Petri, 2013) and for this reason, we cannot fully understand yet the role of emotion in the process of selecting the spaces to produce the rock art. It has been previously mentioned that rock art is the mark we have in the present to determine the locales where social and ritual activities were most likely set (Díaz-Andreu and Mattioli, 2015), but the nature of such activities has radically evolved in its ways and its functions have also been transformed ever since. The concept of ritual itself has diverse meanings. While in modern western cultures ritual carries connotations of religion and sacredness, and it must be understood in the context of the modern knowledge, in the past myths and beliefs were the only way to understand the world and to organize a society (Koutrafouri, 2008). The problem of experimentation with modern-day participants is not only that the ritual-like situation is not recreated with all its variables, but also that it has never and never will be experienced by modern-day individuals in the same way it was at the moment when the rock art was produced. In summary, the hypothesis we set and the conclusions we reached can guide us in our study of the past, but will still relate to the present. Yet, to our understanding, this is one of the probably few approaches we have.

5. Conclusion

Based on the results obtained, our objectives have been partially met. On the one hand, an effect of the acoustic environment has been observed in some of the rock art sites studied over the ratings of presence, closeness and tension. On the other hand, however, only one effect has been found to be consistent across two of the three rock art sites: participants from the first and third experimental groups reported feeling greater tension when listening to the sounds convolved with the IRs from the recording positions in front of the rock art panels. We suggested the role of the reverberation times of the particular IRs used in this study as a potential explanation for this result. Thus, in future experiments, the range of psychoacoustical and emotional dimensions should be expanded, based on the differential acoustic features between the IRs from recording spots with and without rock art.

No commonalities were found between the three experimental groups. Therefore, further research is necessary in order to find more common features between rock art sites, from the acoustic, psychoacoustic and emotional point of view, to support the idea that acoustics was a factor to be taken into account when producing rock art. Finally, the inclusion of the factor of prehistoric period, or functionality of the rock art site might produce interesting results in the future.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Bioethics Commission of the University of Barcelona. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

SL-M, RA-T, and CE conceived the study and designed the experimental approach. SL-M acquired, analyzed, and interpreted the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work is part of the ERC Artsoundscapes project (grant agreement no. 787842) that has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program. PI: MD-A. CE was also supported by the Generalitat de Catalunya SGR2017-974, de María de Maeztu Center of Excellence (Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona) CEX2021-001159-M, Ministry of Science and Innovation, and the ICREA Acadèmia Distinguished Professorship Award.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1188567/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Audacity (version 2.3.3) [Computer software]. USA. Retrieved from https://www.audacityteam.org/.

2. ^ARTA Acoustic Software, by ARTA Labs: https://artalabs.hr/.

3. ^Bidule, by Plogue INC, advanced modular audio software: https://www.plogue.com/products/bidule.html.

4. ^X-MCFX Vst plugin, version 0.5.11 (2014). Mattias Kronlachner [Computer software]. Available online at: http://www.matthiaskronlachner.com/.

References

Alvarez-Morales, L., Lopez, M., and Álvarez-Corbacho, Á. (2020). The acoustic environment of York Minster’s chapter house. Acoustics 2, 13–36. doi: 10.3390/acoustics2010003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez-Morales, L., Santos da Rosa, N., Benítez-Aragón, D., Fernández Macías, L., Lazarich, M., and Díaz-Andreu, M. (2023). The Bacinete Main shelter: a prehistoric theatre? Acoustics 5, 299–319. doi: 10.3390/acoustics5010018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asociación Española de Normalización y Certificación (AENOR). (2010). Acústica. Medición de parámetros acústicos en recintos. Parte 1: Salas de espectáculos. (ISO 3382-1:2010).

Google Scholar

Banse, R., and Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 614–636. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barron, M. (2009). Auditorium acoustics and architectural design. London: Spon Press/Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Baumann, D., and Niederstätter, C. (2008). “Acoustics in sacred buildings” in Sacred buildings (Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter), 54–59.

Google Scholar

Blake, E. C., and Cross, I. (2015). The acoustic and auditory contexts of human behavior. Curr. Anthropol. 56, 81–103. doi: 10.1086/679445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanca, M. J., Alarcón, R., Arnau, J., Bono, R., and Bendayan, R. (2017). Non-normal data: Is ANOVA still a valid option? Psichothema 29, 552–557. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bork, I. (2000). A comparison of room simulation software - the 2nd round Robin on room acoustical computer simulation. Acta Acust. United Acust. 86, 943–956.

Google Scholar

Borodovskiĭ, A. P., Gorokhov, S. V., Kubarev, G. V., Bogdanov, E. S., Derevianko, A. P., and Molodin, V. I. (2016). Arkheologicheskoe nasledie Respubliki Altai po trasse proektiruemogo gazoprovoda “Sila Sibiri-2” (“Altai”): po materialam issledovaniĭ 2011–2012 godov. Novosibirsk: Izdatel′stvo IAĖT SO RAN.

Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology 37, 204–215. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, J. S., Reich, R., and Norcross, S. G. (1999). A just noticeable difference in C 50 for speech. Appl. Acoust. 58, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/S0003-682X(98)00075-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronkhorst, A. W., and Houtgast, T. (1999). Auditory distance perception in rooms. Nature 397, 517–520. doi: 10.1038/17374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carew, T. J., and Ramaswami, M. (2020). The Neurohumanities: an emerging Partnership for Exploring the human experience. Neuron 108, 590–593. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carifio, J., and Perla, R. J. (2007). Ten common misunderstandings, misconceptions, persistent myths and urban legends about Likert scales and Likert response formats and their antidotes. J. Soc. Sci. 3, 106–116. doi: 10.3844/jssp.2007.106.116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Andreu, M., Jiménez Pasalodos, R., Rozwadowski, A., Álvarez Morales, L., Miklashevich, E., and Santos da Rosa, N. (2022a). The soundscapes of the lower Chuya River area, Russian Altai: ethnographic sources, indigenous ontologies and the Archaeoacoustics of rock art sites. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 30, 335–362. doi: 10.1007/s10816-022-09562-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Andreu, M., Jiménez-Pasalodos, R., Rozwadowski, A., Alvarez Morales, L., Benítez-Aragón, D., Miklashevich, E., et al. (2022b). Paisajes vivos y ontologías sonoras en Altai (Siberia, Rusia): el proyecto europeo Artsoundscapes (ERC ref. 787842) ante el reto del contexto intangible del arte rupestre. Actualidad de La Investigación Arqueológica En España IV. Conferencias Impartidas En El Museo Arqueológico Nacional, Madrid, 165–181.

Google Scholar

Díaz-Andreu, M., and Mattioli, T.. (2015). Archaeoacoustics of rock art: quantitative approaches to the acoustics and soundscape of rock art. Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Conference on Computer Applications and Quantitative Methods in Archaeology, 1 and 2, Siena.

Google Scholar

Díaz-Andreu, M., and Mattioli, T. (2017). “Rock art, music, and acoustics: a global overview” in The Oxford handbook of the archaeology and anthropology of rock art. eds. B. David and I. J. McNiven (Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press), 503–528.

Google Scholar

Díaz-Andreu, M., Mattioli, T., and Rainsbury, M. (2021). “The cultural understanding of sound in rock art landscapes: the limits of interpretation” in Perspectives on differences in rock art. eds. J. M. Gjerde and M. S. Arntzen (London: Equinox), 244–265.

Google Scholar

Farina, A. (2007). Impulse response measurements. 23rd Nordic Sound Symposium, Notodden, 27–30.

Google Scholar

Farina, A. (2020). How we take acoustical measurements in the Artsoundscapes project, and how we got there. Artsoundscapes Blog Available at: https://www.ub.edu/artsoundscapes/how-we-take-acoustical-measurements-in-the-artsoundscapes-project/

Google Scholar

Farina, A., and Ayalon, R. (2003). Recording concert hall acoustics for posterity. Proceedings of the AES 24th international conference on multichannel audio, June 26-28, Banff.

Google Scholar

Farina, A., and Chiesi, L. (2016). Measuring spatial MIMO impulse responses in rooms employing spherical transducer arrays. Conference on Sound Field Control, Guildford.

Google Scholar

Feir-Walsh, B. J., and Toothaker, L. E. (1974). An empirical comparison of the Anova F-test, Normal scores test and Kruskal-Wallis test under violation of assumptions. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 34, 789–799. doi: 10.1177/001316447403400406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gemba, K. L. (2014). Characterization of underwater acoustic sources recorded in reverberant environments with application to scuba signatures. Honolulu: University of Hawaii.

Google Scholar

Ghaffari, A., and Mofidi, S. M. (2014). Comparing reverberation time in west churches and mosques of Qajar era in Tabriz. Armanshahr Architect. Urban Dev. 7, 13–29.

Google Scholar

Granot, R. Y., and Eitan, Z. (2011). Musical tension and the interaction of dynamic auditory parameters. Music. Percept. 28, 219–246. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagman, F. (2010). Emotional Response to Sound: Influence of Spatial Determinants. [Master’s Thesis]. Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Chalmers University of Technology, Göteborg (Sweden).

Google Scholar

Hanser, W. E., ter Bogt, T. F. M., Van den Tol, A. J. M., Mark, R. E., and Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2016). Consolation through music: a survey study. Music. Sci. 20, 122–137. doi: 10.1177/1029864915620264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S., and Huber, O. W. (2012). The centrality of religiosity scale (CRS). Religions 3, 710–724. doi: 10.3390/rel3030710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilie, G., and Thompson, W. F. (2006). A comparison of the effects of music and speech prosody on three dimensions of affective experience. 9th international conference on music perception and cognition, Bologna.

Google Scholar

Juslin, P. N., and Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 559–575. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolar, M. A. (2013). Tuned to the senses: an Archaeoacoustic perspective on ancient Chavín. Appendix 1

Google Scholar

Kolarik, A. J., Moore, B. C. J., Zahorik, P., Cirstea, S., and Pardhan, S. (2016). Auditory distance perception in humans: a review of cues, development, neuronal bases, and effects of sensory loss. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 78, 373–395. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-1015-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutrafouri, V. G. (2008). Ritual in prehistory; definition and identification. Religious insights in early prehistoric Cyprus [University of Edimburgh]. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/1842/3288.

Google Scholar

Lawrence, M. A. (2016). Ez: easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments R package (4.4-0). R Core Team.

Google Scholar

Long, M. (2014). “Human perception and reaction to sound” in Architectural acoustics (Boston: Elsevier), 81–127.

Google Scholar

López-Mochales, S., Jiménez-Pasalodos, R., Valenzuela, J., Gutiérrez-Cajaraville, C., Díaz-Andreu, M., and Escera, C. (2022). Experimental enhancement of feelings of transcendence, tenderness, and expressiveness by music in Christian liturgical spaces. Front. Psychol. 13:844029. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.844029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzetti, M. C. (2016). 3D visibility analysis as a tool to validate ancient theatre reconstructions: the case of the large Roman theatre of Gortyn. Virt. Archaeol. Rev. 7:36. doi: 10.4995/var.2016.5922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquis-Favre, C., Vallin, P., Gille, L., and Ellermeier, W. (2019). Estimation of psychoacoustic indices and annoying auditory sensations from sound pressure levels of urban road traffic. 23rd International Congress on Acoustics, Aachen, 7979–7986.

Google Scholar

Martellotta, F., Cirillo, E., Carbonari, A., and Ricciardi, P. (2009). Guidelines for acoustical measurements in churches. Appl. Acoust. 70, 378–388. doi: 10.1016/j.apacoust.2008.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miklashevich, Е. A., and Bove, L. L. (2010). Research of the Torgun Mountain petroglyphs in the Ongudai region of the republic of Altai. Problems of Archaeology, Ethnography, and Anthropology of Siberia and Neighboring Territories 16, 229–233.

Google Scholar

Ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux. (2016). Exemples de sources de bruit et de réactions humaines selon le niveau de bruit.

Google Scholar

Miu, A. C., and Balteş, F. R. (2012). Empathy manipulation impacts music-induced emotions: a psychophysiological study on opera. PLoS One 7:e30618. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mo, R., Wu, B., and Horner, A. (2016). The effects of reverberation on the emotional characteristics of musical instruments. J. Audio Eng. Soc. 63, 966–979. doi: 10.17743/jaes.2015.0082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, G. (2010). Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 15, 625–632. doi: 10.1007/s10459-010-9222-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, S., Banzigar, T., and Scherer, K. R. (2011). Perception of the proximal voice features for emotional speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130:2574. doi: 10.1121/1.3655320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pätynen, J., and Lokki, T. (2016). Concert halls with strong and lateral sound increase the emotional impact of orchestra music. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 139, 1214–1224. doi: 10.1121/1.4944038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, M. T., and Halpern, A. R. (2015). Age-related patterns in emotions evoked by music. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 9, 248–253. doi: 10.1037/a0039279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petri, R. (2013). The idea of culture and the history of emotions. Historein 12:21. doi: 10.12681/historein.160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainio, R., Lahelma, A., Äikäs, T., Lassfolk, K., and Okkonen, J. (2018). Acoustic measurements and digital image processing suggest a link between sound rituals and sacred sites in northern Finland. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 25, 453–474. doi: 10.1007/s10816-017-9343-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos da Rosa, N., Álvarez Morales, L., Martorell Briz, X., Fernández Macías, L., and Díaz-Andreu, M. (2023). The acoustics of aggregation sites: listening to the rock art landscape of Cuevas de la Araña (Spain). J. Field Archaeol. 48, 130–143. doi: 10.1080/00934690.2022.2134964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarre, C., and Lawson, G. (2006). Archaeoacoustics McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research Available at: https://books.google.es/books?id=tSBmAAAAMAAJ.

Google Scholar

Schacher, J. C., and Kocher, P. (2006). Ambisonics spatialization tools for max/msp. Omni 500, 274–277.

Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: a review and a model for future research. Psychol. Bull. 99, 143–165. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.99.2.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory.

Google Scholar

Strutt Help. Reverberant Sound Level (From RT). (2020). ARUP Acoustic Engineering Services. https://strutt.arup.com/help/Building_Acoustics/Lw2Lp.htm

Google Scholar

Surazakov, A. S. (1996). “Sviatilishche Bol’shoi Ialoman III” in Aktual’nye problemy sibirskoi arkheologii (Barnaul: Altaiskii gosudarstvennyi universitet), 82–84.

Google Scholar

Toprac, P., and Abdel-Meguid, A. (2010). “Causing fear, suspense, and anxiety using sound Design in Computer Games” in Game sound technology and player interaction (New York: IGI Global), 176–191.

Google Scholar

Trost, W., Ethofer, T., Zentner, M., and Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Mapping aesthetic musical emotions in the brain. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2769–2783. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, J., Díaz-Andreu, M., and Escera, C. (2020). Psychology meets archaeology: Psychoarchaeoacoustics for understanding ancient minds and their relationship to the sacred. Front. Psychol. 11:550794. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.550794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Västfjäll, D. (2003). The subjective sense of presence, emotion recognition, and experienced emotions in auditory virtual environments. CyberPsychol. Behav. 6, 181–188. doi: 10.1089/109493103321640374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Västfjäll, D. (2012). Emotional reactions to sounds without meaning. Psychology 3, 606–609. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.38091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Västfjäll, D., Larsson, P., and Kleiner, M. (2002). Emotion and auditory virtual environments: affect-based judgments of music reproduced with virtual reverberation times. CyberPsychol. Behav. 5, 19–32. doi: 10.1089/109493102753685854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigeant, M. C., and Celmer, R. D.. (2010). Effect of experimental design on the results of clarity-index just-noticeable-difference listening tests. 20th International Congress on Acoustics 2010, ICA 2010 - Incorporating Proceedings of the 2010 Annual Conference of the Australian Acoustical Society, Sydney, 2567–2573.

Google Scholar

Vorländer, M. (2008). Auralization. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.

Google Scholar

Waaramaa, T., Laukkanen, A.-M., Airas, M., and Alku, P. (2010). Perception of emotional valences and activity levels from vowel segments of continuous speech. J. Voice 24, 30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2008.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinzierl, S., and Lepa, S. (2017). “On the epistemic potential of virtual realities for the historical sciences. A methodological framework” in Augmented reality (Berlin: De Gruyter), 61–80.

Google Scholar

Witew, I. B., Behler, G. K., and Vorländer, M. (2005). About just noticeable differences for aspects of spatial impressions in concert halls. Acoust. Sci. Technol. 26, 185–192. doi: 10.1250/ast.26.185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion 8, 494–521. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: acoustic perception, emotion, rock art, psychoacoustics, archaeoacoustics

Citation: López-Mochales S, Aparicio-Terrés R, Díaz-Andreu M and Escera C (2023) Acoustic perception and emotion evocation by rock art soundscapes of Altai (Russia). Front. Psychol. 14:1188567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1188567

Received: 17 March 2023; Accepted: 04 September 2023;
Published: 19 September 2023.

Edited by:

Virginia Penhune, Concordia University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Yoshiharu Soeta, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Japan
Etienne Thoret, Aix-Marseille Université, France

Copyright © 2023 López-Mochales, Aparicio-Terrés, Díaz-Andreu and Escera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carles Escera, cescera@ub.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.