Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 29 August 2023
Sec. Emotion Science

The inversion effect on the cubic humanness-uncanniness relation in humanlike agents

  • 1Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, Kyoto, Japan
  • 2Cardiff University School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

The uncanny valley describes the typically nonlinear relation between the esthetic appeal of artificial entities and their human likeness. The effect has been attributed to specialized (configural) processing that increases sensitivity to deviations from human norms. We investigate this effect in computer-generated, humanlike android and human faces using dynamic facial expressions. Angry and happy expressions with varying degrees of synchrony were presented upright and inverted and rated on their eeriness, strangeness, and human likeness. A sigmoidal function of human likeness and uncanniness (“uncanny slope”) was found for upright expressions and a linear relation for inverted faces. While the function is not indicative of an uncanny valley, the results support the view that configural processing moderates the effect of human likeness on uncanniness and extend its role to dynamic facial expressions.

The uncanny valley in human–robot interaction

Humanlike robots and androids are being increasingly used in society. Social robots provide an alternative to human caretakers for older adults to mitigate loneliness or as companions for children with autism spectrum disorder (Adams and Robinson, 2011; Kumazaki et al., 2017; Fattal et al., 2020). However, robotic assistance may encounter barriers to acceptance, especially in tasks typically requiring human–human interaction, such as personal care or leisure activities (Smarr et al., 2014). One potential barrier is the uncanny valley effect, in which individuals feel an aversion to artificial entities that closely resemble humans (Mori et al., 2012). Research on the uncanny valley typically relies on self-report measures of human likeness or realism for the independent variable and affect measures for the dependent variable, typically likability, eeriness, or a related measure (Diel et al., 2022). Human likeness is a multidimensional construct describing an entity’s closeness to human norms in outward appearance, behavior, or inner states, although these components tend to be unspecified or combined in self-reported human likeness measures to a holistic perception of humanness (Mori et al., 2012; von Zitzewitz et al., 2013; Ho and MacDorman, 2017; Diel et al., 2022). Uncanniness is considered a specific negative experience associated with emotions like fear, anxiety, and disgust, is potentially linked to the inability to explain presented information, and is considered a specific component of the uncanny valley (Ho et al., 2008; MacDorman and Entezari, 2015; Mangan, 2015; Diel et al., 2022).

Many empirical studies have investigated the relation between human likeliness and emotional responses to humanlike artificial entities. Although the results are somewhat inconsistent across studies (Diel et al., 2022), a recent meta-analysis analyzed the data of 49 studies testing the relation between human likeness and likeability to robot agents (Mara et al., 2022). Researchers found the relation between multiple artificial entities’ human likeness and likability was a cubic (sigmoidal) function. The results indicate that the relation between the human likeness and emotional impressions of artificial entities takes on a nonlinear shape, which could be associated with the uncanny valley phenomenon.

However, uncertainties remain on whether there is a sigmoidal relation between likability and anthropomorphism. First, the meta-analysis by Mara et al. (2022) focused on the Godspeed likability scales (Bartneck et al., 2009) and non-realistic humanlike robots (e.g., NAO). In contrast, uncanny valley research typically assesses the uncanniness of more realistic robots, computer-generated (CG) characters, and fully human stimuli (Diel et al., 2022).

While multiple psychological mechanisms underlying the cubic relation between human likeness and emotional impressions have been proposed and investigated, there is little consensus on the exact processes (Wang et al., 2015; Reuten et al., 2018; Kätsyri et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2020; Diel and MacDorman, 2021). Categorization difficulty or ambiguity has been proposed to cause uncanniness in entities lying at the borders between human and robot categories (Yamada et al., 2013; Cheetham et al., 2014). Evolutionarily, deviating features in an otherwise humanlike entity may elicit disease avoidance mechanisms (MacDorman and Entezari, 2015). Anomalies or deviations in human norms may also elicit error signals in predictive coding (Saygin et al., 2012). On a perceptual level, mismatching or atypical features may appear eerie or uncanny, especially in more realistic and human entities (MacDorman et al., 2009; Kätsyri et al., 2019; Diel and MacDorman, 2021).

This study focuses on the last explanation. We hypothesize that the candidate mechanisms include the configural processing of faces and facial expressions. The uncanny valley effect has been attributed to the specialized processing of familiar categories resulting in stronger error signals for artificial entities (MacDorman et al., 2009; Chattopadhyay and MacDorman, 2016; Kätsyri et al., 2019; Diel and MacDorman, 2021; Diel and Lewis, 2022). Specialized processing describes a particular type of domain-specific cognitive processing marked by an improved ability to discriminate or recognize individual exemplars and by the recruitment of specialized neural areas (Tanaka and Farah, 1993; Kanwisher, 2000; Carbon and Leder, 2006). Atypicalities or deviations may induce negative esthetic evaluations, especially sensitive for stimulus categories that elicit specialized processing.

Configural processing is a form of specialized processing, and the configural processing of faces depends on their upright orientation. The inversion of faces disrupts this processing (inversion effect; Kanwisher and Moscovitch, 2000; Carbon and Leder, 2006). Configural processing also improves the processing of facial expressions and is disrupted when expressions are inverted (Ambadar et al., 2005; Bould and Morris, 2008; Tobin et al., 2016). Facial esthetic ratings’ variance decreases when faces are inverted, likely because face processing becomes less accurate (Bäuml, 1994; Santos and Young, 2008; Leder et al., 2017). Furthermore, uncanniness ratings of faces are less severe when faces are inverted due to a decreased ability to detect changes or distortions in a face (Diel and Lewis, 2022). However, neither the effect of inversion on the likability of entities varying in human likeness nor the uncanniness of dynamic facial expressions has been investigated. As specialized processing is more pronounced in more realistic faces (Crookes et al., 2015), inversion may disrupt more subtle differences in esthetic ratings of highly realistic dynamic expressions. This effect of specialized processing on dynamic face processing may explain why subtle facial movements in realistic androids may appear eerie or uncanny.

The processing of dynamic emotion expressions has been well-investigated using virtual (computer-generated) agents whose temporal trajectory of face muscle movement can be easily manipulated and controlled (Krumhuber et al., 2012; Pan and Hamilton, 2018). Although virtual agents are not physically present when interacting with a human, they offer a way to study the effects of specialized processing in the uncanniness of emotion expressions alongside android stimuli.

This work aims to investigate whether the relation between human likeness and uncanniness is cubic for humanlike agents and whether the relation is mediated by configural processing. To test the humanlike agents, we presented dynamic emotional expressions of human, android, and CG faces. To test the effect of configural processing of faces, we compared upright and inverted facial stimuli. We presented the various types of facial stimuli by using the emotional facial expressions of negative and positive valence (i.e., anger and happiness) and presenting facial expressions with different facial action patterns over time, which were shown to elicit slightly different emotional impressions. Following previous meta-analyses (Diel et al., 2022; Mara et al., 2022), the uncanny valley effect is investigated by testing for a cubic function between ratings of esthetics and human likeness:

Hypothesis 1. A cubic function relates uncanniness to human likeness in upright facial expressions.

However, as we propose that the nonlinear relation between uncanniness and human likeness results from specialized processing of faces and facial expressions, this effect should not occur when the expressions are presented inverted:

Hypothesis 2. A linear function relates uncanniness to human likeness in inverted facial expressions.

However, if upright and inverted facial expressions produced the same uncanniness function, this would indicate a lack of inversion effect, suggesting that specialized processing plays little to no role in evaluating the esthetics of artificial entities.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-four Japanese volunteers participated in this study (31 females, 31 males, and two who preferred not to specify their gender; mean ± SD age, 30.65 ± 3.88 years). The required sample size was determined using an a priori power analysis using G*Power software ver. 3.1.9.2 (Faul et al., 2007). As an approximation of the present analysis using linear mixed-effects models containing seven dependent variables (i.e., the interaction model), a multiple linear regression model with seven dependent variables was analyzed. A power analysis for the coefficient evaluation (two-tailed) with the assumption of f2 of 0.15 (medium size effect), α level of 0.05, and power (1–β) of 0.80 showed that 55 participants were needed.

Because G*Power may be insufficient for power analysis of linear mixed models due to its inability to handle random effects, an additional, simulation-based power analysis was conducted using the simr R package (Green and MacLeod, 2016) and linear mixed model analyses using the lme4 package including random slopes (Bates et al., 2015). Power analysis was conducted using a pilot sample of n = 11 assuming the same coefficients, an α level of 0.05, and 100 simulation runs. For a power of 80%, for linear, quadratic, and cubic human likeness terms respectively, sample sizes of 7, 16, and 14 were necessary. At the n of 64, a power of 100% was reached. The second power analysis thus suggests that no larger sample size would be needed.

Participants were recruited through web advertisements distributed via CrowdWorks (Tokyo, Japan). After the procedures had been explained, all participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study, which the Ethics Committee of RIKEN approved. The experiment was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials

Actors

Android

The study used the android Nikola. Nikola’s 35 pneumatic actuators reproduce the facial actions required to express six basic human emotions (Sato et al., 2022). Their temporal resolution of milliseconds enables natural emotion expression. Android videos were created by filming Nikola’s frontal emotion expressions.

Human

Human videos were created using angry and happy expressions from the AIST Facial Expression Database (Fujimura and Umemura, 2018).

CG

CG videos were created using FACSGen (Roesch et al., 2011; Krumhuber et al., 2012).

Videos

For android and CG faces, the following face action units (AUs) were used for the expressions: angry: 4 (brow lowerer), 5 (upper lid raiser), 7 (lid tightener), 23 (lip tightener); happy: 6 (cheek raiser), 12 (lip corner puller). Asynchronous motion was created for android videos by delaying motion onset. Asynchronies were either absent (original video or synchronous motion), delayed (the movement of the upper right half of the face was delayed 250 ms and the upper left half 500 ms), or doubly delayed (the upper right half of the face was delayed 500 ms and the upper left half 1,000 ms). The lower half of the face started to move at the same time in each condition. Asynchronous motion was created for CG and human videos by delaying motion onset using Adobe Premiere.

All videos were edited to have the noses of each actor at the same height, to cut off at the neck (bottom), head (top), and ears (left and right), and to show a white background. All videos were 1.25 s long and depicted the onset of one out of two emotion expressions: angry and happy.

A total of 36 videos (3 actors, 3 asynchrony levels, 2 orientations, 2 emotions) were used. Screenshots of the android and CG expressions are depicted in Figure 1. The android and CG stimuli are available in the Supplementary material. AIST prohibits the distribution of human stimuli.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CG (computer-generated; top) and android (bottom) stimuli across emotion conditions. Baseline (neutral) expressions are to the right, followed by angry and happy expressions.

Stimulus validation

A stimulus validation pilot study was conducted to test whether the actors’ objective and subjective emotional expressions differed.

Objective expressions

For validation of objective expressions, facial movements of the base stimuli of angry and happy expressions for each actor were analyzed using OpenFace (version 2.2.0; Baltrusaitis et al., 2018). Face action units (AUs) characteristic of angry and happy emotion expressions were used as indicators.

Specifically, AU4 (brow lowerer) and AU12 (lip corner puller) values were used to indicate angry and happy expressions, respectively. The trajectories of both AUs are depicted in Figure 2. While Nikola’s AU12 trajectories began to increase earlier than the other actors, there were no strong deviations in AU intensity between the three actors, indicating that the intensity of AU expressions is analogous across actors.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The intensity of face action units AU4 and AU12 across actor types. Values were analyzed automatically using OpenFace. CG = computer-generated.

Subjective expressions

For subjective expressions, a questionnaire study has been conducted. Single-scale items of valence and arousal based on the bipolar valence-arousal modes were used to assess emotional expressions. Eleven participants were asked to rate the faces on the following scales ranging from 0 to 100: how angry the face is, how happy the face is, emotional arousal, and emotional valence. The study was conducted online. Results show no significant main effects of actor type on ratings on how happy (F(2.63) = 0.1, p = 0.93) or angry (F(2.63) = 0.3, p = 0.76) the faces were, neither on arousal (F(2.63) = 0.1, p = 0.95) or valence (F(2.63) = 0.1, p = 0.89) ratings.

Thus, for both emotions, indicators for both objective and subjective intensity of emotional expressions did not differ across actors.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted online. After providing informed consent, participants were linked to the experiment page. There, participants were shown each video in a randomized order. Participants had to rate each video on three scales used in a previous study (Diel et al., 2022): uncanny, strange, and humanlike. Specifically, participants were shown the terms and had to rate the video on uncanny/strange/humanlike scales ranging from 0 to 100. There was no time limit on rating the videos, which could be repeated at any time.

Statistical analysis

Linear mixed-effect models were used for data analysis. Models were constructed using uncanny ratings as the dependent variable. The main effect model included linear, quadratic, and cubic functions of human likeness and orientation as independent variables, and the interaction model included linear, quadratic, and cubic functions of human likeness, orientation, and interactions between each function of human likeness and orientation as independent variables. Random by-participant intercepts were used for each model. Random by-participant intercepts were added as per traditional repeated-measures analyses; in addition, our preliminary analysis for the interaction model indicated that model comparison using Akaike information criterion (AIC) preferred the model with only by-participant intercepts compared with that with by-participant intercepts and slopes (AIC = 20,280 vs. 20,294).

All statistical analyses were performed using the statistics and machine learning toolbox in MATLAB 2020a (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States). The relations between uncanny and humanlike ratings were analyzed according to the study’s purpose. The data, stimuli (except human videos), and analysis are available at https://osf.io/9cmhp.

Results

All scales were transformed using z-standardization. Z-transformed strange and uncanny items were combined into an uncanniness index by calculating trial-based averages with internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of α = 0.9. The relations between uncanny and humanlike ratings are plotted in Figure 3. Average uncanniness and human likeness values across stimuli are depicted in Figure 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatter plots and cubic regression lines between humanlike and uncanny ratings for upright and inverted presented faces. Standardized scores are shown to indicate consistent patterns across participants. Points represent raw data points.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Average uncanniness ratings for each stimulus along the human likeness axis, divided by orientation. Color and point shape indicate agent and emotion type. Error bars indicate standard errors. CG = computer-generated face created via FACSGen.

Model comparison using AIC supported the interaction model (main effect vs. interaction: 20,282 vs. 20,280). Evaluation of beta estimates using Satterthwaite’s approximation for the interaction model revealed that cubic human likeness × orientation was significant (F(1, 2236.1)= 7.0, p = 0.008). In addition, linear human likeness (F(1, 2247.4) = 170.9, p < 0.001) and the interaction between linear human likeness and orientation (F(1, 2238.8) = 5.7, p = 0.017) were significant.

Follow-up analyses were conducted for each orientation condition using the simple main model, including linear, quadratic, and cubic functions of human likeness as independent variables. For upright faces, linear (F(1, 1090.4) = 255.2, p < 0.001) and cubic (F(1, 1100.0) = 4.6, p = 0.033) function of human likeness were significant. For inverted faces, only linear human likeness was significant (F(1, 1114.2) = 162.4, p < 0.001); the cubic function did not reach significance (F(1, 1124.1) = 0.6, p = 0.459).

In addition, AIC-based model comparisons between cubic and linear models for each orientation supported the cubic model for the upright condition (linear vs. cubic: 10,110 vs. 10,109) and the linear model for the inverted condition (linear vs. cubic: 10,286 vs. 10,290). Thus, hypotheses 1 and 2 were supported.

To investigate the modulatory effects of agent (human, android, and CG) and emotion (anger and happiness), we constructed a linear mixed-effect model adding these effects to the interaction model. Specifically, independent variables included human likeness functions (linear, quadratic, and cubic), orientation, agent, and emotion, and their two-, three-, and four-way interactions. The results showed that cubic human likeness x orientation interaction was significant (F(1, 2236.1) = 4.5, p = 0.034) and that no significant higher-order interactions were related to this two-way interaction. However, cubic human likeness × orientation × agent interaction reached a non-significant trend (cubic human likeness × orientation × agent: F(2, 2237.8) = 2.4, p = 0.087; cubic human likeness × orientation × emotion: F(1, 2236.7) < 0.1, p = 0.853; cubic human likeness × orientation × agent × emotion: F(2, 2236.4) = 0.6, p = 0.546). The results suggest that the effects of agent and emotion are not evident in the above results.

In summary, a cubic (increasing sigmoid) relation between uncanniness and human likeness was observed for upright, but not inverted, expressions. The results support the view that (1) relations between human likeness and uncanniness ratings for human and humanlike agents’ expressions are cubic and that (2) these relations result from configural processing.

Discussion

The present research investigated the effect of inversion, a proxy of configural processing in faces and facial expressions, on the uncanniness of different agents’ facial expressions across human likeness. Differences between upright and inverted expressions were found. Specifically, a cubic (increasing sigmoid) function of human likeness best explained the uncanniness of facial expressions, consistent with previous research (Diel et al., 2022; Mara et al., 2022). Meanwhile, only a linear function of human likeness was significant for inverted facial expressions. Thus, a characteristic cubic, in this case, a sigmoid function of human likeness on esthetic appeal, is only present when the configural processing of facial expressions remains intact. This suggests that the typical observations on the relation between artificial agents’ esthetic ratings and human likeness depend on specialized processing mechanisms.

The present study did not find a proper uncanny valley because uncanniness ratings increased monotonically (Mori et al., 2012). A limited stimulus range has been suggested to be one cause of failing to find an uncanny valley function (Diel et al., 2022): uncanny valley functions are observed when stimuli range from less realistic (mechanically robotic or cartoon) to realistic human faces (Mathur et al., 2020). CG and android faces used here may have been too realistic to plot a complete uncanny valley function. Similarly, previous research observed linear functions using only a limited stimulus range lacking less realistic stimuli (Kätsyri et al., 2019).Selecting additional less humanlike robot stimuli may have led to a proper uncanny valley. However, manipulation of emotion expressions is difficult in robot faces, which would thus not have been suitable for this study. Similarly, although sigmoidal relations between human likeness and likability were found in a previous meta-analysis (Mara et al., 2022), these were found when including studies lacking full human stimuli. For research that includes a broader range of stimuli varying on human likeness, cubic functions akin to Mori et al.’s (2012) uncanny valley are expected (Diel et al., 2022). Thus, the exact cause of the sigmoidal uncanniness function remains unclear. MacDorman and Chattopadhyay (2016) proposed that this nonlinear relation may result from a higher sensitivity to deviations in more familiar face categories, which Kätsyri et al. (2019) identified with the “uncanny slope” found in their results. Consistently, as inversion reduces this sensitivity (Diel and Lewis, 2022), an “uncanny slope” was not found for inverted expressions in the present study, as participants were less sensitive to deviations in inverted stimuli.

Furthermore, logistic patterns akin to two levels connected by an increasing slope are also found in categorization tasks plotted against human likeness (Looser and Wheatley, 2010; Cheetham et al., 2011; MacDorman and Chattopadhyay, 2016). Thus, categorization as human or nonhuman may determine affect ratings, which inversion may influence.

Nevertheless, the results show that configural processing moderates the effect of human likeness on uncanniness. Specialized processing may act as a gateway to enhanced detection of errors or deviations, which may lead to negative evaluations (Chattopadhyay and MacDorman, 2016; Diel and Lewis, 2022). Accordingly, ratings of facial esthetics are more sensitive when faces are presented upright instead of inverted (Bäuml, 1994; Santos and Young, 2008; Leder et al., 2017). The present results show for the first time that configural processing’s role in esthetics extends beyond facial structure to include dynamic facial expressions. Furthermore, the results indicate specialized processing plays a role in evaluating artificial entities (e.g., Diel and MacDorman, 2021) and their facial expressions.

Social robots with the ability to emulate human emotion expressions and affect have the potential to provide emotional support and experiences of social bonding and connectedness (Kirby et al., 2010; Sato et al., 2022). The processing of artificial entities’ emotional expressions may fall under similar scrutiny as evaluations of their physical appearance, leading to perceptions of uncanniness. Configural processing may sensitize the uncanniness of artificial entities expressing emotions with their face, in line with previous research showing analogous effects with faces (Diel and Lewis, 2022). As inversion effects increase with an entity’s level of realism (Crookes et al., 2015), designing social robots in a highly realistic or humanlike manner may increase the chance that imperfections in their appearance or face motion are detected and negatively evaluated. Instead, the design of social robots may profit from less realistic, stylistic, or cartoon-like designs that do not recruit specialized processing mechanisms sensitized to uncanniness. Alternatively, care can be taken for the design of realistic artificial entities, like social robots or CG animations, not only in their appearance but also in the temporal aspects of facial expressions.

In this study, participants were allowed to watch the stimulus videos repeatedly. While this ensures more accurate emotion expression processing, repeated exposure may have decreased uncanniness ratings through habituation. In addition, human stimuli in this study possessed human hair while the android and CG stimuli did not, which could confound human likeness and uncanniness measures. Future research may replicate results with shorter video exposure and controlled stimulus appearance. In addition, future research may investigate a broader range of actors varying in human likeness, including low-realism actors, to attempt to replicate a full uncanny valley function. Finally, perceptual specialization could be measured more directly in future research, for example, by adding an inversion recognition task.

Conclusion

The statistical relation between human likeness and esthetic appeal is typically described in a polynomial manner (e.g., the uncanny valley). One reason for this pattern may be a higher sensitivity to deviations or errors in specialized categories like faces, bodies, or facial expressions. The present study found such a polynomial pattern in dynamic expressions of human and humanlike agents. However, this statistical pattern reverted to a linear relation when stimulus inversion disrupted specialized processing. Thus, specialized processing seems to drive changes in esthetic appeal across the human likeness dimension. Consequently, care must be taken in designing close to humanlike artificial entities, as even subtle errors or deviations can cause uncanniness.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/9cmhp/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of RIKEN, RIKEN. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AD contributed to conceptualization, investigation, formal analysis, data curation, drafting and revising the manuscript, and visualization. WS contributed to conceptualization, formal analysis, revising the manuscript, and visualization. C-TH contributed to validation and revising the manuscript. TM contributed to resources, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1222279/full#supplementary-material

References

Adams, A., and Robinson, P. N. (2011). An android head for social-emotional intervention for children with autism Spectrum conditions. Lect. Notes Comput. Sci 6975, 183–190. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-24571-8_19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambadar, Z., Schooler, J. W., and Cohn, J. F. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic face: the importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 16, 403–410. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltrusaitis, T., Zadeh, A., Lim, Y. C., and Morency, L-P. (2018). OpenFace 2.0: face behavior analysis toolkit. 13th IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition (FG 2018), 59–66.

Google Scholar

Bartneck, C., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., and Hagita, N. (2009). My robotic Doppelgänger — a critical look at the Uncanny Valley. Robot Hum. Interact. Commun. doi: 10.1109/roman.2009.5326351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bäuml, K. T. (1994). Upright versus upside-down faces: how Interface attractiveness varies with orientation. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 56, 163–172. doi: 10.3758/bf03213895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bould, E., and Morris, N. (2008). Role of motion signals in recognizing subtle facial expressions of emotion. Br. J. Psychol. 99, 167–189. doi: 10.1348/000712607X206702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbon, C., and Leder, H. (2006). When faces are heads: view-dependent recognition of faces altered relationally or componentially. Swiss J. Psychol. 65, 245–252. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.65.4.245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chattopadhyay, D., and MacDorman, K. F. (2016). Familiar faces rendered strange: why inconsistent realism drives characters into the Uncanny Valley. J. Vis. 16:7. doi: 10.1167/16.11.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheetham, M., Suter, P., and Jäncke, L. (2011). The human likeness dimension of the "Uncanny Valley hypothesis": behavioral and functional MRI findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:126. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheetham, M., Suter, P., and Jancke, L. (2014). Perceptual discrimination difficulty and familiarity in the Uncanny Valley: more like a “Happy Valley”. Front. Psychol. 5:1219. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crookes, K., Ewing, L., Gildenhuys, J. D., Kloth, N., Hayward, W. G., Oxner, M., et al. (2015). How well do Computer-generated faces tap face expertise? PLoS One 10:e0141353. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diel, A., and Lewis, M. (2022). Familiarity, orientation, and realism increase face uncanniness by sensitizing to facial distortions. J. Vis. 22:14. doi: 10.1167/jov.22.4.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diel, A., and MacDorman, K. F. (2021). Creepy cats and strange high houses: support for Configural processing in testing predictions of nine Uncanny Valley theories. J. Vis. 21:1. doi: 10.1167/jov.21.4.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diel, A., Weigelt, S., and MacDorman, K. F. (2022). A meta-analysis of the Uncanny Valley’s independent and dependent variables. ACM Trans. Hum. Rob. Interact. 11, 1–33. doi: 10.1145/3470742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattal, C., Cossin, I., Pain, F., Haize, E., Marissael, C., Schmutz, S., et al. (2020). Perspectives on usability and accessibility of an autonomous humanoid robot living with elderly people. Disabil. Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 17, 418–430. doi: 10.1080/17483107.2020.1786732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujimura, T., and Umemura, H. (2018). Development and validation of a facial expression database based on the dimensional and categorical model of emotions. Cognit. Emot. 32, 1663–1670. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1419936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, P., and MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 493–498. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C.-C., and MacDorman, K. F. (2017). Measuring the Uncanny Valley effect. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 9, 129–139. doi: 10.1007/s12369-016-0380-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C., Macdorman, K. F., and Pramono, Z. A. (2008). Human emotion and the Uncanny Valley: a GLM, MDS, and Isomap analysis of robot video ratings. 2008 3rd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), 169–176.

Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N. (2000). Domain specificity in face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 759–763. doi: 10.1038/77664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., and Moscovitch, M. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of face processing: an introduction. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 1–11. doi: 10.1080/026432900380454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kätsyri, J., de Gelder, B., and Takala, T. (2019). Virtual faces evoke only a weak Uncanny Valley effect: an empirical investigation with controlled virtual face images. Perception 48, 968–991. doi: 10.1177/0301006619869134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, R., Forlizzi, J., and Simmons, R. G. (2010). Affective social robots. Robot. Auton. Syst. 58, 322–332. doi: 10.1016/j.robot.2009.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E. G., Tamarit, L., Roesch, E. B., and Scherer, K. R. (2012). FACSGen 2.0 animation software: generating three-dimensional FACS-valid facial expressions for emotion research. Emotion 12, 351–363. doi: 10.1037/a0026632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumazaki, H., Warren, Z., Muramatsu, T., Yoshikawa, Y., Matsumoto, Y., Miyao, M., et al. (2017). A pilot study for robot appearance preferences among high-functioning individuals with autism Spectrum disorder: implications for therapeutic use. PLoS One 12:e0186581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leder, H., Goller, J., Forster, M. J., Schlageter, L., and Paul, M. J. (2017). Face inversion increases attractiveness. Acta Psychol. 178, 25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Looser, C. E., and Wheatley, T. (2010). The tipping point of Animacy: how, when, and where we perceive life in a face. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1854–1862. doi: 10.1177/0956797610388044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F., and Chattopadhyay, D. (2016). Reducing consistency in human realism increases the Uncanny Valley effect; increasing category uncertainty does not. Cognition 146, 190–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F., and Entezari, S. O. (2015). Individual differences predict sensitivity to the Uncanny Valley. Interact. Stud. Soc. Behav. Commun. Biol. Artif. Syst. 16, 141–172. doi: 10.1075/is.16.2.01mac

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F., Green, R. C., Ho, C., and Koch, C. T. (2009). Too real for comfort? Uncanny responses to computer generated faces. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25, 695–710. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangan, B. (2015). The Uncanny Valley as fringe experience. Interact. Stud. 16, 193–199. doi: 10.1075/is.16.2.05man

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mara, M., Appel, M., and Gnambs, T. (2022). Humanlike robots and the Uncanny Valley. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 230, 33–46. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, M. B., Reichling, D. B., Lunardini, F., Geminiani, A., Antonietti, A., Ruijten, P. A. M., et al. (2020). Uncanny but not confusing: multisite study of perceptual category confusion in the uncanny valley. Comput. Hum. Behav. 103, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, M., MacDorman, K. F., and Kageki, N. (2012). The Uncanny Valley [from the field]. IEEE Robot. Automat. Mag. 19, 98–100. doi: 10.1109/mra.2012.2192811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, X., and Hamilton, A. F. C. (2018). Why and how to use virtual reality to study human social interaction: the challenges of exploring a new research landscape. Br. J. Psychol. 109, 395–417. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuten, A., Van Dam, M., and Naber, M. (2018). Pupillary responses to robotic and human emotions: the Uncanny Valley and media equation confirmed. Front. Psychol. 9:774. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roesch, E. B., Tamarit, L., Reveret, L., Grandjean, D. M., Sander, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2011). FACSGen: a tool to synthesize emotional facial expressions through systematic manipulation of facial action units. J. Nonverbal Behav. 35, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s10919-010-0095-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos, I., and Young, A. J. (2008). Effects of inversion and negation on social inferences from faces. Perception 37, 1061–1078. doi: 10.1068/p5278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Namba, S., Yang, D., Nishida, S., Ishi, C. T., and Minato, T. (2022). An android for emotional interaction: spatiotemporal validation of its facial expressions. Front. Psychol. 12:800657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.800657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P., Chaminade, T., Ishiguro, H., Driver, J., and Frith, C. (2012). The thing that should not be: predictive coding and the Uncanny Valley in perceiving human and humanoid robot actions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 413–422. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smarr, C., Mitzner, T. L., Beer, J. M., Prakash, A., Chen, T. Y., Kemp, C. C., et al. (2014). Domestic robots for older adults: attitudes, preferences, and potential. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 6, 229–247. doi: 10.1007/s12369-013-0220-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, J. W., and Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. The. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A Hum. Exp. Psychol. 46, 225–245. doi: 10.1080/14640749308401045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobin, A., Favelle, S., and Palermo, R. (2016). Dynamic facial expressions are processed holistically, but not more holistically than static facial expressions. Cognit. Emot. 30, 1208–1221. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1049936

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Zitzewitz, J., Boesch, P. M., Wolf, P., and Riener, R. (2013). Quantifying the Human Likeness of a Humanoid Robot. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 5, 263–276. doi: 10.1007/s12369-012-0177-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Lilienfeld, S. O., and Rochat, P. (2015). The uncanny valley: Existence and explanations. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 19, 393–407. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamada, Y., Kawabe, T., and Ihaya, K. (2013). Categorization difficulty is associated with negative evaluation in the "uncanny valley" phenomenon. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 55, 20–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00538.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Li, S., Zhang, J. Y., Du, F., Qi, Y., and Liu, X. (2020). “A Literature Review of the Research on the Uncanny Valley,” in Cross-Cultural Design. User Experience of Products, Services, and Intelligent Environments. HCII 2020. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 12192. ed. P. L. Rau (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Keywords: configural processing, dynamic facial expression, emotion expression, inversion effect, uncanny valley

Citation: Diel A, Sato W, Hsu C-T and Minato T (2023) The inversion effect on the cubic humanness-uncanniness relation in humanlike agents. Front. Psychol. 14:1222279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1222279

Received: 14 May 2023; Accepted: 11 August 2023;
Published: 29 August 2023.

Edited by:

Karl F. MacDorman, Indiana University, Purdue University Indianapolis, United States

Reviewed by:

Marnix Naber, Utrecht University, Netherlands
Anna Flavia Di Natale, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Diel, Sato, Hsu and Minato. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexander Diel, diela@cardiff.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.