ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 June 2025

Sec. Environmental Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1534014

Combining mechanisms of action with behavior change techniques—theory-based development of an app promoting heating energy-saving behaviors

Updated
  • 1Medical Assistance Systems Group, Medical School OWL, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 2Interactive Robotics in Medicine and Care, Medical School OWL, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • 3Department of Social Sciences, University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Bielefeld, Germany

Introduction: There is an ongoing debate whether the currently used psychological interventions to motivate people to switch to more pro-environmental behavioral alternatives are effective. In the present paper the ‘theory and technique tool’ (TaTT) developed by the Human Behavior Change Project is used to demonstrate the theory-based development of a mobile app promoting heating energy saving behaviors.

Methods: For this purpose, from the stage model of self-regulated behavioral change (SSBC) so-called Mechanisms of Action (MoA) are derived mediating the impact of the intervention on behavioral change. The TaTT is then used for linking these MoAs systematically with evidence based ‘behavior change techniques’ (BCTs).

Results: In a next step, conceptual design ideas are developed as operationalizations of the included BCTs. In an experimental lab study, we test the effectiveness of one central conceptual design idea aiming to motivate participants to use intervention packages specially tailored to the needs which according to the SSBC an intervention has to target in that stage. The results, however, provide little empirical evidence that this design idea works as theoretically expected.

Discussion: This finding underlines the importance of explicitly testing the ability of conceptual design ideas to activate theoretically proposed MoA-BCT links before the large-scale implementation of that intervention in a costly field study.

1 Introduction

Approximately two-thirds of global greenhouse emissions are directly or indirectly linked to household consumption (Ivanova et al., 2020). Changes in consumption patterns toward low-carbon alternatives, therefore, present a great potential for reducing greenhouse emissions. In the transport sector, for example, a car-free life or switching to a battery-electric vehicle with “green electricity” and avoiding air travel could reduce a person’s CO2 emissions by around 1.7 t per year (Ivanova et al., 2020). Unfortunately, there is still a debate about whether the currently available empirical evidence actually supports the effectiveness of behavioral change interventions used for this purpose (see, for example, the exchange between Nisa et al., 2019; van der Linden and Goldberg, 2020; Stern, 2020). In health psychology, a similar debate has led to the realization that more intellectual and empirical investment is urgently needed in designing effective, theory-based interventions. As a consequence, two research projects were established, focusing on the systematic theory-based intervention development: the Science Of Behavior Change (SOBC, Sumner et al., 2018) and the Human Behavior Change Project (HBCP, Michie et al., 2017). These research projects have provided important theoretical and methodological impulses on the subject of theory-based intervention development over the past decade.

Because there is currently no theoretical model that can understand and predict behavior change across all behavioral domains and contexts (Davis et al., 2014; Michie et al., 2014), both HBCP and SOBC take a more metatheoretical approach: theories of behavioral change are seen as ‘… a set of concepts and/or statements with specification of how phenomena relate to each other’, providing ‘… an organizing description of a system that accounts for what is known, and explains and predicts phenomena’ (Davis et al., 2014, p. 5). From this perspective, theories of behavior change are important for intervention development because they summarize why and how behavior change occurs, what is known about the role of specific constructs in this process, and propose both mechanisms of action and moderators of change along various causal pathways (Michie et al., 2018). Consequently, within the HBCP approach, behavior change theories play a central role in providing so-called ‘mechanisms of action’ (MoAs), which are defined as processes that mediate the effect of interventions on behavior change. On the other hand, in the HBCP approach, the active components of an intervention are referred to as “Behavior Change Techniques” (BCTs). BCTs are defined as an “observable, replicable, and irreducible component of an intervention designed to alter or redirect causal processes that regulate behavior” (Michie et al., 2013). The BCTs cause changes in the MoAs, which in turn cause behavioral changes (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. BCTs change behavior via mechanisms of action (adapted from Michie et al., 2016, p. 12).

Thus, according to the HBCP approach, the core of systematic intervention development consists of the theory-based development of so-called BCT-MoA links. An improved understanding of the links between BCTs and MoAs can facilitate the development of more effective interventions and improve the ability to explain how effective interventions bring about change. Most behavior change interventions use combinations of multiple BCTs, that is, the active ingredients within an intervention that lead to behavior change. The use of multiple BCTs within an intervention does not in itself necessarily increase intervention effectiveness; however, interventions that use a combination of BCTs aligned with a behavior change theory have been associated with increased intervention effectiveness (Dombrowski et al., 2012). Therefore, thinking systematically about how different BCTs work synergistically could improve our ability to examine associations between theory and intervention effectiveness.

Within the context of this discussion, the development of the BCT Taxonomy v1 (BCTTv1; Michie et al., 2013) was an important step. The BCTTv1 is a classification system for systematically specifying intervention components in terms of 93 BCTs organized into 16 groupings (Michie et al., 2013). In a methodologically similar process, Michie and colleagues (e.g., Michie et al., 2018; Carey et al., 2019) extracted a taxonomy of 26 MoAs. Of the 26 MoAs, 14 stemmed from the theoretical domains framework (Cane et al., 2012) and the remaining 12 MoAs included the most frequently occurring MoAs from 83 behavioral theories documented in the research literature (Michie et al., 2018; Carey et al., 2019). In two studies, both taxonomies were systematically linked: Connell et al. (2019) conducted an expert consensus study using the Delphi approach to link the most frequently occurring BCTs with the 26 MoAs (Connell et al., 2019); Johnston et al. (2020) conducted a triangulation study drawing on the results of the first studies to produce the so-called heat map classifying the BCT-MoA links into those with evidence for a “link,” “inconclusive,” “non-link,” and “no evidence.” This classification is based on an analysis of the results of 277 evaluation studies. By creating the web-based theory and technique tool (TaTT1), Michie and colleagues (Michie et al., 2018; Carey et al., 2019; Connell et al., 2019) have made the results of the above-described research process easily accessible to practitioners. Thus, intervention designers and evaluators can use the TaTT as a starting point for their intervention development process.

However, while MoA-BCT relationships can be systematically derived using TaTT, the concrete operationalization of a specific BCT in the context of an intervention project remains a primarily creative task. More precisely, this task consists of translating the relatively abstractly formulated BCTs into practical intervention elements used to influence the MoAs in such a way that the persons targeted by the intervention change their behavior or preserve this behavioral change.

2 The present study

Inspired in particular by the HBCP approach, the present study aims to use the theory-based intervention development approach in the area of promoting environmentally friendly behavior. We view applying the HBCP approach as a promising strategy to move forward in the abovementioned debate about how to effectively identify interventions that promote pro-environmental behaviors. More precisely, we apply the HBCP approach within the context of developing a tablet-based app that promotes heating energy-saving behaviors (e.g., lowering the temperature at night or if away for a long time, shock ventilation, and installing electronically controlled thermostats). Because 70% of a household’s fossil energy use relates to heating, interventions aiming to motivate consumers to use heating energy carefully have a high potential for reducing a household’s greenhouse emissions. The aim of the app is to motivate people to find out about their way to save heating energy, form an intention to actually implement selected heating energy-saving behaviors, implement this intention, and give them the opportunity to get feedback on the effects of these behavioral changes on their actual heating energy consumption.

In the first theoretical section of the article, we present the stage model of self-regulated behavioral change (SSBC) proposed by Bamberg (2013). We use the SSBC for identifying central mechanisms of action (MoAs) assumed to be associated with changes in pro-environmental behaviors. In the second conceptual section, we apply the web-based theory and technique tool (TaTT; Carey et al., 2019) to identify and select evidence-supported BCT-MoA links. In the third section, we develop design ideas for operationalizing the BCTs in the app. In the fourth empirical section, we present a lab-based approach for testing a priori the validity of assumptions derived from the conceptual framework. However, instead of testing the whole behavior-MoA-BCT links presented in Figure 1, the study focuses on a central assumption underlying the SSBC: As a stage model, it assumes that people in different stages of the behavioral change process need and prefer different intervention packages. The aim of the study is, therefore, to empirically test whether preferences for intervention packages differ between participants at different stages in the theoretically expected manner.

3 The stage model of self-regulated behavioral change

In the last decades, stage models such as the Transtheoretical Model of Behavioral Change (TTM, Prochaska and Velicer, 1997) have become increasingly popular; however, they are also heavily criticized theoretical approaches to explain behavioral change. In addition to the question of whether behavioral change actually occurs in stages, the number as well as the theoretical demarcation and operationalization of these stages is controversial (e.g., West, 2005). Responding to this criticism, Bamberg (2013) developed the stage model of self-regulated behavioral change (SSBC). The SSBC combines the model of action phases (MAP; Gollwitzer, 2012) with other more static theories, such as the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991) and the norm activation model (Schwartz and Howard, 1981). Thereby, it aims to extend previous stage models by considering both the dynamic, longitudinal processes that reflect multiple stages of decision-making, as well as the factors influencing each of those single decisions or stage transitions. Figure 2 presents the SSBC graphically.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The stage model of self-regulated behavioral change (SSBC, Bamberg, 2013, p. 153).

In the first stage, the predecision stage, the current behavior needs to be perceived as problematic, leading to an intention to reduce this behavior and, subsequently, the negative consequences resulting from it (goal intention). Variables assumed to influence the formation of such a goal intention are primarily taken from the norm activation model (NAM; Schwartz and Howard, 1981). In the next stage, the preaction stage, an individual has to choose an alternative behavior to achieve the goal of reducing their current problematic behavior formed in the predecision stage. The formation of such a behavioral intention could be modeled via the theory of planned behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 1991). Next, in the actional stage, the new behavior has to be implemented in everyday life, determined by the strength of a person’s implementation intention (Gollwitzer, 1999), which depends on a person’s planning abilities and maintenance self-efficacy. Finally, in the postaction stage, the strength and weaknesses associated with the performance of the new behavior are validated. Preventing relapse in critical situations is also an important task in the postaction stage.

A systematic review of studies applying the SSBC, published by Keller et al. (2019), includes 10 studies on environmental topics, such as travel mode choice and smart meter use, published between 2013 and 2018, six of which employed a cross-sectional design, three an interventional design, and one a correlational longitudinal design. The cross-sectional and longitudinal studies generally support the model, although there are some irregularities that warrant further investigation.

The SSBC was explicitly developed as a conceptual framework for guiding systematic intervention development. There is one practical important implication of considering behavioral change as a transition process through different stages: There is no single intervention for all. Instead, specific intervention packages have to be developed to meet the needs and barriers of persons in each specific stage. For instance, interventions targeting persons in the predecision stage are more likely to be successful if they concentrate on providing information that can increase both problem awareness and perceived personal responsibility. Moreover, interventions activating social and personal norms are likely to be important in this stage. Individuals who already have set a goal-intention (preaction stage) need information concerning the availability and the pros and cons of different behavioral alternatives. Individuals who intend on switching to an alternative behavior (actional stage) probably benefit most from interventions supporting the implementation and initiation of this intention, e.g., detailed behavioral planning. Individuals who have already implemented a new behavior (postaction stage) benefit from interventions that provide feedback on the behavioral consequences achieved in maintaining this new behavior. At the same time, these interventions help reduce relapses.

4 Using the web-based TaTT for systematically linking SSBC MoAs and BCTs

For applying the TaTT, in the first step, we have to translate the construct labels used by the SSBC into the more general MoA labels used by the TaTT. In the second step, for the SSBC constructs thus translated into the TaTT “language,” we used the TaTT app to select MoA-BCT links. This selection process was steered by two criteria: the links should be supported by empirical evidence, and the BCTs should be potentially applicable within the context of developing an app for promoting heating energy-saving behaviors.

4.1 Method

The selection process was conducted by the two authors. In the first step, each author separately decided how to translate the SSBC constructs into the MoA labels used by the TaTT. Then, disagreements were discussed and solved. In the second step, each author separately searched the TaTT for evidence-based links between the MoAs included in the SSBC and specific BCTs. In three iterative rounds, the authors compared their results, discussed disagreements, tried to solve them, and then repeated their rating.

4.2 Results

Columns 1 and 2 of Table 1 report the SSBC stages and the names of the stage-specific SSBC constructs selected as targets of the planned app. Column 3 presents the results of the first step, which involves translating the SSBC construct into the names of the respective TaTT-MoAs. Column 4 presents the results of the evidence-based MoA-BCT links resulting from information reported in the TaTT. As can be seen in Table 1, column 4, from this procedure for persons in the predecision stage, a package includes the following four BCTs ‘5.3. Information about social and environmental consequences’, ‘1.6. Discrepancy between current behavior and goal’, ‘6.2. Social comparison’, and ‘1.3. Goal setting (outcome)’ results. This BCT package is linked with the three MoAs ‘social norms’, ‘perceived negative consequences of own behavior’, and ‘goal intention to reduce heating energy consumption’, postulated by the SBCC as important MoAs for promoting in the predecisional stage doubts about the adequacy of current heating behaviors.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Using the TaTT for linking MoAs derived from the SSBC with specific BCTs.

For persons in the preaction stage, a package including the six BCTs ‘5.3. Information about social and environmental consequences’, ‘4.1. Instruction on how to perform behavior’, ‘6.1. Demonstration of the behavior’, ‘9.1. Credible source’, ‘1.1. Goal setting (behavior)’, and ‘1.9 Commitment’ is rated as linked with the three MoAs ‘attitude toward energy saving behavioral options’, ‘perceived behavioral control over energy saving behavioral options’, and ‘behavioral intention to perform energy saving behavioral options’, postulated by the SBCC as important MoAs in the preaction stage for promoting participants’ intention to change their current heating behaviors.

For persons in the action stage, a package including the nine BCTs ‘1.2. Problem solving’, ‘1.4 Action planning’, ‘9.3 Comparative imagining of future outcomes’, ‘4.1. Instruction on how to perform behavior’, ‘6.1. Demonstration of the behavior’, ‘2.2. Feedback on behavior’, ‘1.6. Discrepancy between current behavior and goal ‘, ‘7.1. Prompts/cues’, ‘3.2 Social support (practical)’ is rated as linked with the three MoAs ‘action planning’, ‘cognitive planning’, and ‘maintenance self-efficacy’ postulated by the SBCC as important MoAs for implementing the intended behavioral change process.

For persons in the postaction stage, a package including the eight BCTs ‘1.6. Discrepancy between current behavior and goal’, ‘2.2 Feedback on behavior’, ‘2.7 Feedback on outcome(s) of behavior’, ‘6.2. Social comparison’, ‘10.10. Reward (outcome)’; ‘1.5. Review behavior goal(s)’, ‘1.7. Review outcome goal(s)’, and ‘15.3. Focus on past success’ is rated as linked with the three MoAs ‘maintenance self-efficacy’, ‘recovery self-efficacy’, and ‘evaluation of outcomes associated with adopted new behaviors’ postulated by the SBCC as important MoAs for evaluating and maintaining the behavioral change process.

5 Developing design ideas for operationalizing BCTs implemented in the app

As stated above, operationalizing a specific BCT in the context of a concrete intervention project is primarily a creative task, however, based on knowledge of the scientific intervention literature. In the present case, this task consists of translating the generally defined BCTs into concrete ideas of how to operationalize them in the context of an app-based intervention. This should be done in such a way that they are actually able to change the constructs assumed to underlie participants’ current heating behavior and their intentions to change this behavior and/or maintain this change.

5.1 Method

In the present project, we started the creative process of developing BCT operationalizations by searching the empirical intervention literature for examples of already existing BCT operationalizations in the field of pro-environmental behaviors (e.g., Abrahamse, 2019, 2020). Before this knowledge background, we conducted a creative ideas producing brainstorming workshop with the whole research group. Three members of the research group are experienced intervention developers.

5.2 Results

In Table 2, columns 5 and 6 document the results of this brainstorming process. For example, we operationalize the BCT ‘5.3.1 Information about social and environmental consequences’ taken from the TaTT in the context of heating energy saving as ‘Show the CO2 emissions of the own energy consumption’. The creative solution to implement this BCT in an app is ‘Line graphs show the own heating energy consumption in equivalent CO2 emissions’.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Assignment of main menu buttons to SSBC stage and BCTs.

6 Testing stage-specific preferences for different intervention packages

In the following sections, we present a laboratory-based experimental approach that allows us to test the empirical validity of theoretically derived intervention design ideas during the intervention development phase. As stated in Section 2, in the case of this study, we conducted a lab experiment with the aim to test the central SSBC idea that a person’s stage membership determines their preference for specific intervention elements. Specifically, this means that we need a design idea of how we can make it clear to the user on the app’s home screen which stage-specific intervention packages are included in the app. Against the background of the SSBC, Sunio and Schmöcker (2019) assume that users then select the app element that they believe provides the information relevant to their current situation (stage membership). The central task of app intervention developers is, therefore, to develop a design idea that signals to potential users in a simple and clear way what kind of information/services they can expect from selecting a particular app element. If empirically validated, the advantage of this approach would be that no complex preliminary classification is required when conducting a stage-based intervention study. Instead, users are provided with stage-specific interventions from which they can select the one that best suits their needs.

6.1 The design idea of how to signal the stage-specific significance of intervention elements

Figure 3 shows the conceptual design idea developed to test this theoretical idea. The six buttons “My Energy Consumption,” “Energy Saving Tips,” “Set Energy Saving Goal,” “My Goal Progress,” “Compare with Neighbors,” and “Plan Energy Saving Behavior” are intended to provide users with the information that they can use to decide whether the intervention elements summarized under one button meet their stage-specific needs.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. App home screen with the virtual agent Floka and the main menu.

Furthermore, a virtual agent named Floka is used to introduce the user to the functions summarized under a button. Table 3 reports which BCTs the virtual agent mentions, when presenting and explaining the six app buttons (in the appendix the dialogs are documented, the German speaking video could be seen under https://youtu.be/Oe2VH8d2RMY).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Items used for stage classification.

6.2 Hypotheses

From the SSBC, we derive the hypotheses on how stage membership determines persons’ preference for the six app buttons:

• H1. Persons assigned to the predecision stage prefer most frequently the buttons ‘My energy consumption’ (H1a), ‘Comparison with neighbors’ (H1b), and ‘Set energy saving goal’ (H1c).

• H2. Persons assigned to the preaction stage prefer most frequently the button ‘Energy saving tips’.

• H3. Persons assigned to the action stage prefer most frequently the buttons ‘Plan energy saving behavior’ (H3a) and ‘My goal progress’ (H3b).

• H4. Persons assigned to the postaction stage prefer most frequently the buttons ‘My goal progress’ (H4a) and ‘Comparison with neighbors’ (H4b).

6.3 Method

6.3.1 Procedure and measures

Data collection followed APA guidelines for the ethical conduct of research and included informed consent. After reading the introduction explaining the study’s aim (evaluating the relevance and attractiveness of a planned intervention aiming to motivate participants to save energy and related information), participants first reported their age and gender. Then, they completed a measure used for classifying their current stage membership. In the next step, they viewed the above-presented video explaining the six app buttons with the help of the virtual agent Floka. In the final step, participants filled out three measurement instruments that we used to record participants’ preferences for the six app buttons shown in the video. These three preference measures consist of one forced choice task and two Likert scales asking participants to rank the perceived personal relevance as well as the attractiveness of the six app buttons presented in the video.

The measure for classifying participants’ stage membership was developed originally by Bamberg (2013) in the context of reducing car use. For the present study, this measure was adapted to the domain of heating energy saving. The measure consists of a forced-choice task asking participants to choose from the five statements presented in Table 3, the one that best describes their current heating behavior and their plans for the future. For example, participants who select item 1 or 2 are assigned to the predecisional stage.

After completing the stage classification task, participants viewed the video and completed one control item, assuring that participants have actually observed the video (‘In the video you just watched: What is the name of the character who spoke in the energy saving app?’). Participants were only included in the study if they answered this attention control question correctly. As a final step, all participants answered the three preference measures. The first measure consists of a forced choice task: ‘Which of the six buttons presented in the video would you choose if you were using this app in your current situation? Please select the feature that you find most helpful in your current situation.’ Participants then could select only one of the presented six app buttons ‘My current energy consumption’, ‘Energy saving tips’, ‘Set an energy saving goal’, ‘My goal progress’, ‘Comparison with neighbors’, and ‘Plan energy saving behavior’. The other two measures asked participants to judge the perceived personal relevance (Not relevant at all = 1; very relevant = 5) and perceived personal attractiveness (Not attractive at all = 1; very attractive = 5) of the same six app buttons.

6.3.2 Experimental context

The online experiment for testing the hypotheses was conducted in December 2021. In December 2021, not only did rents continue to rise in Germany but also the prices for natural gas, which is used to heat 70% of all German households. Therefore, tenants should be particularly interested in saving heating costs. December 2021, on the other hand, was significantly too warm in Germany (up to 17 degrees at the end of the month), which may have reduced interest in energy-saving tips.

6.3.3 Participants

The participants in the experiment came from the Germany-wide access panel of a commercial online market research institute. Because the experiment was planned as a pre-study of a main study with tenants as the target group, only tenants were surveyed in the online experiment. The specific place of residence was not recorded. To be accepted as a study participant, a person has to be older than 17 years and has to live in a rented flat. All included participants fulfill these two eligibility criteria. Participation in the survey was compensated with €2.50. During the sampling process, the measure presented in Table 3 was used for classifying participants into the four SSBC stages. The goal was to acquire an equal number of participants for each of the four stages, large enough (n = 120) to have enough statistical power to conduct pairwise tests. For the SBCC stages ‘predecision’ and ‘postaction’, this goal was reached quickly. Consequently, we stopped acquiring participants assigned to these two stages and included only participants classified as being in the preaction and action stages. Using this procedure results in a total sample of 507 participants: 131 (25.9%) of the 507 participants were assigned to the pre-decisional stage, 129 (25.4%) to the pre-actional stage, 121 (23.4%) to the actional stage, and 126 (24.9%) to the post-actional stage. Of the 507 participants, 264 define themselves as female and two (0.4%) as neither female nor male. The average age is 49 years (median, range 18–86 years, SD = 14.9 years).

6.4 Results

6.4.1 Results of the forced choice preference task

Separately for the four SSBC stage groups, Table 4 presents participants’ decisions in the forced choice task (‘Please select the feature that you find most helpful in your current situation.’).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Stage-dependent preference for the app buttons (force choice task, absolute number in brackets).

Differences in frequencies reported in the 6×4 contingency table were analyzed using the ꭓ2 test. The test results indicate only the choice of two app buttons ‘My current energy consumption’ [χ2(3) = 10.17, p < 0.05] and ‘Energy saving tips’ [χ2(3) = 15,20, p < 0.05] significant differences between the four stage groups: Compared to the other three stage groups, participants diagnosed as belonging to the predecisional stage choose most frequently the app button ‘My current energy consumption’. Compared to the other three stage groups, participants diagnosed as belonging to the preactional stage choose most frequently the app button ‘Energy saving tips’. These results provide some empirical support for H1a and H2. All other six (sub)hypotheses concerning stage-specific button preferences, however, are not empirically supported by the forced choice task.

In the next step, we analyzed how participants perceived the personal relevance and attractiveness of the six app buttons. Explanatory factor analysis indicates that both measures used for assessing the relevance as well as attractiveness are unidimensional (promax method, 55.4% common variance of relevance scale, 60.6% common variance of attractiveness scale) and consistent (Cronbach’s α of relevance measure = 0.82; of attractiveness measure = 0.85). Furthermore, participants’ ratings of the relevance and attractiveness of the app buttons correlate quite high: For the button ‘Set an energy saving goal’, the correlation of both aspects is the lowest (r = 0.70) and for the aspect ‘Comparison with neighbors’, it is the highest (r = 0.82). However, additional factor analysis does not confirm that both scales load on a common dimension. For this reason, we analyze both measures separately.

Table 5 presents separately for the four stage groups participants perceived personal relevance of the six app buttons.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Perceived relevance for the app buttons for the four stage groups separately.

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to compare the perceived relevance of the six app buttons across the four SSBC stages. The multivariate result was significant for the SSBC stage, Pillai’s Trace = 0.131, F = 3.79, df = (18,1500), p < 0.01, indicating significantly different means of the perceived relevance of the six buttons across the four SSBC stages. The univariate F tests showed that there was a significant difference between the stages for the buttons ‘My current energy consumption’, F = 14.93, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘Energy saving tips’, F = 9.11, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘Set an energy saving goal’, F = 4.81, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘My goal progress’, F = 5.74, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, and ‘Plan energy saving behavior’, F = 3.97, df = (3,503), p < 0.01. Only for the button ‘Comparison with neighbors’, univariate F test is insignificant, F = 1.76, df = (3,503), p = 0.15. However, for the five buttons with univariate F tests, post-hoc Bonferroni corrected pairwise tests indicate only significant differences between the predecisional stage and all other three stage groups. The means of the three stage groups, preactional, actional, and postactional, do not differ significantly. Summarizing, the analyses show that compared to the other three stage groups, people assigned to the predecisional stage rate the perceived personal relevance of all offered app buttons as the lowest. Furthermore, the analyses provide no empirical support for the expected differences in the perceived personal relevance of the offered app buttons between the other three stage groups.

Table 6 presents separately for the four stage groups the mean personal attractiveness of the app buttons perceived by the participants.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Perceived attractiveness of the app buttons for the four stage groups separately.

Again, a MANOVA was used to compare the perceived attractiveness of the six app buttons across the four SSBC stages. The multivariate result was significant for the SSBC stage, Pillai’s Trace = 0.105, F = 3.02, df = (18,1500), p < 0.01, indicating significantly different means of the perceived relevance of the six buttons across the four SSBC stages. The univariate F tests showed that there was a significant difference between the stages for the buttons ‘My current energy consumption’, F = 10.28, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘Energy saving tips’, F = 7.80, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘Set an energy saving goal’, F = 5.50, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, ‘My goal progress’, F = 7.61, df = (3,503), p < 0.01, and ‘Plan energy saving behavior’, F = 4.62, df = (3,503), p < 0.01. Again for the button ‘Comparison with neighbors’, the univariate F test is insignificant, F = 0.75, df = (3,503), p = 0.52. Furthermore, the post-hoc Bonferroni corrected pairwise tests of the stage group means again indicate only significant differences between the predecisional stage and all other three stage groups. The means of the three stage groups, preactional, actional, and postactional, however, do not differ significantly again.

7 Discussion

This study aimed to systematically research the following three questions: (1) Could the stage-specific SSBC constructs be translated into the TaTT-MoA labels so that the TaTT can be used to identify evidence-based MoA-BCT connections? (2) How can the generally defined BCTs be translated into concrete intervention elements? (3) How can we test whether these specific intervention elements work as theoretically expected? Concerning the first question, we found that the SSBC constructs could be quite easily translated into the TaTT-MoA labels. Perhaps because the TaTT-MoA labels and definitions were extracted from 83 behavioral theories, their definitions also fit the more specific SSBC constructs. From our perspective, it would also be a real advance in environmental psychology research if we could agree on the use of consistent construct names and definitions. Often, the same names are used for differently defined constructs, or different names are used for similarly defined constructs. This inconsistency represents a barrier that should not be underestimated for the cumulative development of evaluation research in particular. In our view, however, the MoAs contained in the TaTT need to be supplemented. For example, MoAs that can be derived from social identity theory, such as social identity, group-based emotions, or collective effectiveness (Fritsche et al., 2018), are missing. Furthermore, with anticipated regret, only one emotion-related construct is included in the TaTT. In environmental psychology, many more emotions are discussed as potential MoAs, such as guilt, shame, anger, pride, joy, or being moved (e.g., Taufik and Venhoeven, 2018). A second major advantage of the TaTT is the systematic, evidence-based connection of MoAs with BCTs. Here, too, it was relatively easy for us to identify the evidence-based BCTs in the TaTT that should lead to a change in the respective SSBC constructs. In our view, environmental psychology has not yet recognized the great importance of identifying such evidence-based MoA-BCT relationships for the systematic development of evaluation research. On the one hand, there is no widely accepted classification of BCTs in environmental psychology. On the other hand, the work examining the effectiveness of various intervention techniques (e.g., Varotto and Spagnolli, 2017) focuses on their direct behavioral effect. This focus on direct behavioral effects has the disadvantage that the question of which MoA a specific intervention technique works through remains open. In our view, however, evidence-based knowledge about behavior-MoA-BCT links is important to better understand the processes on which behavior change is based. To remedy this deficit in environmental psychology intervention research, a project similar to the HBCP or SOBC project in health psychology would be very helpful.

It is interesting that the second question of how to operationalize the generally defined BCTs into concrete intervention elements is not explicitly discussed even within the HBCP literature. This is unfortunate because the question of how to operationalize abstractly defined BCTs in the context of a concrete intervention project is central. Here too, systematic compilations of already existing BCT operationalizations, as well as their critical, theoretical, and practical evaluations, are missing. Furthermore, there is little information in the literature about the creative process through which new BCT operationalizations are developed. We have not found any explanations of how to proceed methodically or how to empirically evaluate the new operationalizations before they are used in the main studies. For this reason, we explicitly described the creative development process of new BCT operationalizations in our study.

Consequently, in answering the third question, “How to test whether these specific intervention elements work as theoretically expected?,” we demonstrated how a newly developed BCT operationalization could be empirically validated in a laboratory experiment before its use in the main intervention study. The new intervention design idea consisted of a video in which a virtual agent presented and explained six app buttons summarizing different intervention packages. The conducted online study focused on empirically testing the assumption that a person’s stage membership determines their preference for the six app buttons in a theoretically expected way. However, the empirical results provided only limited support for this theoretical hypothesis: only the participants diagnosed as belonging to the predecisional stage showed the theoretically expected preference for the two app buttons ‘My energy consumption’ and ‘Energy saving tips’. Moreover, as expected, participants in this stage generally rated the personal relevance and attractiveness of all six app buttons significantly lower than participants assigned to the other three stages. This result raises doubt about whether the newly developed intervention design idea works sufficiently well. There is a high probability that the implementation of the described app-based intervention in a real, expensive intervention study would result in the finding ‘no clear intervention effect’. One possible explanation for this failure might be that dialogs used for presenting the kind of information behind the app buttons were not able to convince especially participants in the last two stages that this information fits their needs and has personal relevance for them. So, we have to return to the design table and have to develop a new, hopefully better intervention idea. Summarizing, our study results underline that the development of a theory-based intervention is obviously a complex and iterative design–evaluation–redesign–evaluation circle.

7.1 Limitations

We want to mention that the reliability of the discussed results of our experimental design idea test depends on the validity of our stage diagnosis. It consists of five statements from which the participant must choose one. That means that, in fact, the diagnosis is based on the selection of one item. The validity of this approach has not yet been adequately tested. Furthermore, one can criticize the artificiality of the procedure of presenting the app and Floka via a video.

7.2 Future research

The framework concept presented for systematic theory-driven intervention development can and should stimulate a wide range of further studies. For example, the reviewers criticize that our study does not test the Behavior-MoA-BCT chain itself. To do this, we would have to carry out a study that, in addition to evaluating the intervention design, also contains appropriate items to measure the MoAs (SSBC constructs) before and after the intervention and, if possible, a behavioral measure.

Future studies should also consider the following alternative explanations suggested by one reviewer for the non-hypothesis-consistent experimental results: Especially tenants consistently expressed their powerlessness and lack of control over actions and decisions relevant to heating and energy behaviors. This helplessness may be a reason why, in the experiment, participants were not motivated to process the provided information. Furthermore, the inconsistent experimental results could reflect participants’ interest in learning all the features of the new Floka app. From this perspective, the inconsistent results could reflect the effect of novelty or curiosity.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans because we performed an online study with participants of an online access panel. Participants register to participate in online studies. The description of the online study included the informed consent statement. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MB: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SB: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was conducted as part of the interdisciplinary project ‘ENVIRON’, funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF), Germany. Funded by Hochschule Bielefeld - University of Applied Sciences and Arts for the article processing charge.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all those who scientifically or practically supported our project. Special thanks go to our partners in the project ‘ENVIRON.’

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1534014/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

Abrahamse, W. (2019). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: What works, what doesn't, and why. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Abrahamse, W. (2020). How to effectively encourage sustainable food choices: a mini-review of available evidence. Front. Psychol. 11:589674. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.589674

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamberg, S. (2013). Changing environmentally harmful behaviors: a stage model of self-regulated behavioral change. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cane, J., O’Connor, D., and Michie, S. (2012). Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement. Sci. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-7-37

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, R. N., Connell, L. E., Johnston, M., Rothman, A. J., De Bruin, M., Kelly, M. P., et al. (2019). Behavior change techniques and their mechanisms of action: a synthesis of links described in published intervention literature. Ann. Behav. Med. 53, 693–707. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Connell, L. E., Carey, R. N., De Bruin, M., Rothman, A. J., Johnston, M., Kelly, M. P., et al. (2019). Links between behavior change techniques and mechanisms of action: an expert consensusstudy. Ann. Behav. Med. 53, 708–720. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R., Campbell, R., Hildon, Z., Hobbs, L., and Michie, S. (2014). Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: a scoping review. Health Psychol. Rev. 9, 323–344. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.941722

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dombrowski, S. U., Sniehotta, F. F., Avenell, A., Johnston, M., MacLennan, G., and Araújo-Soares, V. (2012). Identifying active ingredients in complex behavioural interventions for obese adults with obesity-related co-morbidities or additional risk factors for co-morbidities: a systematic review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2012, 7–32. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.513298

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsche, I., Barth, M., Jugert, P., Masson, T., and Reese, G. (2018). A social identity model of pro-environmental action (SIMPEA). Psychol. Rev. 125, 245–269. doi: 10.1037/rev0000090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. Am. Psychol. 54, 493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases P. LangeVan, A. W. Kruglanski, and E. T. Higgins Handbook of theories of social psychology 526–545 London, UK Sage Publications

Google Scholar

Ivanova, D., Barrett, J., Wiedenhofer, D., Macura, B., Callaghan, M., and Creutzig, F. (2020). Quantifying the potential for climate change mitigation of consumption options. Environ. Res. Lett. 15:093001. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/ab8589

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, M., Carey, R. N., Bohlen, L. C., Johnston, D. W., Rothman, A. J., Bruin, M. D., et al. (2020). Development of an online tool for linking behavior change techniques and mechanisms of action based on triangulation of findings from literature synthesis and expert consensus). Transl. Behav. Med. 11, 1049–1065. doi: 10.1093/tbm/ibaa050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, A., Eisen, C., and Hanss, D. (2019). Lessons learned from applications of the stage model of self-regulated behavioral change: a review. Front. Psychol. 10:1091. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01091

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, S., Atkins, L., and West, R. (2014). The Behaviour Change Wheel: A Guide to Designing Interventions. Great Britain: Silverback Publishing.

Google Scholar

Michie, S., Carey, R. N., Johnston, M., Rothman, A. J., De Bruin, M., Kelly, M. P., et al. (2018). From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: a protocol for developing and testing a methodology for linking behaviour change techniques to theoretical mechanisms of action. Ann. Behav. Med. 52, 501–512. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9816-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, S., Carey, R., Johnston, M., Rothman, A. J., Kelly, M., Davidson, K., et al. (2016). From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: linking behaviour change techniques to their mechanisms of action. Eur. Health Psychol. 18:395.

Google Scholar

Michie, S., Richardson, M., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., et al. (2013). The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann. Behav. Med. 46, 81–95. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Michie, S., Thomas, J., Johnston, M., Aonghusa, P. M., Shawe-Taylor, J., Kelly, M. P., et al. (2017). The human behaviour-change project: harnessing the power of artificial intelligence and machine learning for evidence synthesis and interpretation. Implement. Sci. 12, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s13012-017-0641-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisa, C. F., Bélanger, J. J., Schumpe, B. M., and Faller, D. G. (2019). Meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials testing behavioural interventions to promote household action on climate change. Nat. Commun. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12457-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., and Velicer, W. F. (1997). The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. Am. J. Health Promot. 12, 38–48. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-12.1.38

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H., and Howard, J. A. (1981). “A normative decision-making model of altruism” in Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives. ed. J. P. Rushton (Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum), 189–211.

Google Scholar

Stern, P. C. (2020). A reexamination on how behavioral interventions can promote household action to limit climate change. Nat. Commun. 11, –3.

Google Scholar

Sumner, J. A., Carey, R. N., Michie, S., Johnston, M., Edmondson, D., and Davidson, K. W. (2018). Using rigorous methods to advance behaviour change science. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 797–799. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0471-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunio, V., and Schmöcker, J.-D. (2019). Implementing stage based models of behavioral change as menu-based intervention. J. Eastern Asia Society Transport. Studies. 13, 427–441.

Google Scholar

Taufik, D., and Venhoeven, L. (2018). “Emotions and pro-environmental behaviour” in Environmental psychology. eds L. Steg and J. I. M. Groot.

Google Scholar

van der Linden, S., and Goldberg, M. H. (2020). Alternative meta-analysis of behavioral interventions to promote action on climate change yields different conclusions. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–2. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17613-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Varotto, A., and Spagnolli, A. (2017). Psychological strategies to promote household recycling. A systematic review with meta-analysis of validated field interventions. J. Environ. Psychol. 51, 168–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.03.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R. (2005). Time for a change: putting the Transtheoretical (stages of change) model to rest. Addiction 100, 1036–1039. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01139.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stage model of self-regulated behavioral change, virtual agent, theory-based intervention development, mechanisms of action, behavior change techniques, theory and technique tool, menu-based, energy-saving behavior

Citation: Brandt M and Bamberg S (2025) Combining mechanisms of action with behavior change techniques—theory-based development of an app promoting heating energy-saving behaviors. Front. Psychol. 16:1534014. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1534014

Received: 25 November 2024; Accepted: 29 May 2025;
Published: 18 June 2025.

Edited by:

Giuseppe Carrus, Roma Tre University, Italy

Reviewed by:

Dimitrios Xenias, Cardiff University, United Kingdom
Xochitl Cruz-Núñez, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico
Kristin Konnyu, University of Aberdeen, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Brandt and Bamberg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sebastian Bamberg, c2ViYXN0aWFuLmJhbWJlcmdAaHNiaS5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.