Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 February 2024
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Evolving Online Modalities: How Uses and Abuses of Text, Image and Video-based Communication Impact Interpersonal Interactions View all 5 articles

Exploring discontinuous intentions of social media users: a cognition-affect-conation perspective

Sara Qaisar
Sara Qaisar1*Anum Nawaz KianiAnum Nawaz Kiani2Afsheen JalilAfsheen Jalil1
  • 1International Islamic University, Islamabad, Pakistan
  • 2Rawalpindi Women University, Rawalpindi, Pakistan

Introduction: Drawing on the cognition-affect-conation (C-A-C) framework, this study investigates how perceived information and social and system feature overload induce depression and anxiety, which leads to affect discontinuous intentions of the social media users.

Methods: The data collected from 570 social networking site users in China are analyzed through structural equation modeling (SEM).

Results and Discussion: The findings show that perceived information overload, perceived social overload, and perceived system feature overload directly affect depression and anxiety among social networking site users, which directly leads to discontinuous intentions. This study fulfills the identified need for an in-depth investigation of discontinuous behavior in social networking sites. The findings provide social networking site providers with guidelines on how to actively manage social networking site user’s behavior to reduce the effects of negative emotions on social networking sites.

1 Introduction

Social networking sites (SNSs) are online service providers that aim to build social relationships among users who share common interests and activities (Luqman et al., 2017). SNSs provide a platform with good service quality, for example, by adding new features and functions that allow users to enhance their social lives and improve their social interactions with communication technology (Zhang et al., 2016). SNSs have enriched sources of information and the primary function of SNS is to share information among SNS users (Bright et al., 2015). Users invest a considerable amount of time in SNSs to expect improvement in their social relations and productivity with respect to communication technology (Choi and Lim, 2016). However, recent research has shown that users become depressed and their productivity is affected by the time spent using SNSs (Sagioglou and Greitemeyer, 2014). Many researchers have started investigating the negative side of SNSs usage and have reported that compulsive use of SNSs can result in negative emotional (i.e., depression and anxiety) and behavioral (i.e., discontinuous) consequences (Luqman et al., 2017; Dhir et al., 2018).

Many scholars have focused on determining the factors affecting the discontinuous intentions of SNS users. For example, Maier et al. (2015a) found that discontinuous behavior is a strategy to cope with stress and exhaustion. Discontinuous intentions refer to the behavioral intentions of users to reduce SNS usage, temporarily or permanently quit or switch to other SNSs (Ravindran et al., 2014; Maier et al., 2015b). Previous studies have highlighted the importance of stressful experiences and found that information, social, and system feature overload-induced psychological strains such as technostress, dissatisfaction, regret, exhaustion, and fatigue, which lead to user discontinuous intentions (Luqman et al., 2017; Cao and Sun, 2018; Nawaz et al., 2018; Cao et al., 2019; Rahmanyanti and Yasa, 2019).

Although limited studies have examined discontinuous behavior among social media users, several gaps remain for exploring discontinuous behavior in SNSs. First, extant studies have found that cognitive factors (i.e., perceived information and social and system feature overload) caused psychological strains (Choi and Lim, 2016; Lee et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016), thus the effect of these cognitive factors on user’s affective factors (i.e., depression and anxiety) remains unclear. Second, previous studies have found a direct relationship between compulsive SNSs usage (i.e., time spent on SNS and SNS checking frequency) with depression and anxiety (Dhir et al., 2018; Yoon et al., 2019; Chu et al., 2021) but the effect of user’s affective factors (i.e., depression and anxiety) on discontinuous intentions must be explored thoroughly. Third, studies have found the effect of information, social, and system feature overload on discontinuous intentions by drawing on stressor–strain–outcome (SSO) (Zhang et al., 2016; Cao and Sun, 2018; Qaisar et al., 2021) and stimuli–organism–response (SOR) framework (Luqman et al., 2017), thus these effects by drawing on cognition-affect-conation (C-A-C) framework remains unknown.

To fill the research gap, the current study explores the mechanism underlying the influence of cognitive and affective factors among SNS users on their intention to avoid using SNSs. We develop our research model based on the cognition-affect-conation (C-A-C) framework. Specifically, we assume that the perceived information, social, and system feature overload on SNSs induce negative emotions, i.e., depression and anxiety, thereby influencing SNS users to avoid using SNS and allowing them to adopt a strategy to exit from the situation.

This study exhibits three important contributions. First, we extend the applicability of the cognition-affect-conation (C-A-C) framework, which is an effective approach for examining customer loyalty in marketing, to the domain of the behavior of SNS users. Second, we extend previous studies that address discontinuous intentions from the perspective of overloads (e.g., information, social, and system features) and negative psychological factors (e.g., exhaustion, fatigue, dissatisfaction, and regret). Finally, this study strengthens the existing literature on emotions in information systems usage behavior research by explaining the vital role of negative emotions (i.e., depression and anxiety) among SNS users on their discontinuous intentions.

2 Literature review and hypothesis development

2.1 Cognition-affect-conation framework

The cognition-affect-conation model provides three basic stages of the mind, namely, cognitive (thinking or knowing), affective (emotions or feelings), and conative (doing or action). According to Huitt and Cain (2005), cognition refers to the perceptions, knowledge, and information that are acquired by the combination of direct experience. Affect refers to the emotions or feelings toward such perceptions and conation refers to the specific action or behavior of users.

Various studies have proposed and empirically confirmed theoretical models based on the C-A-C framework. For example, Hill et al. (1977) reveal that a user’s knowledge, beliefs, and opinions about an object generate favorable and unfavorable feelings toward that object, which demonstrate the user’s behavioral intentions or actions in the presence of a given object. The C-A-C framework is used in information system studies to find the user’s behavioral response, such as Davis (1989) described two cognitive beliefs (perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use) that affect their attitude (i.e., affection) and influence their intentions toward IT use (i.e., conation). Similarly, Huang et al. (2019) found that a person’s cognitive beliefs affect a user’s attitude, which directly enhances their intentions toward mobile health app usage. Dai et al. (2020) found that perceived information overload directly affects users’ emotions (frustration, fatigue, and dissatisfaction), thereby influencing their information avoidance intentions.

In the current study, we used the C-A-C framework as a theoretical framework to highlight the mechanism underlying the discontinuous intentions among SNS users. In the previous studies, the C-A-C model was used to analyze individual perception in the context of online services (Harris and Goode, 2004; Zhao et al., 2012; Lin, 2014). These studies verified the effectiveness of this model by identifying the sequential causal relationships among cognition, affect, and conation. Few studies have used other classic frameworks that are similar to C-A-C framework, such as stressor–strain–outcome (SSO) model (Lee et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016; Cao et al., 2018; Dhir et al., 2018; Nawaz et al., 2018; Fu et al., 2020; Qaisar et al., 2022), stimuli–organism–response (SOR) model (Luqman et al., 2017; Cao and Sun, 2018), and stress and coping theory to understand the influence of stress-related situations on the affective and behavioral outcomes of the users in the context of IT use. The purpose of using the C-A-C framework is that it aligns with the main objective of our research study, that is, to examine how the perceived information, social, and system feature overload of SNS users affects their affective (depression and anxiety) and conative (discontinuous intentions) responses.

2.2 Perceived information overload and depression

Social media improves individual social capital and subjective wellbeing, but the time spent on social media and the usage frequency of social media can have negative consequences once it exceeds from optimal level (Karr-Wisniewski and Lu, 2010). According to Moore (2000), overload is a key element in promoting negative consequences. Previous studies have described information overload occurs when users receive excessive information that affects their cognitive ability to process the information (Farhoomand and Drury, 2002; Eppler and Mengis, 2004; Carver and Turoff, 2007). That is, such overload transpires when the processing capabilities of users are exceeded by the information processing requirement. Previous research on social load theory implies that users experience cognitive limitation when their accessibility increases to a certain limit (Lee et al., 2016). Moreover, information overload refers to the situation when a user fails to process additional information due to its size and volume (Hoq, 2014). Zhang et al. (2016) described that information overload indicates the perceived amount of information that exceeds the processing capacity of the user. Similarly, the growing number of users on social media produces an excessive volume of information such as information about their friends and family, their personal lives, news, events, expertise, and rumors. These types of information attract intense attention from users, thereby inhibiting their cognitive ability with excessive information (Lee et al., 2016; Cao and Sun, 2018). Previous studies show that SNS users experience perceived information overload may result in negative behavioral consequences (Lee et al., 2016; Gao et al., 2018). Few studies confirmed that information overload exhibits a direct relationship with discontinuous usage intention (Dai et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2020). Moreover, information overload also induced many psychological and emotional consequences, such as stress, exhaustion, regret, dissatisfaction, and SNS fatigue (Choi and Lim, 2016; Cao et al., 2018; Nawaz et al., 2018). Lee et al. (2016) found a direct relationship between information overload and SNS fatigue. Moreover, Dhir et al. (2018) reported that SNS fatigue is significantly related to depression. “Depression is an emotional state wherein pleasurable feelings are either diminished or disappeared.” Previous studies have found that depression has various psychological symptoms such as depressive moods and distress. Extant research studies found interference of depression in daily routine activities (i.e., sleep disorder, eating, loss of concentration, and fatigue) (Sapolsky, 2004; Bianchi et al., 2014). Few studies investigated depression in the context of social media and found that usage intensity and online communication caused the consequences of depression (Morrison and Gore, 2010; Cotten et al., 2013, 2014). Hussain et al. (2017), found that extreme smartphone social application usage results in high levels of depressive symptoms. Therefore, the current study expected that the perceived information overload of social media users directly influences their negative emotions (e.g., depression). Thus, the proposed hypothesis is as follows:

H1(a). Perceived information overload has a positive relationship with depression.

2.3 Perceived information overload and anxiety

Anxiety is defined as a pervasive state of mind, which is concerned with difficult situations or threats (Bowers, 1986). Many scholars have found that anxious people believe that their ability to engage in certain tasks is detrimental (Madan et al., 2014). Previous studies have investigated the prevalence of anxiety among social media users. For example, Lepp et al. (2015) found that compulsive use of mobile social media applications exerts a direct impact on the anxious state of users. Social media requires deep involvement and concentration of users to participate in SNS activities (Can and Kaya, 2016). Excessive information on SNSs affects users’ psychological wellbeing (Chen and Li, 2017). Many studies have reported that information overload causes psychological distress, exhaustion, and emotional malfunctions (Hoq, 2014; Luqman et al., 2017; Swar et al., 2017). Psychology literature has suggested that anxious people are more likely to suffer from multiple disorders such as engaging in false judgment and unsystematic information processing (Kuss and Griffiths, 2011; Vermani et al., 2011; Stein and Sareen, 2015). Therefore, the current study expected that the perceived information overload of SNS users induces anxiety. Users with high perceived information overload are more likely to suffer from anxiety. Thus, the proposed hypothesis is as follows:

H1(b). Perceived information overload has a positive relationship with anxiety.

2.4 Perceived social overload and depression

The number of social media relationships increases with the expansion of social media. Users become exhausted as they perceived to provide too much social support to their online friends. This phenomenon is known as social overload (Maier et al., 2015b). Users have to build and enhance their social contacts and interactions, which requires deep attention and more time to invest in maintaining social relationships, which leads to mental and psychological distress (McCarthy and Saegert, 1978). When social media requests are far more than the processing capability of users, the feeling of losing control over the social situation causes negative emotions. LaRose and Eastin (2004) defined that when the demands of maintaining and updating social media have detrimental effects on user’s lives. Previous studies have reported that social overload caused psychological distress due to a large number of social demands (Choi and Lim, 2016; Rahmanyanti and Yasa, 2019). According to Dunbar et al. (2018), there is a cognitive limit to the number of individuals with whom one can maintain stable relationships, which is approximately 150 users. Moreover, studies on Facebook reveal that a number of friends greater than Dunbar number decrease a user’s psychological wellbeing (Koc and Gulyagci, 2013; Bright et al., 2015; Li et al., 2015). Previous studies have revealed that social overload has a direct relationship with exhaustion, regret, and dissatisfaction (Maier et al., 2015b; Cao and Sun, 2018; Cao et al., 2019; Chu et al., 2021). Excessive social demands on SNSs may interrupt users’ attention and concentration from their daily work routine and evoke feelings of depression (Frison and Eggermont, 2015). Therefore, the current study expected that social overload exerts a direct impact on depression. Thus, we proposed the following hypothesis:

H2(a): Perceived social overload has a positive relationship with depression.

2.5 Perceived social overload and anxiety

Previous research studies have found that perceived social overload usually involves deficient self-reaction to social demands and compulsive use of SNS, which contribute to negative affect such as stress (Lee et al., 2016; Cao et al., 2019; Fu et al., 2020; Qaisar et al., 2021). Moreover, social overload easily leads to low user satisfaction level and leads to social withdrawal (Lo et al., 2018). Maier et al. (2015b) found that users get tired of receiving too many virtual requests that limit their cognitive capacity to process and lead to influence negative emotions. Recently, a study found that compulsive use of SNS causes fatigue, which ultimately leads to anxiety (Dhir et al., 2018). Therefore, the current study expected that users with perceived social overload are more likely to be affected with anxiety.

H2(b). Perceived social overload has a positive relationship with anxiety.

2.6 Perceived system feature overload and depression

System feature overload is defined as a situation when the technology is too complex to use by adding new features that increase its complexity (Karr-Wisniewski and Lu, 2010). Zhang et al. (2016) found that users frequently received system updates and modifications in SNSs. Too many features on SNS attract users to some extent but lead to feelings of strain such as fatigue and dissatisfaction. According to cognitive fit theory, unlimited and unnecessary features distract users’ attention and increase cognitive load, which leads to affect individual performance (Oviatt, 2006). The ultimate demands of SNS features require full attention to perceived usefulness, which, in turn, leads to psychological distress and exhaustion (Hsi and Potts, 2000; Thompson et al., 2005). The negative outcomes of system feature overload are due to the complexity of features, which requires more time to learn and decreases users’ perception of ease of use, which, in turn, leads to depression (Mick and Fournier, 1998). In this study, the researcher expected that perceived system feature overload exerts a direct impact on negative emotion (e.g., depression). Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed.

H3(a). Perceived system feature overload has a positive relationship with depression.

2.7 Perceived system feature overload and anxiety

In the SNSs environment, new features and updates influence SNS users to a certain point but afterward decline. However, frequent changes occur in system features that are highly complex for users and could have negative consequences. When SNS users perceive the costs of learning and using the system features, they may get tired of SNS usage and feel fatigued (Zhang et al., 2016). Recent studies have linked fatigue and a state of anxiety (Dhir et al., 2018). Scholars have found that in users experiencing fatigue, cognitive capabilities decline, which inadequately regulates their mood and concentration. Particularly, researchers found that users experiencing fatigue and exhaustion are more likely to experience anxiety (Nawaz et al., 2018; Xie et al., 2018). Moreover, system feature overload limits the cognitive limit of users with too many features and functions, which increase negative emotions such as stress and anxiety (Lang, 2000; Li et al., 2014). Therefore, the current study expected that perceived system feature overloads influence negative emotions (e.g., anxiety). Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed.

H3(b). Perceived system feature overload has a positive relationship with anxiety.

2.8 Discontinuous intentions and depression

Ravindran et al. (2014) defined three types of discontinuous intentions, namely, short breaks, control activities, and suspended behavior. Thus, in the SNS context, discontinuous intention is defined as an individual intention to decrease SNS use intensity, stop SNS, or switch to other alternative SNSs. Previous studies on SNSs have identified discontinuous intentions in stressful situations and users adopt this coping strategy to exit from the situation to restore their emotional stability (Maier et al., 2015a; Turel, 2015; Turel and Qahri-Saremi, 2016). Scholars have found that users who feel stressed will adopt behavioral coping strategies to exit from the situation (Turel and Serenko, 2012; Maier et al., 2015a; Tarafdar et al., 2019). Previous studies in psychology have demonstrated that information overload, system feature overload, and social overload induce psychological consequences that influence the discontinuous intentions of the users (Lee et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016; Cao and Sun, 2018; Dhir et al., 2018; Nawaz et al., 2018; Qaisar et al., 2022). Scholars argue that users with psychological, emotional, and behavioral consequences can experience depression (Sapolsky, 2004; Bashir and Bhat, 2017). Recent studies have found that intensive engagement with social media results in depression (Lin et al., 2016; Hussain et al., 2017). Moreover, depression can involve users in interpersonal and physiological difficulties (Scherr and Brunet, 2017). Moreover, many scholars have found that psychological and emotional consequences, such as exhaustion, regret, fatigue, and dissatisfaction, exhibit a positive effect on user’s discontinuous intentions (Luqman et al., 2017; Cao and Sun, 2018; Rahmanyanti and Yasa, 2019; Dai et al., 2020). Therefore, the current study expected that depression influences the discontinuous intentions of SNS users. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed.

H4. Depression has a positive relationship with discontinuous intentions.

2.9 Discontinuous intentions and anxiety

Zhang et al. (2016) used the SSO framework to examine discontinuous intentions with perceived system features, information, and social overload as stressors that induce strain in terms of social network fatigue and dissatisfaction. Similarly, Cao and Sun (2018) described that information, communication, and social overload cause exhaustion and regret and ultimately affect the discontinuous intentions of users. Maier et al. (2015a) proposed that SNS stress creators and SNS exhaustion cause discontinuous intention, while switching stress creators and switching exhaustion reduce the intentions. Previous studies have investigated the antecedents and outcomes of anxiety such as psychological illness, emotional malfunctions, distress, and exhaustion (Kuss and Griffiths, 2011; Vermani et al., 2011; Baldwin et al., 2014). Users with high levels of exhaustion and fatigue are more likely to exit from the situation (Fu et al., 2020). Previous studies have revealed that SNS fatigue induced through compulsive use of SNSs exerts a direct impact on anxiety (Dhir et al., 2018). Lepp et al. (2014) found that compulsive mobile SNS users are more likely to suffer from anxious states as compared to non-compulsive users. Excessive social demand on SNSs reduces user capabilities, thereby triggering anxiety, and fatigue (Ravindran et al., 2014; Bright et al., 2015). Therefore, the current study expected that anxiety exerts a direct impact on users’ discontinuous intentions (Figure 1). Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research model.

H5. Anxiety has a positive relationship with discontinuous intentions.

3 Methodology

3.1 Sample and data collection

The current research study analyses the mechanism of user’s intentions toward social media. Thus, the study context is confined to social media and the participants are social media users. Before collecting the data through the survey, the Back-translation method was used because the original questionnaire items were in English and translated into the Chinese language through Chinese translators for data collection; then, the questions were translated back into the English language for further analysis (Brislin, 1970). The instruments used in this study are well-established and underwent a translation process. Therefore, we conducted a pilot test and invited 50 volunteer students to fill out the questionnaire. The results showed that the measurement items were adequate for further implementation. Finally, the questionnaire was distributed online by sharing a link to the questionnaire among university students’ social groups (WeChat, Weibo, and QQ) as well as by sending invitations to students via university email. The targeted sample was based on SNS users because SNSs use in China has increased rapidly. Thus, SNS users are an adequate source of data for our study. All participants were assured that their data would remain confidential and that it was collected for research purposes only. A convenience sampling technique was used to collect data. The sample size for the unknown population was calculated through the criteria defined by Godden (2004); the recommended sample size was 384. A total of 596 survey responses were collected. After the elimination of outliers and incomplete responses, 570 responses were selected for further analysis.

3.2 Measures

Our study constructs were adapted from previous studies to test the proposed research model. All items were evaluated using a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree. Furthermore, sociodemographic data such as gender, age, frequency of use, and experience were also measured. The study measures are mentioned in Appendix A.

3.2.1 Perceived information overload

The scale of perceived information overload was adapted from Zhang et al. (2016) and Karr-Wisniewski and Lu (2010). The four items of perceived information overload reflect the excessive amount of information available on SNSs. The Cronbach’s alpha (CA) value of the construct used in this study is 0.78.

3.2.2 Perceived social overload

The scale of perceived social overload was adapted from Maier et al. (2015b). The four items of perceived social overload represent the need to provide social support to SNSs. The Cronbach’s alpha (CA) value of the construct used in this study is 0.78.

3.2.3 Perceived system feature overload

The scale of perceived system feature overload was adapted from Karr-Wisniewski and Lu (2010). The four items of perceived system feature overload represent the addition of excessive features on SNSs. The Cronbach’s alpha (CA) value of the construct used in this study is 0.91.

3.2.4 Discontinuous intentions

The discontinuous intention scale was adapted from Cao and Sun (2018) and van Offenbeek et al. (2013) to measure the behavioral patterns of SNS users. The Cronbach’s alpha value of the construct used in this study is 0.98.

3.2.5 Depression

The scale of depression was adapted from Salokangas et al. (1995) and Dhir et al. (2018). The five items of depression represent the emotional state of SNS users. The Cronbach’s alpha (CA) value of the construct used in this study is 0.96.

3.2.6 Anxiety

The scale of anxiety was adapted from La Greca and Lopez (1998) and Dhir et al. (2018). The five items of anxiety represent the user’s pervasive state of mind. The Cronbach’s alpha (CA) value of the construct used in this study is 0.93.

4 Data analysis and results

The data were analyzed through the following approaches. First, we used IBM SPSS 22 to perform exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to measure the reliability and validity (convergent and discriminant) of the constructs. Second, with the comprehensive technique of structure equation modeling (SEM), we used IBM AMOS 23 for confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and determination of model fit indices of our proposed model. Finally, the research hypotheses were tested by examining the structural model.

Table 1 reveals the sociodemographic data of the respondents. Male respondents (52.6%) are more than female (47.4%) respondents. Among them, 47.7% of the respondents have more than 4 years of experience in using SNSs, and 41.6% uses SNSs many times a day.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of respondents.

4.1 Measurement model

4.1.1 Reliability and validity

Validity pertains to how the constructs define the concept of the study, and reliability pertains to the component analysis produced four factors with an Eigen-value greater than 1, explaining 81.07% of the total variance. All factor loadings on the expected factor are within the range of 0.70 to 0.98 (Table 2), and the recommended values should exceed 0.7 to ensure construct validity (Hair et al., 1998). Second, to measure the reliability of the constructs, we used CA and composite reliability (CR) values. The values of CA and CR must exceed the threshold of 0.7 (Anderson and Gerbing, 1988). Table 2 indicates that all CA and CR values exceed 0.7, thereby ensuring measurement reliability. We also checked the average variance extracted (AVE) for each variable against its correlation with other variables to ensure convergent validity. In our data, all AVE values are greater than the minimum threshold of 0.50 as recommended by Fornell and Larcker (1981). Thus, these items satisfied the convergent validity requirements.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analysis, AVE, and composite reliability.

Third, we performed the discriminant validity of our constructs. Discriminant validity is the square root of the AVE and it should be greater than the correlation between it and all other constructs. Table 3 shows that all the values of the square root of AVEs are higher than outer correlations. This finding affirms discriminant validity. Furthermore, we used IBM AMOS 23 to conduct CFA to validate the measures. The CMIN/df = 1.76; NFI = 0.98; IFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.96; CFI = 0.98 and RMSEA = 0.03. The results indicated that the values are within the acceptable range as suggested by Hair et al. (1998). Therefore, the results show a valid model fit.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean, standard deviation, and correlations.

4.1.2 Common method bias

We performed Harman’s one-factor test to evaluate the extent of common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2003) because all questions were answered by the same individual. In this test, the threat of common method bias is considered high if a single factor accounts for more than 50% of the total variance (Harman, 1976). The results reveal that none of the factors dominates the explanation of the variance, in which the most influential factor accounts for 27.0% of the variance. Moreover, other evidence of common method bias includes high correlations (r > 0.9) among variables (Pavlou and El Sawy, 2006). Table 3 shows no unusually high correlation in the sample. Thus, common method bias is not a serious concern in this study. Consistency of the construct is measured through reliability (Carmines and Zeller, 1979).

4.2 Structural model

Our study used IBM AMOS 23 to run the SEM. The basic purpose of SEM is to measure the fitness of the model. The results of the model fit indices show that the model has good fit [ χ 2 957.78 , df = 243, χ 2 /df = 3.94, NFI = 0.94, IFI = 0.95, CFI = 0.95 and RMSEA = 0.07]. The proposed model is within the acceptable range defined by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), that is, χ 2 / d f < 5, NFI > 0.90, IFI > 0.90, CFI > 0.90 and RMSEA <1.0.

This study used SEM to test the proposed hypotheses. H1(a) states that the perceived information overload has a positive relationship with depression was supported ( β = 0.09 , p < 0.001). H1(b) states that the perceived information overload has a positive relationship with anxiety was supported ( β = 0.39 , p < 0.001). H2 (a) states that perceived social overload has a positive relationship with depression was supported ( β = 0.33 , p < 0.001). Similarly, H2(b) states that perceived social overload has a positive relationship with anxiety was supported ( β = 0.26 , p < 0.001). H3 (a) states that perceived system feature overload has a positive relationship with depression was supported ( β = 0.32 , p < 0.001). H3(b) states that perceived system feature overload has a positive relationship with anxiety was supported ( β = 0.12 , p < 0.001). Moreover, H4 states that depression has a positive relationship with discontinuous intention was supported ( β = 0.38 , p < 0.001). H5 states that anxiety has a positive relationship with discontinuous intention was supported ( β = 0.21 , p < 0.001). Furthermore, we used ANOVA to check the significant differences in control variables (gender, age, frequency to use, and experience). The control variables exhibit insignificant effects on discontinuous intentions. Therefore, we exclude the control variables for further analysis.

5 Discussion

5.1 Findings

On the basis of the cognitive–affect–conation (C-A-C) model, the current study proposed a model to examine the effects of overloads on the discontinuous intentions of users. Specifically, the study finds out the empirical linkage of perceived information, social and system feature overload with depression and anxiety that influence users’ discontinuous intentions. The study findings supported the proposed hypotheses. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to examine perceived information, and social and system feature overload in relation to depression, anxiety, and discontinuous intentions.

The current study offers important findings. First, the perception of SNS users toward information overload, social overload, and system feature overload exerts positive influences on their emotions such as depression and anxiety. The study results are in line with the previous study by Zhang et al. (2016), which revealed that perceived information overload, social overload, and system feature overload had a positive relationship with SNS fatigue and dissatisfaction. Similarly, Lee et al., 2016 that information overload exhibits a positive relationship with SNS fatigue. Fu et al. (2020) found that information overload, social overload, and system feature overload lead to an individual’s social media fatigue. Dhir et al. (2018) found that SNS fatigue exhibits a positive significant relationship with dissatisfaction and anxiety. Choi and Lim (2016) found that social overload leads to effect psychological wellbeing of the users. Moreover, Cao and Sun (2018) revealed that information overload, communication overload, and social overload lead to affect individual internal psychological processes, namely, exhaustion and regret.

Second, the study findings show that depression has a positive relationship with discontinuous intentions. The results are in line with the study of Swar et al. (2017), which found that an individual’s psychological ill-being (i.e., negative effect, depressive symptoms, trait anger, and trait anxiety) has a positive relationship with users’ discontinuous intentions. Similarly, Yoon et al. (2019) and Wang et al. (2019) found a positive significant relationship between SNS usage variables (i.e., time spent and frequency to use) and depression.

Finally, the current study found that anxiety has a positive relationship with discontinuous intention. The results are consistent with the study of Becker et al. (2012), which revealed that individuals who suffer from a decline in cognitive capabilities become anxious. Similarly, Dhir et al. (2018) and Hussain et al. (2017) found that compulsive use of SNS causes SNS fatigue, which leads to detrimental consequences (i.e., anxiety). Users with high levels of exhaustion and fatigue are more likely to exit from the situation (Fu et al., 2020).

5.2 Theoretical implications

The study findings have several important theoretical implications. First, the current study extends the research on the negative effects of SNSs use because limited research was conducted on it which has to be explored thoroughly. Our work expands the previous studies that examined the effect of overloads on discontinuous intention and offers a more comprehensive understanding of the dark side of SNSs. Second, previous research studies have described the C-A-C model to understand the sequential casual relationships among cognition, affect, and conation, which has been confirmed by empirical studies of e-commerce, marketing, and information systems. To date, this model is merely utilized in the context of discontinuance among SNSs users. The current study extends the application of the C-A-C framework in discontinuance of SNSs usage. As expected, their cognition (i.e., perceived information and social and system feature overload) exhibits a positive significant effect on their affection (i.e., depression and anxiety), thus affecting their conation (i.e., discontinuous intention). Third, previous studies have examined the effect of information overload, social overload, and system feature overload on user’s psychological wellbeing and find out psychological factors, such as exhaustion, regret, fatigue, and dissatisfaction (Maier et al., 2015a; Luqman et al., 2017; Cao and Sun, 2018; Nawaz et al., 2018). The current study contributes to the existing literature and finds out the effect of information overload, social overload, and system feature overload on depression and anxiety which the previous studies have not examined. Finally, the study examines the negative emotional reactions of individuals, particularly depression and anxiety, thereby substantially contributing to the research on discontinuous use. The study complements the previous studies and extends the literature on discontinuous intention. Our results reveal that depression and anxiety are critical antecedents to the users’ discontinuous intentions.

The current study has several important practical implications. First, perceived information overload, social overload, and system feature overload are the predictors of depression and anxiety. The SNS providers should filter the information available on SNSs such as content personalization and also limit friends’ social circles, and social activities to reduce the effects induce by social overload. Moreover, SNS providers should provide detailed guides about new features to the SNS users to avoid the negative consequences induced through the addition of new features and also remove unnecessary features. Second, our findings demonstrate that depression and anxiety are the key factors of user’s discontinuous intention. Therefore, SNS providers and social marketers on SNS should provide a new mechanism that could effectively prevent users from producing negative emotions. Moreover, SNS users must control their usage and remove unnecessary friends to reduce the emotional consequences. Finally, SNS providers must provide training to SNS users about how to efficiently use SNS and how to effectively extract useful information to get benefits to reduce the negative behavioral consequences.

5.3 Limitation and future work

This study poses several limitations. First, data were collected from university students, which was the best fit for our research study because it is a represented sample of SNSs in China. It is an empirical question as to whether the findings can be generalized to other countries and cultures. Various cultural factors, values, and beliefs have an impact on individual psychological wellbeing (Wissing and Temane, 2008; Grossi et al., 2012). Future research should examine this model in different settings (i.e., organizations), with other samples (i.e., working professionals and users with low education levels), and in different countries to ascertain interesting results. Second, this study was single-center, and the convenience sampling method was used to limit the diversity of results, so the results may not represent the total number of SNSs users to find their discontinuance intentions. Future studies should use some other sampling techniques such as snowball or cluster sampling technique to find out more diverse results. Third, the current study analyzed three cognitive factors (i.e., perceived information overload, social overload, and system feature overload) and two affective factors (i.e., depression and anxiety) in the model. Several other cognitive (i.e., perceived usefulness, perceived information quality, and perceived technology overload) and affective (i.e., envy and sleep disorder) factors may affect discontinuous intentions. Future studies should address the other factors in our model that lead to discontinuous intentions. Finally, the current study did not focus on any specific SNSs because most of the SNSs have similar features and functions, the context of the study is general. Future studies should focus on a comparative study of popular SNSs to find out different results.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants or participants legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

SQ: Writing – original draft. AN: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AJ: Methodology, Resources, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Muhammad Qaisar for his helpful comments and suggestions.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, D. S., Anderson, I. M., Nutt, D. J., Allgulander, C., Bandelow, B., den Boer, J. A., et al. (2014). Evidence-based pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A revision of the 2005 guidelines from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 403–439. doi: 10.1177/0269881114525674

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bashir, H., and Bhat, S. A. (2017). Effects of social media on mental health: a review. Int. J. Ind. Psychol. 4, 125–131. doi: 10.25215/0403.134

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., and Hopwood, C. J. (2012). Media multitasking is associated with symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 132–135. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Schonfeld, I. S., and Laurent, E. (2014). Is burnout a depressive disorder? A reexamination with special focus on atypical depression. Int. J. Stress. Manag. 21, 307–324. doi: 10.1037/a0037906

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowers, W. A. (1986). Cognitive therapy for anxiety disorders. New Dir. Ment. Health Serv. 1986, 45–55. doi: 10.1002/yd.23319863206

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bright, L. F., Kleiser, S. B., and Grau, S. L. (2015). Too much Facebook? An exploratory examination of social media fatigue. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44, 148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.048

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-Translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Can, L., and Kaya, N. (2016). Social networking sites addiction and the effect of attitude towards social network advertising. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 235, 484–492. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2016.11.059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., Khan, A. N., Ali, A., and Khan, N. A. (2019). Consequences of cyberbullying and social overload while using SNSs: a study of users’ discontinuous usage behavior in SNSs. Inf. Syst. Front. 22, 1343–1356. doi: 10.1007/s10796-019-09936-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., Masood, A., Luqman, A., and Ali, A. (2018). Excessive use of mobile social networking sites and poor academic performance: Antecedents and consequences from stressor-strain-outcome perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 85, 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, X., and Sun, J. (2018). Exploring the effect of overload on the discontinuous intention of social media users: An S-O-R perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 81, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.035

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmines, E. G., and Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and Validity Assessment. London: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Carver, L., and Turoff, M. (2007). Human-computer interaction: The human and computer as a team in emergency management information systems. Commun. ACM 50, 33–38. doi: 10.1145/1226736.1226761

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H.-T., and Li, X. (2017). The contribution of mobile social media to social capital and psychological well-being: Examining the role of communicative use, friending and self-disclosure. Comput. Hum. Behav. 75, 958–965. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, S. B., and Lim, M. S. (2016). Effects of social and technology overload on psychological well-being in young South Korean adults: The mediatory role of social network service addiction. Comput. Hum. Behav. 61, 245–254. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, J., Qaisar, S., Shah, Z., and Jalil, A. (2021). Attention or distraction? the impact of mobile phone on users’ psychological well-being. Front. Psychol. 12:612127. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.612127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotten, S. R., Anderson, W. A., and McCullough, B. M. (2013). Impact of internet use on loneliness and contact with others among older adults: cross-sectional analysis. J. Med. Internet Res. 15:e39. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotten, S. R., Ford, G., Ford, S., and Hale, T. M. (2014). internet use and depression among retired older adults in the united states: a longitudinal analysis. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 69, 763–771. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, B., Ali, A., and Wang, H. (2020). Exploring information avoidance intention of social media users: A cognition–affect–conation perspective. Internet Res. 30, 1455–1478. doi: 10.1108/INTR-06-2019-0225

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, F. D. (1989). Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS Q. 13, 319–340. doi: 10.2307/249008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhir, A., Yossatorn, Y., Kaur, P., and Chen, S. (2018). Online social media fatigue and psychological wellbeing—A study of compulsive use, fear of missing out, fatigue, anxiety and depression. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 40, 141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, D., Proeve, M., and Roberts, R. (2018). Problematic Internet Usage self-control dilemmas: The opposite effects of commitment and progress framing cues on perceived value of internet, academic and social behaviors. Comput. Hum. Behav. 82, 16–33. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.039

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eppler, M. J., and Mengis, J. (2004). The concept of information overload: a review of literature from organization science, accounting, marketing, MIS, and related disciplines. Inf. Soc. 20, 325–344. doi: 10.1080/01972240490507974

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farhoomand, A. F., and Drury, D. H. (2002). Managerial information overload. Commun. ACM 45, 127–131. doi: 10.1145/570907.570909

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.2307/3151312

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frison, E., and Eggermont, S. (2015). The impact of daily stress on adolescents’ depressed mood: The role of social support seeking through Facebook. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44, 315–325. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.070

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, S., Li, H., Liu, Y., Pirkkalainen, H., and Salo, M. (2020). Social media overload, exhaustion, and use discontinuance: Examining the effects of information overload, system feature overload, and social overload. Inf. Process. Manag. 57:102307. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2020.102307

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, W., Liu, Z., Guo, Q., and Li, X. (2018). The dark side of ubiquitous connectivity in smartphone-based SNS: An integrated model from information perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 84, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Godden, F. D. (2004). Sample Size Formulas. Available at: http://www.williamgodden.com/samplesizeformula.pdf (Accessed September 11, 2019).

Google Scholar

Grossi, E., Tavano Blessi, G., Sacco, P. L., and Buscema, M. (2012). the interaction between culture, health and psychological well-being: data mining from the italian culture and well-being project. J. Happiness Stud. 13, 129–148. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9254-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., and Tatham, R. L. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (Vol. 5). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Harman, H. H. (1976). Modern factor analysis. 3rd Edn. Chicago, University of Chicago: University of Chicago press.

Google Scholar

Harris, L. C., and Goode, M. M. H. (2004). The four levels of loyalty and the pivotal role of trust: A study of online service dynamics. J. Retail. 80, 139–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2004.04.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, R. J., Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1977). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Contemp. Sociol. 6:244. doi: 10.2307/2065853

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoq, K. M. G. (2014). information overload: causes, consequences and remedies - a study. Philos. Prog. 49–68, 49–68. doi: 10.3329/pp.v55i1-2.26390

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsi, I., and Potts, C. (2000). “Studying the evolution and enhancement of software features” in Proceedings of the 2000 IEEE International Conference on Software Maintenance, 143–151.

Google Scholar

Huang, Y.-M., Lou, S.-J., Huang, T.-C., and Jeng, Y.-L. (2019). Middle-aged adults’ attitudes toward health app usage: A comparison with the cognitive-affective-conative model. Univ. Access Inf. Soc. 18, 927–938. doi: 10.1007/s10209-018-0621-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huitt, W., and Cain, S. (2005). An overview of the conative domain. Educ. Psychol. Interact. 3:45.

Google Scholar

Hussain, Z., Griffiths, M. D., and Sheffield, D. (2017). An investigation into problematic smartphone use: The role of narcissism, anxiety, and personality factors. J. Behav. Addict. 6, 378–386. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karr-Wisniewski, P., and Lu, Y. (2010). When more is too much: Operationalizing technology overload and exploring its impact on knowledge worker productivity. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1061–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koc, M., and Gulyagci, S. (2013). Facebook addiction among Turkish college students: the role of psychological health, demographic, and usage characteristics. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 279–284. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0249

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuss, D. J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Online social networking and addiction—a review of the psychological literature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 8, 3528–3552. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

La Greca, A. M., and Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 26, 83–94. doi: 10.1023/A:1022684520514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. J. Commun. 50, 46–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.tb02833.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LaRose, R., and Eastin, M. S. (2004). A social cognitive theory of internet uses and gratifications: toward a new model of media attendance. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 48, 358–377. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4803_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. R., Son, S.-M., and Kim, K. K. (2016). Information and communication technology overload and social networking service fatigue: A stress perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 55, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., and Karpinski, A. C. (2014). The relationship between cell phone use, academic performance, anxiety, and Satisfaction with Life in college students. Comput. Hum. Behav. 31, 343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.049

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., and Karpinski, A. C. (2015). The relationship between cell phone use and academic performance in a sample of U.S. college students. SAGE Open 5:215824401557316. doi: 10.1177/2158244015573169

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., Chen, W., and Popiel, P. (2015). What happens on Facebook stays on Facebook? The implications of Facebook interaction for perceived, receiving, and giving social support. Comput. Hum. Behav. 51, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, M., Wang, L., and Wu, M. (2014). An integrated methodology for robustness analysis in feature fatigue problem. Int. J. Prod. Res. 52, 5985–5996. doi: 10.1080/00207543.2014.895443

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, J. (2014). The effects of gratifications on intention to read citizen journalism news: The mediating effect of attitude. Comput. Hum. Behav. 36, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, L. Y. I., Sidani, J. E., Shensa, A., Radovic, A., Miller, E., Colditz, J. B., et al. (2016). Association between social media use and depression among U.S. young adults. Depress. Anxiety 33, 323–331. doi: 10.1002/da.22466

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, J., Guo, C., and Bradley, B. (2018). The buffer effect of receiving social support on SNS exhaustion and SNS satisfaction: an exploratory study of the lonely and emotionally unstable. In: Proceedings of the 51st Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS).

Google Scholar

Luqman, A., Cao, X., Ali, A., Masood, A., and Yu, L. (2017). Empirical investigation of Facebook discontinues usage intentions based on SOR paradigm. Comput. Hum. Behav. 70, 544–555. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, A., Mrug, S., and Wright, R. A. (2014). The effects of media violence on anxiety in late adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 116–126. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0017-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, C., Laumer, S., Eckhardt, A., and Weitzel, T. (2015b). Giving too much social support: Social overload on social networking sites. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 24, 447–464. doi: 10.1057/ejis.2014.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, C., Laumer, S., Weinert, C., and Weitzel, T. (2015a). The effects of technostress and switching stress on discontinued use of social networking services: A study of Facebook use. Inf. Syst. J. 25, 275–308. doi: 10.1111/isj.12068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, D., and Saegert, S. (1978). Residential density, social overload, and social withdrawal. Hum. Ecol. 6, 253–272. doi: 10.1007/BF00889026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mick, D. G., and Fournier, S. (1998). Paradoxes of technology: consumer cognizance, emotions, and coping strategies. J. Consum. Res. 25, 123–143. doi: 10.1086/209531

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, J. E. (2000). One road to turnover: an examination of work exhaustion in technology professionals. MIS Q. 24, 141–168. doi: 10.2307/3250982

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, C. M., and Gore, H. (2010). The relationship between excessive internet use and depression: a questionnaire-based study of 1, 319 young people and adults. Psychopathology 43, 121–126. doi: 10.1159/000277001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nawaz, M. A., Shah, Z., Nawaz, A., Asmi, F., Hassan, Z., and Raza, J. (2018). Overload and exhaustion: Classifying SNS discontinuance intentions. Cogent Psychol. 5, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2018.1515584

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oviatt, S. (2006). “Human-centered design meets cognitive load theory: Designing interfaces that help people think” in Proceedings of the 14th ACM International Conference on Multimedia, 871–880.

Google Scholar

Pavlou, P. A., and El Sawy, O. A. (2006). From IT leveraging competence to competitive advantage in turbulent environments: the case of new product development. Inf. Syst. Res. 17, 198–227. doi: 10.1287/isre.1060.0094

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., Mac Kenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Qaisar, S., Chu, J., Shah, Z., and Hassan, Z. (2021). Effects of social networking site overloads on discontinuous intentions of users: A moderated mediation analysis. Behav. Inform. Technol. 41, 3530–3551. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2021.2002411

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Qaisar, S., Chu, J., Shah, Z., and Hassan, Z. (2022). Impact of compulsive use of mobile social networking sites on discontinuous intentions of users: a perspective of stressor-strain-outcome model. Int. J. Mob. Commun. 1:1. doi: 10.1504/IJMC.2022.10036205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahmanyanti, P. L. D., and Yasa, N. N. K. (2019). Discontinuous usage intention of millennials in using Facebook: the role of SNS exhaustion mediates social overload. Eur. J. Bus. Manag. Res. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.24018/ejbmr.2019.4.3.24

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravindran, T., Yeow Kuan, A. C., and Hoe Lian, D. G. (2014). Antecedents and effects of social network fatigue: Antecedents and Effects of Social Network Fatigue. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 65, 2306–2320. doi: 10.1002/asi.23122

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagioglou, C., and Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Facebook’s emotional consequences: Why Facebook causes a decrease in mood and why people still use it. Comput. Hum. Behav. 35, 359–363. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salokangas, R. K. R., Poutanen, O., and Stengård, E. (1995). Screening for depression in primary care Development and validation of the Depression Scale, a screening instrument for depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 92, 10–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1995.tb09536.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Social status and health in humans and other animals. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 33, 393–418. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.070203.144000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherr, S., and Brunet, A. (2017). Differential influences of depression and personality traits on the use of Facebook. Soc. Media Soc. 3:2056305117698495. doi: 10.1177/2056305117698495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., and Sareen, J. (2015). Generalized anxiety disorder. N. Engl. J. Med. 373, 2059–2068. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp1502514

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swar, B., Hameed, T., and Reychav, I. (2017). Information overload, psychological ill-being, and behavioral intention to continue online healthcare information search. Comput. Hum. Behav. 70, 416–425. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarafdar, M., Maier, C., Laumer, S., and Weitzel, T. (2019). Explaining the link between technostress and technology addiction for social networking sites: A study of distraction as a coping behavior. Inf. Syst. J. 30, 96–124. doi: 10.1111/isj.12253

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, D. V., Hamilton, R. W., and Rust, R. T. (2005). Feature fatigue: when product capabilities become too much of a good thing. J. Mark. Res. 42, 431–442. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.2005.42.4.431

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turel, O. (2015). Quitting the use of a habituated hedonic information system: A theoretical model and empirical examination of Facebook users. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 24, 431–446. doi: 10.1057/ejis.2014.19

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turel, O., and Qahri-Saremi, H. (2016). Problematic use of social networking sites: antecedents and consequence from a dual-system theory perspective. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 33, 1087–1116. doi: 10.1080/07421222.2016.1267529

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turel, O., and Serenko, A. (2012). The benefits and dangers of enjoyment with social networking websites. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 21, 512–528. doi: 10.1057/ejis.2012.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Offenbeek, M., Boonstra, A., and Seo, D. (2013). Towards integrating acceptance and resistance research: Evidence from a telecare case study. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 22, 434–454. doi: 10.1057/ejis.2012.29

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermani, M., Marcus, M., and Katzman, M. A. (2011). Rates of detection of mood and anxiety disorders in primary care: A descriptive, cross-sectional study. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 13:PCC.10m01013. doi: 10.4088/PCC.10m01013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P., Liu, S., Zhao, M., Yang, X., Zhang, G., Chu, X., et al. (2019). How is problematic smartphone use related to adolescent depression? A moderated mediation analysis. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 104:104384. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104384

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wissing, M., and Temane, Q. M. (2008). The structure of psychological well-being in cultural context: Towards a hierarchical model of psychological health. J. Psychol. Afr. 18, 45–55. doi: 10.1080/14330237.2008.10820170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, J., Ma, H., Zhou, Z. E., and Tang, H. (2018). Work-related use of information and communication technologies after hours (W_ICTs) and emotional exhaustion: A mediated moderation model. Comput. Hum. Behav. 79, 94–104. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, S., Kleinman, M., Mertz, J., and Brannick, M. (2019). Is social network site usage related to depression? A meta-analysis of Facebook–depression relations. J. Affect. Disord. 248, 65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.01.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Ma, L., Zhang, G., and Wang, G.-S. (2020). An integrated model of the antecedents and consequences of perceived information overload using WeChat as an example. Int. J. Mob. Commun. 18, 19–40. doi: 10.1504/IJMC.2020.104419

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Zhao, L., Lu, Y., and Yang, J. (2016). Do you get tired of socializing? An empirical explanation of discontinuous usage behaviour in social network services. Inf. Manag. 53, 904–914. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2016.03.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, L., Lu, Y., Zhang, L., and Chau, P. Y. K. (2012). Assessing the effects of service quality and justice on customer satisfaction and the continuance intention of mobile value-added services: An empirical test of a multidimensional model. Decis. Support. Syst. 52, 645–656. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2011.10.022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix A

Measures and operationalisation of constructs.

Keywords: social media, depression, information overload, social overload, system feature overload, anxiety, cognition-affect-conation (C-A-C) framework

Citation: Qaisar S, Nawaz Kiani A and Jalil A (2024) Exploring discontinuous intentions of social media users: a cognition-affect-conation perspective. Front. Psychol. 15:1305421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1305421

Received: 01 October 2023; Accepted: 08 January 2024;
Published: 02 February 2024.

Edited by:

Christopher A. Was, Kent State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Ivan Herrera-Peco, Alfonso X El Sabio University, Spain
Umar Farooq, Southern University of Science and Technology, China

Copyright © 2024 Qaisar, Nawaz Kiani and Jalil. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Qaisar, saraqaisar@mail.ustc.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.