- 1Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
- 2Institute for Mindfulness South Africa, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
- 3Department of Surgery, Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
Three decades into democracy, the corollaries of apartheid continue to pattern South African society, with complicated race feelings and resistance to reparative government policies still driving separation. Sharing a grounding with African knowledge systems in the interconnectedness of all people, compassion-based meditation has proven to be a powerful promoter of prosocial action toward strangers and stigmatized groups abroad. It is, however, unclear whether such findings would translate to South Africa with its history of racialised conflict. Here, we piloted a mixed methods study to examine whether 8 weeks of compassion-based meditation would foster positive intergroup attitudes and prosocial outcomes, beyond personal wellbeing, in a White South African sample. We found greater compassion self-practice to be associated not only with significantly increased life satisfaction and reduced stress, but also with heightened outgroup compassion and reduced desire for social distance. Furthermore, post intervention, we observed significantly reduced racial prejudice, increased intergroup contact, and greater prosocial outcomes expressed in support toward collective action and restitutive government policies. Qualitatively, participants reported broadened compassion and affiliation with strangers, suggesting enhanced self-transcendence. These findings offer early support for the potential of compassion-based meditation to improve intergroup relations locally. Future directions include a randomized controlled trial in an appropriately powered sample, and expansion of the methodology to include other social groups.
Introduction
During apartheid South Africa, racialised categories1 of White, Indian, Colored, and Black African were utilized as iconic signifiers of social rank, enforcing limits to people's humanity (Posel, 2001). These race labels persist in contemporary South Africa, trailing along the unresolved pain, division, and violence of their histories, despite significant efforts to redress the power differential (Fourie et al., 2022; Hammett, 2010). While the fall of apartheid undoubtedly ushered in a new era for South Africans, stubborn political divides between racialized groups often dictate the behavior of voters during general elections. Notably, opposition to post-apartheid restorative policies suggests that lingering prejudicial feelings may continue to operate covertly (Dixon et al., 2010a; Fourie and Moore-Berg, 2022; Fourie et al., 2017).
Social identity theory holds that belonging to a social group is associated with the harboring of more positive appraisals of ingroups vs. outgroups, affording a self-esteem gain (Tajfel et al., 1971). Favorable contact with outgroup members, as proposed by Allport (1954), is one of the most well-evidenced strategies to aid in reversing this process through humanizing the “other”2 (Pettigrew and Tropp, 2006). In situations of high prejudice, however, Allport's optimal conditions for contact are not always easily met (Holtman et al., 2005; Pettigrew, 1998), with negative intergroup contact risking a prejudice surge (Barlow et al., 2012). In the South African context, Dixon et al. (2007, 2010b) reported contact-related reductions in prejudice in both Black and White groups, however, those gains did not necessarily predict meaningful prosocial outcomes. While positive intergroup contact undeniably contributes to prejudice reduction and prosocial outcomes amongst advantaged members (Hässler et al., 2020, 2021; Turner et al., 2020), it is not a panacea for stimulating reparative action in historically divided societies (McKeown and Dixon, 2017).
Much intergroup research emphasizes the key role of empathy as a driver of prosocial motivation for outgroups (Finlay and Stephan, 2000; Mealy and Stephan, 2010). Empathy is a multifaceted emotional process, however, with affective sharing, perspective taking, and empathic concern associated with different subjective and behavioral outcomes (Decety and Cowell, 2015). Perspective taking, or the ability to mentally place oneself in another's situation, has been associated with lower levels of prejudice against African Americans in White American college students (Dovidio et al., 2004). Similarly, Vorauer and Sasaki (2009) reported a reduction in prejudice in White Canadians when asked to adopt an imagine-other perspective when watching a documentary showing the adversities of Aboriginal Canadians. However, this gain disappeared when participants had direct contact with outgroup members and was even reversed in high-prejudice individuals.
Empathic concern, which involves other-oriented feelings of compassion or sympathy for someone suffering, is typically associated with altruistic motivation and care-giving behaviors (Batson et al., 2015). Yet various personal and situational factors influence whether and toward whom empathic concern is extended (Zaki, 2014). Empathy-based interventions, for example, may unintentionally foster disproportionate ingroup empathy, which has been associated with increased outgroup harm and reduced helping behavior (Bruneau et al., 2017). In addition, in contexts where (historically) advantaged group members are benefited by the status quo, feelings of concern for those marginalized might not produce prosocial change, but rather serve to mask continued structural inequality (Fourie and Verwoerd, 2022; Dixon et al., 2010a). Cialdini et al. (1997) proposed that for empathic concern to lead to prosocial outcomes, an integration of the other into the self-concept is needed. In line with this view, Decety and Cowell (2015) postulated that the lower levels of prosocial mobilization they observed in individuals with a high social status was linked to a reduced incorporation of outgroup members in their self-construals. How self-other affiliation is mentally constructed may therefore act to modulate prosocial outcomes.
Selfhood is inexorably tied to group identity in African philosophical frameworks, which are ontologically rooted in the premise that the individual derives wholeness from communal belonging (Ramose, 1999). This is clearly expressed in the principle of Ubuntu, articulated by Tutu (1999, p. 31) as: “My humanity is caught up, is inextricably bound up, in yours.” Accordingly, African models of social healing conceptualize personal wellbeing as inseparable from that of the community, framing reconciliation in the aftermath of conflict as arising out of mutual recognition, empathy, and social reintegration (Tutu, 1999). This worldview has guided processes such as South Africa's Truth and Reconciliation Commission, where social repair was enacted through truth-telling, public acknowledgment of harm, and collective forgiveness (Murithi, 2006).
Indigenous social healing traditions from other contexts include the Hawaiian practice of Ho'oponopono, where a mutual release from grievance is achieved through confession, remorse, and the requesting of forgiveness (Yates, 2014). Translating to reciprocity in Quechua, the Andean philosophy of Ayni emphasizes cooperation between people, as well as with a living earth (Pachamama) and the ancestors, as fundamental to a healthy society (Apffel-Marglin, 2011). The Navajo justice and harmony ceremony of Hozhooji Naat‘aanii negotiates peacemaking via a horizontal system where all views and emotional experiences are considered equal in the restoration of solidarity (Bluehouse and Zion, 1993).
Deriving from Eastern contemplative traditions and a relative newcomer to Western scientific research, compassion-based meditation (CBM) acts on the limits of empathy. The CBM practices of mindfulness, loving-kindness, and compassion meditation are seen to cultivate mettā, translating to a love that is universal, pure, benevolent, and unattached (Gilbert, 2005; Hutcherson et al., 2008; Leaviss and Uttley, 2014). The new millennium has seen the rise of CBM practices in the West, popularized for their ability to boost happiness and foster a sense of connectedness, even with those outside of relational circles (Condon et al., 2013; Galante et al., 2014; Hofmann et al., 2011; Hutcherson et al., 2008; Kang et al., 2014; Weng et al., 2013). Strikingly, increases in positive regard, affiliation, and altruistic behavior toward neutral strangers have been observed even after a single session of CBM (Hutcherson et al., 2008; Leiberg et al., 2011; Weng et al., 2013), and also when faced with their suffering (Klimecki et al., 2012).
Underpinned by the affiliative neurochemical systems and shown to activate feelings of interpersonal connection and trust, CBM has been found to lower threat and stress responses and is associated with a shift in motivation from self-orientation to relational care (Condon et al., 2013; Gilbert, 2019; Kang, 2019). Direct effects reported in this regard include heightened positive emotions, improved emotional regulation, broadened attention, and increased empathy and tolerance for self and others, with accompanying reductions in criticism, judgement, blame and defensiveness (Hofmann et al., 2011; Kang, 2019; Weng et al., 2013). Based on these powerful prosocial effects, recent research has been harnessing CBM in a drive to mitigate abiding intergroup prejudice. Specifically, it has proven effective in reducing implicit (Kang et al., 2014; Stell and Farsides, 2016) and explicit (Hunsinger et al., 2012) bias toward stigmatized groups, as well as raising interest in future contact with outgroups (Parks et al., 2014).
Kang (2019) has proposed that CBM's mechanism of action could be that of self-transcendence, defined here as the progression from a defensive favoring of the self to an attitude of beneficence toward others (Kitson et al., 2020). Self-transcendence in its ultimate form can be seen to extend further than mere empathic concern or a broadening of self-construals, but rather a remapping of self and “other” in a newly forged compassionate and global identity. Manuel (2015) refers to a “natural identity,” a oneness that does not deny difference, but is inclusive of multiplicity. In this way, CBM differs from empathy-based interventions, which can inspire parochialism. Moreover, the self-transcendent state mirrors the constituting of the self-in-the-collective in Ubuntu's relational ontology. Being able to experience the “other” as an extension of oneself enables the non-dualistic holding of different viewpoints and a harnessing of common goals (Gilbert, 2019). Further intersecting with Ubuntu values, Manuel (2015) describes compassion as a radical act of interconnectedness that recognizes suffering as collective rather than individual.
Taken together, the intrapersonal dissolution of self-other boundaries and boost in positive feelings implicated in CBM may open a pathway to mediate stubborn intergroup divides in the South African post-apartheid setting, where structural inequalities persist and deep psychosocial divides continue to be reinforced by racialised group identities. An intervention that acts on the “otherness” boundary without the limitations of interpersonal approaches, such as contact, may present the long-sought agent of change, where high levels of prejudice and/or defensive self-interest have consistently posed roadblocks to reconciliation. CBM may furthermore be well suited to the historically fractured South African society, as it operationalises the Ubuntu ethic through realigning personal with collective orientation. With principles and objectives auspiciously congruent with those of African social healing paradigms, CBM may uniquely contribute to recognition and transforming of racialised suffering in societies with intergenerational trauma (Manuel, 2015).
Here we examined whether a CBM intervention can reduce intergroup prejudice and enhance prosocial behavior, potentially through the mechanism of self-transcendence, in a local setting using a mixed methods approach. We recruited a pilot sample of White South African adults to explore the effects of 8 weeks of CBM practice on self-reported wellbeing, prejudice, and other intergroup outcomes. Although this intervention can realistically be expected to hold benefits for all social groups, we elected to pilot the study in a White demographic, to which the authors correspond.3 This decision was motivated by the goal of encouraging meaningful, reparative social action in the social group favored under apartheid.
We hypothesized that following the CBM intervention, (i) subjective wellbeing would increase significantly (Whitesman et al., 2018), (ii) racial prejudice would decrease significantly (Hunsinger et al., 2012), (iii) intergroup contact and compassion would increase significantly (Parks et al., 2014), and that (iv) prosocial intergroup outcomes would improve significantly (Zheng et al., 2023). We further argued that changes in these measures would be significantly associated with CBM self-practice. Finally, we analyzed participants' open-ended responses to assess whether these converged with the quantitative results, and to gain a richer understanding of their experiences.
Methods
Participants
We recruited White participants through online platforms and community notice boards. Exclusion criteria included an ongoing or past contemplative practice exceeding one weekly session, with contemplative practice defined as incorporating a single point of focus, such as mindfulness, conscious breathing, Tai Chi, or Qigong (Bruce et al., 2018). In addition, candidates taking recreational drugs or psychoactive medication were excluded to avoid confounding neurochemical influences. Thirty-one eligible participants signed up for the study, of whom 16 completed all study procedures (Mage= 40.63, SDage= 14.00; 11 female) and received ZAR300 for their participation. Despite the small sample, our post-hoc power analysis indicated that we achieved β > 0.80 for most outcome effects (G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2007).
Materials
Subjective wellbeing and intergroup measures were administered online 1 week prior to the CBM intervention (pre-test), and 1 week afterwards (post-test), with items shuffled to counteract expectancy effects. Survey sections are provided in the Supplementary material. Participants were informed that survey items focused on the three largest racialized groups in South Africa, but that we do not endorse the legitimacy of these artificial racial categories.
Subjective wellbeing
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985) assessed participants' levels of subjective fulfillment (e.g., “In most ways my life is close to my ideal”; 1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree; αs > 0.84), whereas the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen et al., 1983) measured subjective global stress (e.g., “In the last 2 weeks, how often have you been angered because of things that were outside of your control?;” 0 = never, 4 = very often; αs > 0.86).
Racial prejudice
Three feeling thermometers assessed participants' affective prejudice toward White, Black African, and Colored groups on a spectrum from cold to warm (0 = very cold, 100 = very warm; Haddock et al., 1993). Scores were reverse-coded so that higher scores indicate greater prejudice. A South African adaptation of Social Distance Scale (Bogardus, 1925) was used to evaluate affective desire for social distance with racial outgroups. Participants indicated their level of discomfort with three race scenarios (e.g., “It would bother me if my son or daughter ended up marrying a [outgroup] person;” 0 = strongly disagree, 100 = strongly agree; 0.79 < αs <0.86).
Intergroup contact
Four questions assessed the quantity (“In general, how often do you interact?;” 1 = never, 7 = all the time) and quality (“When you do interact, how often are the interactions positive/pleasant vs. negative/unpleasant?;” 1 = always negative, 7 = always positive) of interactions with Black African and Colored outgroups (Bruneau et al., 2020).
Outgroup compassion
Nine questions, adapted from Bruneau et al. (2015), assessed compassion toward White, Black African, and Colored race groups in scenarios involving adversity (e.g., “Colored people who struggle to make ends meet;” 0 = not sorry at all, 100 = very sorry (0.79 < αs <0.96).
Prosocial intergroup outcomes
These were measured across two scales. Collective action support gauged participants' support for disadvantaged groups engaged in collective action for social repair over four local issues (e.g., “Communities from informal settlements protesting over slow land reform;” 0 = strongly oppose, 100 = strongly support; 0.78 <αs; Fourie et al., 2022). While these items did not specifically refer to race groups, the issues raised affect those marginalized by apartheid. Petition support assessed participants' willingness to support progressive outgroup policies (Bruneau et al., 2015). Specifically, they indicated whether they would add their signatures to five petitions that support redistributive policies, e.g., affirmative action (1 = support this petition,−1=oppose this petition, 0=neither support nor oppose this petition; αs > 0.72).
CBM practice
Following Kang et al. (2014), the post-test additionally asked participants to record (i) the number of workshop sessions they attended, (ii) the number and (iii) total duration of CBM self-practice sessions they conducted over the intervention period, and (iv) their enjoyment of other participants in the group.
Open-ended questions
The post-test also asked participants to respond to the following three questions in their own words: (i) “Please share any experiences that stand out for you of the workshop, positive or negative,” (ii) “Please share something important that you learned and what value it had for you with regard to your interactions with other people,” and (iii) “Do you have suggestions for what could have made your experience more rewarding?”.
Procedure
An online CBM workshop intervention comprising 8 weekly 1-h sessions was hosted on Zoom in this within-group pilot study. Facilitated by an experienced and qualified meditation teacher, these sessions involved an anchoring mindfulness meditation practice, followed by guided loving-kindness and compassion meditation group practice, time for discussion, and guidance on self-practice. The compassion practice was gradually expanded each week, inviting participants to cumulatively include in their visualizations: 1) Someone for whom they have unconditional positive regard; 2) themselves; 3) a family member or friend; 4) other course participants; 5) neutral others in their life; 6) someone with whom they have a difficult relationship; 7) other people in the city/country; and 8) unknown others on the continent and in the world. Participants were also asked to undertake daily 10–15 min CBM self-practice sessions, guided by audio files uploaded for them weekly (see Supplementary material).
Data analysis
To test our hypotheses, we first analyzed changes in all intergroup outcome variables with paired-samples t-tests, controlling for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure and a false discovery rate (FDR) of 10% (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). We then examined associations between change scores and intervention-related measures (workshop attendance, number and duration of self-practice sessions, and enjoyment of other participants) with zero-order correlations. Because our hypotheses were directional, these analyses were 1-tailed to achieve greater power. Given the small sample, we inspected 95% confidence intervals (CIs) derived through bootstrapping running 1,000 iterations to help validate our findings (Efron, 1987), and report only associations that survived a Benjamini-Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) of 10%.
Participants' qualitative reflections on their experiences were explored via thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, 2006). These analyses were approached inductively, to explore themes emerging for participants, as well as deductively, to examine a priori-determined themes of personal wellbeing and compassion toward others.
Results
Quantitative analysis
Descriptive statistics for the test variables are reported in Table 1. As anticipated, paired-samples t-tests showed a significant improvement in participants' satisfaction with life [SWLS: t(15) = −3.66, p < 0.001, Hedges' g = −0.87], along with a significant reduction in their perceived stress [PSS: t(15) = 4.44, p < 0.001, g = 1.05], offering support for increased subjective wellbeing.
Regarding measures of racial prejudice, we observed significantly reduced affective prejudice on all feeling thermometers [Black African: t(15) = 2.36, p = 0.016, g = 0.56; Colored: t(15) = 3.57, p = 0.001, g = 0.85; White: t(15) = 2.65, p = 0.009, g = 0.63]. Social distance also decreased for both racial outgroups, suggesting a lowering of interracial barriers. These analyses did not reach significance, however (ps <0.217; gs > 0.20).
In line with expectations, intergroup contact following the intervention increased significantly across both racial outgroups [Black African: t(14) = −2.59, p = 0.011, g = 0.63; Colored: t(14) = −2.44, p = 0.014, g = 0.60]. Compassion scores for racial outgroups were also higher following the intervention, however these gains were not significant (ps <0.149, gs > 0.26).
Prosocial intergroup outcomes were also more favorable following the intervention: support for collective action showed a significant increase, t(15) = −2.68, p = 0.009, g = 0.64, and participants reported a significantly increased willingness to support petitions that would positively impact outgroup members (historically marginalized groups), t(14) = −1.78, p = 0.048, g = 0.44.
To examine associations between changes in test variables (i.e., post-test score – pre-test score) and CBM practice, we performed zero-order correlations (see Table 2). Higher workshop attendance was significantly associated with more CBM self-practice sessions, r = 0.65, p = 0.003, and with a longer duration of self-practice, r = 0.69, p = 0.002, over the 8-week period.
As predicted, positive changes in satisfaction with life and perceived stress were significantly associated with the number (SWLS: r = 0.65, p = 0.004; PSS: r = −0.50, p = 0.028) and duration (SWLS: r = 0.59, p = 0.010) of CBM self-practice sessions. Furthermore, positive changes in compassion toward racial outgroups were significantly associated with more CBM self-practice sessions, r = 0.60, p = 0.009. Finally, reduced outgroup social distance was significantly associated with greater workshop attendance, r = −0.55, p = 0.016. No other intergroup outcomes were significantly associated with CBM practice measures, and notably, none were associated with enjoyment of other participants in the group (ps = n.s.). The latter finding suggests that positive intergroup outcomes were not linked to enjoyment of other participants, offering further support for the specific role of CBM practice as the agent of change.
In summary, the more participants engaged in CBM self-practice over the 8-week period, the greater subjective wellbeing they reported, together with greater outgroup compassion and reduced need for social distance. Although positive changes on other intergroup outcomes were also associated with increased CBM self-practice, these correlations did not reach significance after employing the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR threshold.
Qualitative findings
Central themes were extracted from participants' reflections to support and elucidate the quantitative findings. These were improved personal wellbeing, with subthemes of (i) positive effects and (ii) reductions in negative experience; prosocial outcomes, with subthemes of (i) improved interpersonal relations, and (ii) increased affiliation; and mettā, with (i) broadened compassion and (ii) self-transcendence subthemes.
Improved personal wellbeing
Many positive effects of CBM were reported, with participant #7 expressing: “I was able to be more positive, … engaged with people, present, focused, and in tune with my own emotions both positive and negative,” while finding the practice “incredibly calming, relaxing and beneficial to my day.” Participant #11 said: “I was surprised that I was feeling calmer almost with immediate effect.” Reductions in negative experience were also reported. Participant #9 professed to be “less impatient whilst driving,” while participant #3 found the practice a “very powerful tool for reducing irritability,” and participant #7 echoing: “I am less bothered by small irritants in my daily life than I used to be.” Participant #6 reported “reprieve from racing thoughts,” while participant #4 “learned to take a step back, be less reactive and more aware.”
Prosocial outcomes
Several participants noticed positive changes in themselves that translated into improved social interactions, with participant #7 finding “being happy in myself leading to being happy, kind, encouraging toward others,” adding that “this transformation in myself led to me being more interested in other people.” Participant #4 expressed that: “Compassionate responses create peaceful and happy situations,” and participant #5 found: “The calmer I am, the better space I am to see the good in others.” Increased affiliation with others was also reported, with participant #7 finding: “I am faster to greet strangers and engage in a conversation.” Participant #6 stated: “I believe that in a country such as South Africa, learning to have a greater sense of others” wellbeing is important, as we have become so desensitized to the poverty and horrible living conditions many in this country face.'
Mettã
Most participants reported the desired outcome of the CBM practice, namely, a cultivation of compassion, with participant #12 reporting being “surprised to see how I can easily expand my compassion broadly.” Participant #14 described: “I started looking at people and their actions with more understanding and was also kinder to myself when usually I would be quite harsh.” Participant #10 expressed that the practice “helped me to value those people around me,” while “approaching others and situations and myself with curiosity rather than judgement” was experienced by participant #8. Participant #1 described that the practice helped them: “to have compassion even though you do not agree or understand someone's actions.”
Self-transcendence, a shift in focus from self to other, is evidenced in feedback from participant #15, who expressed that it is “just so essential to view others as individuals and recognize that you are not the main character.” Participant #4 said that “it's easy to see how we are all part of one whole.” Similarly, participant #6 reported: “I learned to consider other people's wellbeing, and as someone that is naturally quite self-centered, I think this was an important lesson for me.” CBM helped participant #3 “be more present and conscious about how I arrive for other people.” This self-transcendence process is visualized in Figure 1, with the widening circle of mettā expanding self-construals and enhancing compassionate relationships with both the self and others.

Figure 1. The widening circle of self-transcendence from compassion-based meditation (CBM) practice.
Discussion
Compassion-based meditation (CBM) focuses on the transformation of self-awareness through self-transcendence, which is particularly relevant in the context of structural inequalities in post-apartheid South Africa. This mixed methods pilot study was conducted to examine whether CBM practice could improve not only prejudicial, but also prosocial, intergroup outcomes in the South African context characterized by enduring historical racial conflict and power asymmetry. Preliminary results suggest that the sample of White adults who participated experienced greater subjective wellbeing and expressed more positive intergroup attitudes and prosocial tendencies following 8 weeks of CBM practice. Qualitative findings provided converging support, echoing CBM research in other settings associated with enhanced compassion for the self and others (Hofmann et al., 2011; Kang, 2019; Weng et al., 2013).
Post intervention, we observed significant increases in life satisfaction and reductions in perceived stress, and increasingly so, the greater CBM practice time participants reported. Positive intrapersonal effects described by participants included feelings of calm, positivity, increased presence, focus, awareness, groundedness, and tolerance of negative emotions. Reductions in irritability, anxiety, overthinking, reactivity, judgment, animosity, and anger were also reported. Significant positive changes in intergroup outcomes following the intervention included reduced affective prejudice toward all social groups, increased intergroup contact with both racial outgroups, and greater prosocial outcomes, as measured by collective action support and petitions supporting government restitution policies. It should be noted that all participants did not have the same experience throughout the 8-week CBM intervention, as some engaged in considerably more self-practice than others. Indeed, correlation analysis showed that those participants who engaged in more CBM self-practice sessions not only reported enhanced personal wellbeing, but also expressed greater outgroup compassion and reduced desire to socially distance from other racial groups. The qualitative findings suggest that participants made spontaneous connections between their increased wellbeing and improvements in their social experiences, reporting enhanced interpersonal peace, happiness, kindness, benevolent engagement, and a more positive view of all others. Their reports described a renewed awareness of others, eliciting feelings of empathy and openness, in many cases followed by a bridging of social distance in their daily lives.
A key finding emerging from participants' feedback describes a shift from self-orientation to greater affiliation with others, in line with Parks, Birtel and Crisp (2014) self-transcendence theory and the Ubuntu principle. Participants were guided to cultivate mettā through contemplating their most cherished relationships, sequentially extending this quality to themselves and their circles of affiliation, followed by those outside of relational circles. As the circle was broadened, conceivably inviting the expanding of self-construals to incorporate previous outgroup circles, the qualities of mettā can be seen to have extended further, stimulating the positive findings seen. With continued practice, the boundary between self and not-self is said to ultimately dissolve (see Figure 1), in accordance with Gilbert's (2005, p. 106) explanation of the change being “from a self-preservative to a species-preservative mode.” In effect, we could understand mettā as eventually subsuming all “others” into selfhood, where selfhood becomes synonymous with subjectively experienced oneness. This effect converges with Ubuntu's forgoing of individual objectives in service of collective wellbeing, suggesting that CBM may offer a culturally consonant vehicle for promoting intergroup affiliation in South Africa, with further study needed to assess its action in other social groups.
Participants commented on finding the workshop environment safe and non-judgemental. The function of the workshop sessions could be conceptualized as initiating and supporting the compassion practice, with those attending more workshop sessions also engaging in more self-practice between sessions. Importantly, enjoyment of other participants during the workshop sessions was not significantly associated with any positive outcomes, suggesting that the “active” ingredient was the compassion training itself. Many participants expressed the intention to continue their CBM practice going forward, motivated by the benefits already experienced. Participant 7 expressed: “I am now able to sit and offer compassion to myself and others, [and] notice periods of time or situations where a mindfulness practice will be useful or needed.”
This study's main objective reflects in feedback such as this by participant 6: “I specifically remember a time in the park, where I listened to … a part of the practice recording that asked me to imagine somebody in my mind. Near the park is a traffic guard, who always greets me when I walk past. That day I envisioned the man, properly picturing his face, his feelings and his wellbeing. Whilst before I had merely glanced at him.”
Limitations and directions for future research
The small sample size prevents a generalization of the present findings to the broader population. We employed bootstrapping, FDR correction, and qualitative triangulation as strategies to mitigate this limitation, however, so that the results do provide an indication of trends that could reasonably be expected in a stronger powered study. In addition, while small sample sizes may inflate effect sizes, we reported Hedges' g effect sizes, which provide a correction factor to reduce positive bias when the sample size is small (Durlak, 2009). We observed large effects (>0.85) for subjective wellbeing and moderate effects for various intergroup outcomes, including prosocial behavior (0.44 <g < 0.75). These results are practically meaningful for practitioners and policymakers who seek effective interventions to promote complex social change.
The inclusion of an active control condition would be needed to distinguish conclusively the specific contribution of CBM practice from (i) experimental factors such as expectancy effects and social desirability bias, (ii) situational factors, such as external sociopolitical influences, and (iii) more generally from increased awareness or greater mindfulness (Aggarwal and Ranganathan, 2019; Van Dam et al., 2018). Active control conditions might include relaxation training, general discussion groups, mindful movement, or sham meditation. The absence of a control group currently limits any causal interpretations.
A factor in the present study that potentially obscured the extent of intergroup benefits possible, is the sample's fairly progressive intergroup attitudes pre-intervention. Because of high baseline attitudes, the scope of positive change may have been reduced in our sample, and the absence of significant results across some outcome variables, such as compassion for racial outgroups, could be understood in this light. These effects may have been exacerbated by self-selection bias, which can be reduced in future research by recruiting a more representative sample through stratified or purposeful sampling. Partnering with community leaders, tailoring recruitment material with culturally inclusive language, and removing structural barriers to participation could also assist in securing a more diverse sample (Watson-Singleton et al., 2019).
Despite these limitations, our findings suggest that CBM may be an area worthy of further exploration not only in the South African context, but also in other post-conflict or divided societies (e.g., Rwanda, Sri Lanka, Bosnia) to support the recovery from collective trauma. Conducting a larger randomized controlled trial (RCT) in future research would be necessary to validate the present statistical conclusions, while longitudinal studies would need to ascertain the long-term effects of CBM on intergroup relations and prosocial attitudes. The intervention should also be extended to other social groups (race and socioeconomic status), including people of color (Manuel, 2015). Transferring the methodology to other groups would need to be undertaken with an awareness of the added cultural and contextual complexities at play, however. CBM interventions have primarily been studied in western populations and may not be a good fit for all cultural groups. Additional piloting and conducting focus groups, for example, may be necessary to ascertain whether the present methodology is appropriate.
Finally, effective scaling of CBM interventions to address social divisions might be achieved through participatory adaptations, such as training community leaders as local facilitators of CBM practices (e.g., teachers, religious leaders, social workers), and integrating brief compassion practices into existing curricula (e.g., wellness, parenting, education) using locally resonant language and symbols (Potvin et al., 2023). In this way, age-appropriate and flexible CBM delivery options could be implemented in community, education, and religious settings, supported through cross-sectoral collaboration.
Conclusion
Many of the benefits associated with CBM practice in other settings were seen to translate to a South African context in this pilot study, offering promising initial support for the potential of CBM to enhance positive outgroup attitudes and boost prosocial outcomes in South African adults. In contexts of high prejudice where other social interventions driving structural reform may have stalled, dismantling the barriers to connection may well be an “inside job.”
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Health Research Ethics Committee at Stellenbosch University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
AP: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. MG: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MF: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by Stellenbosch University through internal research funding.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1450549/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
1. ^We acknowledge that these racial constructions are not based in biological realities or markers of inherent identities. Rather, racial identifiers are carriers of social and political meaning, in flux over time and space (Dolby, 2001). It is in this spirit that race labels are used in the present study, with an acknowledgment of the heterogeneities that they represent and the violence inherent in these terms.
2. ^The term “other” is used to signify another with whom the subject does not identify, i.e., an outgroup member, conceptually mapped outside of self-construals.
3. ^Positionality Statement: Mindful that social identities can influence one's approach to research and interpretation of data (Roberts et al., 2020), the authors wish to provide the reader with potentially relevant information about their backgrounds. With respect to race and nationality: all authors identify as White and South African. While conscious of the intersectional complexity and heterogeneity within socially constructed categories, we believe referring to Black African, Colored, and White people is warranted, without intending to oversimplify, given the legacies of systematic racial discrimination under colonialism and apartheid in South Africa.
References
Aggarwal, R., and Ranganathan, P. (2019). Study designs: part 4–interventional studies. Pers. Clin. Res. 10, 137–139. doi: 10.4103/picr.PICR_91_19
Apffel-Marglin, F. (2011). Subversive Spiritualities: How Rituals Enact the World. New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199793853.001.0001
Barlow, F. K., Paolini, S., Pedersen, A., Hornsey, M. J., Radke, H. R., Harwood, J., et al. (2012). The contact caveat: negative contact predicts increased prejudice more than positive contact predicts reduced prejudice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 1629–1643. doi: 10.1177/0146167212457953
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., and Stocks, E. L. (2015). “The empathy-altruism hypothesis,” in The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior, eds. D. A. Schroeder and W. G. Graziano (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 259–281.
Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple hypothesis testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Bluehouse, P., and Zion, J. W. (1993). Hozhooji Naat'aanii: the Navajo justice and harmony ceremony. Med. Q. 10, 327–337. doi: 10.1002/crq.3900100403
Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Bruce, M. A., Jeffers, K. S., Robinson, J. K., and Norris, K. C. (2018). Contemplative practices: a strategy to improve health and reduce disparities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15, 1–9. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102253
Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M., and Saxe, R. (2015). Minding the gap: narrative descriptions about mental states attenuate parochial empathy. PLoS ONE 10, 1–18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140838
Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M., and Saxe, R. (2017). Parochial empathy predicts reduced altruism and the endorsement of passive harm. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 8, 934–942. doi: 10.1177/1948550617693064
Bruneau, E. G., Hameiri, B., Moore-Berg, S. L., and Kteily, N. (2020). Intergroup contact reduces dehumanization and meta-dehumanization: cross-sectional, longitudinal and quasi-experimental evidence from 16 samples in 5 countries. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 47, 906–920. doi: 10.1177/0146167220949004
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., and Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: when one into one equals oneness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 481–494. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.481
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Beh. 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404
Condon, P., Desbordes, G., Miller, W. B., and DeSteno, D. (2013). Meditation increases compassionate responses to suffering. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2125–2127. doi: 10.1177/0956797613485603
Decety, J., and Cowell, J. M. (2015). Empathy, justice, and moral behavior. AJOB Neurosci. 6, 3–14. doi: 10.1080/21507740.2015.1047055
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., and Tredoux, C. (2007). Intergroup contact and attitudes toward the principle and practice of racial equality. Psychol. Sci. 18, 867–872. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01993.x
Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., Tredoux, C., Tropp, L., Clack, B., and Eaton, L. (2010a). A paradox of integration? Interracial contact, prejudice reduction, and perceptions of racial discrimination. J. Soc. Iss. 66, 401–416. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01652.x
Dixon, J., Tropp, L. R., Durrheim, K., and Tredoux, C. (2010b). “Let them eat harmony”: Prejudice-reduction strategies and attitudes of historically disadvantaged groups. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 76–80. doi: 10.1177/0963721410363366
Dolby, N. E. (2001). Constructing Race: Youth, Identity, and Popular Culture in South Africa. New York, NY: SUNY Press.
Dovidio, J. F., Ten Vergert, M., Stewart, T. L., Gaertner, S. L., Johnson, J. D., Esses, V. M., et al. (2004). Perspective and prejudice: antecedents and mediating mechanisms. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1537–1549. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271177
Durlak, J. A. (2009). How to select, calculate, and interpret effect sizes. J. Ped. Psychol. 34, 917–928. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp004
Efron, B. (1987). Better bootstrap confidence intervals. J. Am. Stat. Ass. 82, 171–185, doi: 10.1080/01621459.1987.10478410
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Meth. 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
Finlay, K. A., and Stephan, W. G. (2000). Improving intergroup relations: the effects of empathy on racial attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 1720–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02464.x
Fourie, M. M., Deist, M., and Moore-Berg, S. L. (2022). Hierarchies of being human: Intergroup dehumanization and its implication in present-day South Africa. Peace Conflict J. Peace Psychol. 28, 345–360. doi: 10.1037/pac0000616
Fourie, M. M., and Moore-Berg, S. L. (2022). We cannot empathize with what we do not recognize: perceptions of structural vs. interpersonal racism in South Africa. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.838675
Fourie, M. M., Stein, D. J., Solms, M., Gobodo-Madikizela, P., and Decety, J. (2017). Empathy and moral emotions in post-apartheid South Africa: an fMRI investigation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 881–892. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx019
Fourie, M. M., and Verwoerd, W. J. (2022). COVID-19 as natural intervention: guilt and perceived historical privilege contributes to structural reform under conditions of crisis. Affect. Sci. 3, 34–45. doi: 10.1007/s42761-021-00073-7
Galante, J., Galante, I., Bekkers, M. J., and Gallacher, J. (2014). Effect of kindness-based meditation on health and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 82, 1101–1114. doi: 10.1037/a0037249
Gilbert, P. (2019). Explorations into the nature and function of compassion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.002
Gilbert, P. ed. (2005). Compassion: Conceptualisations, Research and Use in Psychotherapy. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203003459
Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P., and Esses, V. M. (1993). Assessing the structure of prejudicial attitudes: the case of attitudes towards homosexuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 1105–1118. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1105
Hammett, D. (2010). Ongoing contestations: the use of racial signifiers in post-apartheid South Africa. Soc. Ident. 16, 247–260. doi: 10.1080/13504631003691090
Hässler, T., Ullrich, J., Bernardino, M., Shnabel, N., Laar, C. V., Valdenegro, D., et al. (2020). A large-scale test of the link between intergroup contact and support for social change. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 380–386. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0815-z
Hässler, T., Ulug, Ö. M., Kappmeier, M., and Travaglino, G. A. (2021). Intergroup contact and social change: an integrated contact-collective action model. J. Soc. Iss. 77, 217–241. doi: 10.1111/josi.12412
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., and Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: potential for psychological interventions. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 1126–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.003
Holtman, Z., Louw, J., Tredoux, C., and Carney, T. (2005). Prejudice and social contact in South Africa: a study of integrated schools ten years after apartheid. SA J. Psychol. 35, 473–493. doi: 10.1177/008124630503500306
Hunsinger, M., Livingston, R., and Isbell, L. (2012). Spirituality and intergroup harmony: meditation and racial prejudice. Mindfulness 5, 139–144. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0159-5
Hutcherson, C. A., Seppala, E. M., and Gross, J. J. (2008). Loving-kindness meditation increases social connectedness. Emot. 8, 720–724. doi: 10.1037/a0013237
Kang, Y. (2019). Examining interpersonal self-transcendence as a potential mechanism linking meditation and social outcomes. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 115–119. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.007
Kang, Y., Gray, J. R., and Dovidio, J. F. (2014). The nondiscriminating heart: Lovingkindness meditation training decreases implicit intergroup bias. J. Exp. Psychol. 143, 1–8. doi: 10.1037/a0034150
Kitson, A., Chirico, A., Gaggioli, A., and Riecke, B. E. (2020). A review on research and evaluation methods for investigating self-transcendence. Front. Psychol. 11:547687. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.547687
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2012). Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142
Leaviss, J., and Uttley, L. (2014). Psychotherapeutic benefits of compassion-focused therapy: an early systematic review. Psychol. Med. 45, 927–945. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002141
Leiberg, S., Klimecki, O., and Singer, T. (2011). Short-term compassion training increases prosocial behavior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS ONE 6, 1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017798
Manuel, Z. E. (2015). The Way of Tenderness: Awakening through Race, Sexuality, and Gender. Boston: Wisdom Publications.
McKeown, S., and Dixon, J. (2017). The “contact hypothesis”: critical reflections and future directions. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 11:e12295. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12295
Mealy, M., and Stephan, W. G. (2010). “Intergroup empathy,” in The Encyclopedia of Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, eds. J. Levine and M. A. Hogg (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications), 475–478.
Murithi, T. (2006). Practical peacemaking wisdom from Africa: Reflections on Ubuntu. J. Pan Afr. Stud. 1, 25–34.
Parks, S., Birtel, M. D., and Crisp, R. J. (2014). Evidence that a brief meditation exercise can reduce prejudice toward homeless people. Soc. Psychol. 45, 458–465. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000212
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 49, 65–85. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65
Pettigrew, T. F., and Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90:751. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751
Posel, D. (2001). Race as common sense: racial classification in twentieth-century South Africa. Afr. Stud. Rev. 44, 87–113. doi: 10.2307/525576
Potvin, A. S., Penuel, W. R., Dimidjian, S., and Jinpa, T. (2023). Cultivating skillful means of care in schools through compassion practice and individual and joint inquiry. Mindfulness 14, 2499–2515. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01867-x
Roberts, S. O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F. A., Goldie, P. D., and Mortenson, E. (2020). Racial inequality in psychological research: trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Pers. Psychol. Sci. 15, 1295–1309. doi: 10.1177/1745691620927709
Stell, A. J., and Farsides, T. (2016). Brief loving-kindness meditation reduces racial bias, mediated by positive other-regarding emotions. Motiv. Emot. 40, 140–147. doi: 10.1007/s11031-015-9514-x
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., and Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1, 149–178. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420010202
Turner, R. N., Hodson, G., and Dhont, K. (2020). The role of individual differences in understanding and enhancing intergroup contact. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 14:e12533. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12533
Tutu, D. (1999). No Future Without Forgiveness. New York: Random House. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5842.1999.tb00012.x
Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., et al. (2018). Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect Psychol. Sci. 13, 36–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691617709589
Vorauer, J. D., and Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Helpful only in the abstract? Ironic effects of empathy in intergroup interaction. Psychol. Sci. 20, 191–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02265.x
Watson-Singleton, N. N., Black, A. R., and Spivey, B. N. (2019). Recommendations for a culturally-responsive mindfulness-based intervention for African Americans. Complement. Ther. Clin. Pract. 34, 132–138. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2018.11.013
Weng, H. Y., Fox, A. S., Shackman, A. J., Stodola, D. E., Caldwell, J. Z., Olson, M. C., et al. (2013). Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1171–1180. doi: 10.1177/0956797612469537
Whitesman, S. L., Hoogenhout, M., Kantor, L., Leinberger, K.J, and Gevers, A. (2018). Examining the impact of a mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention on the health of urban South Africans. Afr. J. Prim. Health Care Fam. Med. 10, e1–e5. doi: 10.4102/phcfm.v10i1.1614
Yates, M. (2014). Na'auao Ola Hawaii: Hawaiian Principles and Practices of Being Well. Bloomington, IN: Balboa Press.
Keywords: compassion, meditation, self-transcendence, prejudice, prosocial, intergroup
Citation: Prevost AM, George M and Fourie MM (2025) A kind mind: effects of compassion-based meditation on prosocial intergroup outcomes in a South African pilot sample. Front. Psychol. 16:1450549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1450549
Received: 17 June 2024; Accepted: 13 June 2025;
Published: 11 July 2025.
Edited by:
Sérgio A. Carvalho, University of Coimbra, PortugalReviewed by:
Rohan Bodapati, University of Wisconsin-Madison, United StatesNur Fadhilah Umar, State University of Makassar, Indonesia
Copyright © 2025 Prevost, George and Fourie. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Melike M. Fourie, bWFyZXRoZW1AZ21haWwuY29t
‡ORCID: Adala M. Prevost orcid.org/0000-0002-8536-2128
Melike M. Fourie orcid.org/0000-0003-4879-7250