ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 May 2025

Sec. Evolutionary Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1541836

Gray hair influences perceived age and social perceptions

  • 1Department of Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
  • 2Munsell Color Science Laboratory, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States

Physical traits can influence how people are perceived and evaluated by others, often reflecting underlying qualities considered important for social interaction. Gray hair color is one such trait that can potentially alter social perceptions related to aging, but has rarely been investigated independently from other correlated physical characteristics. The aim of the current work is to investigate how gray hair independently influences important social evaluations including perceived age, attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, and trustworthiness. Participants (N = 120) were presented with images of male and female faces that exhibited non-gray hair (brown, blonde or red), and versions of the same faces manipulated to have gray hair, and were asked to rate these faces according to those social evaluations. Linear mixed-effects models indicated that faces with gray hair were perceived as older and less attractive. Men (but not women) also perceived faces with gray hair as less trustworthy. Results showed that gray hair did not impact assessments of social status or aggression. These results suggest that gray hair is independently used as an indicator of some important social evaluations (age, attractiveness, and trustworthiness), while others (social status and aggression) may be better informed by other characteristics.

1 Introduction

People readily categorize others into groups, an adaptive process allowing us to rapidly organize our complex social environment (Macrae et al., 1994). Perceptions of age are among the first categorizations that are formed when encountering someone (Cuddy and Fiske, 2002; Fiske, 1998). People perceived as being older are often characterized as having high warmth (e.g., friendly, good-natured, sincere and warm) but low competence (e.g., capable, competent, confident, and skillful; Fiske et al., 2002). Perceptions of age can be predicted by physical traits. For example, wrinkles, age spots, arcus senilis and pattern baldness tend to be perceived as indictors of older age (Bulpitt et al., 2001; Gunn et al., 2009). Gray hair is another physical trait that is often associated with aging (Bulpitt et al., 2001; Gunn et al., 2009).

Gray hair becomes more prevalent with increasing age (Bulpitt et al., 2001; Gunn et al., 2009; Keogh and Walsh, 1965; Panhard et al., 2012), with a significant increase in gray hair between the ages of 45 and 60 years old (O’Sullivan et al., 2020; Panhard et al., 2012). Similar to other physiological changes associated with aging that involve declines in the ability of tissues to maintain homeostasis and regenerate new tissue, gray hair develops when melanocytes cannot maintain homeostasis and replenish melanin (the pigment that colors new hair; Nishimura, 2011; Sarin and Artandi, 2007). Gray hair may therefore be an honest indicator of biological aging (Jawor and Breitwisch, 2003). It is also possible for hair to turn gray in younger people; such premature graying of hair may result from genetic predispositions, environmental factors, or psychological stress (Pandhi and Khanna, 2013).

From an evolutionary perspective on human mate selection (Buss, 1996), men generally seek women with high reproductive potential, a status that is reinforced by qualities including youth and health. Conversely, women tend to seek men who have high social status. These qualities are associated with aging, as reproductive potential in women and social status in men declines with advanced age (Conroy-Beam and Buss, 2019). Therefore, observable characteristics associated with aging likely convey social information that influences inferences related to mate quality and competition (Buss and Schmitt, 2019), even when mate seeking is not an imminent goal. Such age-related signals may be conveyed indirectly through observable characteristics like hair color (Sorokowski, 2008; Fink et al., 2016). Because decreased hair quality, including graying, is linked to aging (Bulpitt et al., 2001), it can be inferred as a signal for reduced reproductive capability in women (Hinsz et al., 2001), decreased health status (Mesko and Bereczkei, 2004), and poorer social status in men. Thus, these inferences are likely to be reflected in evaluations of perceived age, but also in perceptions related to age, such as attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, and trustworthiness.

Hair color has indeed been shown to influence social perceptions (Aberg et al., 2020; Barrett, 2003; Beddow, 2011; Feinman and Gill, 1978; Kyle and Mahler, 1996; Swami and Barrett, 2011). Both men and women are perceived as being older when they have more gray hair (Bulpitt et al., 2001; Gunn et al., 2009). Based on questionnaires and qualitative reports, women perceive gray hair as unattractive and associated with poor health and social disengagement (Clarke and Korotchenko, 2010; Ward and Holland, 2010). Several studies have suggested that men and women could potentially leverage their gray hair as a sign of credibility and competence (Isopahkala-Bouret, 2016; Mitra et al., 2014; Ward and Holland, 2010). While the above studies provide important insight into the effects of gray hair, we are unaware of any experimental studies that have specifically examined the effects of gray hair on perceived age and social perceptions, independently of other correlated qualities.

1.1 Current research

We therefore aimed to experimentally examine the influence of gray hair on perceived age and social perceptions. Because gray hair is more common with older age (Keogh and Walsh, 1965; Panhard et al., 2012), we predicted that men and women with gray hair would be perceived as older. Previous research has demonstrated that men and women consider older faces to be less attractive (Foos and Clark, 2011; He et al., 2021; Kwart et al., 2012) and less trustworthy (Pehlivanoglu et al., 2022; Zebrowitz et al., 2013; but see Fiske et al., 2002) than younger faces. In addition, male faces that are older are considered less aggressive (Richardson et al., 2020) and have lower social status (plateauing at approximately 35 years old; Batres et al., 2015; Mueller and Mazur, 1996; Roll and Verinis, 1971). Because no previous studies have experimentally assessed whether gray hair impacts social perceptions, we decided to focus our study on social perceptions that are commonly explored in face perception studies: attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, and trustworthiness (e.g., Carré et al., 2009; Goetz et al., 2013; Jones et al., 2021; Little et al., 2011; Stirrat and Perrett, 2010; Sutherland et al., 2013). Assuming gray hair is an indicator of older age, we predicted that faces with gray hair would be rated as less attractive, less aggressive (men only), lower social status (men only), and less trustworthy, than those with non-gray hair. We tested these predictions by presenting men and women with photographs of faces exhibiting non-gray (brown, blonde or red) or gray hair. The subjects evaluated these photographs based on perceived age as well as other related social perceptions (attractiveness, social status, aggression, and trustworthiness).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

One hundred and twenty (60 men and 60 women) people participated in this study at Texas A&M University from September 2022 through December 2023. They were between the ages of 18 and 43 years old (mean ± SE: 23 ± 0.4 years old). 97 and 93% of the men and women participants, respectively, were 30 years old or younger. Most of the participants (80%) indicated that they were heterosexual. The self-reported ethnic group of the participants was Caucasian (36%), Asian (36%), Hispanic (12%), multi-racial (6%), African American (6%), and unspecified (5%). Most of the female participants (73%) had not taken hormonal contraceptives within 3 months prior to participating. We recruited the participants through emails. The participants were told that they would be participating in a study that explored face perception and they each earned $15.

2.2 Experimental stimuli and procedure

The experimental stimuli consisted of photographs of men and women that exhibited non-gray hair (blonde, brown, or red) and gray hair. The stimuli of people with non-gray hair were selected from a face database (Chelnokova et al., 2014; Oslo Face Database) and we obtained permission to use these stimuli in our study. The face database included photographs of students from the University of Oslo (Oslo, Norway). We created the stimuli of people with gray hair by modifying the stimuli with non-gray hair. We did so by creating a color mask using Matlab (Matlab, 2022) and Photoshop (Adobe Systems, USA), so that color manipulations were constrained only to hair on the head while keeping color unchanged across the rest of the image. In Matlab, we converted the sRGB image into CIELab color space and then multiplied the lightness of each image (L*) by a scalar (1.2) while setting the other color variables (a* and b*) to zero (removing the chromatic content of the hair). For images in which the original hair color was very light, we first subtracted 10 units from L* before applying the scalar so that the hair color would not appear ‘washed out’; conversely, for images in which the original hair color was very dark, we first added 5 or 10 units to L* before applying the scalar so that the hair color was not too dark. This step of adding or subtracting units of L* was determined subjectively but resulted in hair that we qualitatively assessed as appearing naturally gray. This process resulted in two versions of the same face: one in which the hair color was the original (non-gray) color and one in which the hair color was gray (Figure 1). The final stimuli consisted of 140 faces (faces of 35 men and 35 women with non-gray hair, and faces of the same people with gray hair) and a summary of their hair colors is included in Table 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of face stimuli displaying a male with (A) non-gray and (B) gray hair color. The man in the stimuli gave informed consent for his photographs to be used for scientific purposes (Chelnokova et al., 2014, reproduced with permission from the Oslo Face Database, https://affectivebrains.com/oslo-face-database/). Face stimuli displaying a woman are not shown due to lack of consent for their images being used as examples in publications.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of hair colors in the experimental stimuli.

The subjects viewed the stimuli on a computer monitor (EIZO FlexScan EV2451) while resting their chins in a chin cup (UHCOTech HeadSpot). Their gaze was monitored with an eye-tracker (Tobii Pro Spectrum) but those data are not reported here. We used custom Matlab scripts to program the experiments (Niehorster et al., 2020). The monitor was color calibrated at least once every 2 weeks (Calibrite ColorChecker Display Pro; luminance: 120 cd/m2; gamma = 2.2; white point CCT = 6,500 K). Each subject evaluated all of the face stimuli (n = 140) twice: they assessed the faces based on social perceptions and perceived age. The social perceptions included evaluations of attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, or trustworthiness. Social status was defined as how likely the person in the image was to have a high-ranking social position and command respect over other people in the community (Dixson and Vasey, 2012). A given subject only evaluated the stimuli based on one of the four social perceptions. The order of the evaluations (social perception or perceived age) was randomized across subjects and the subjects took a two-minute break between them. Thirty subjects (15 men and 15 women) participated in each of the four social perception experiments (attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, or trustworthiness) for a total of 120 subjects. The sample size was similar to other studies on social perception (Yorzinski and Platt, 2010; Fink et al., 2016). The same experimenter (K.N.) collected all of the data.

Each face stimulus was displayed for 5 s (the same duration used in other studies of social perception; e.g., Bowdring et al., 2021; Walla et al., 2020; Yorzinski and Platt, 2010) and then an assessment screen appeared (the face was not visible during the assessment screen). The subjects indicated their assessment of the preceding face (via a mouse click) before viewing the next face. The social perception assessment was scored on a scale of 1 to 10, with text above the numbers to remind subjects of the scale. Depending on the experiment, above the number one, it stated “least attractive,” “lowest social status,” “least aggressive,” or “least trustworthy;” above the number 10, it stated “most attractive,” “highest social status,” “most aggressive,” or “most trustworthy.” The perceived age assessment options ranged from 18 to 77 years old. In rare cases (45/33,600 stimuli; 0.13%), the subject did not click on a social perception or age score for a stimulus (i.e., they clicked on a blank spot within the assessment screen) and the data for such stimuli were therefore not included in the analysis. The face stimuli were displayed in blocks of male or female faces; block order was randomized across subjects. The face stimuli within each block were also randomized across subjects except that the faces of the same individuals (with different hair colors) were never shown consecutively. In summary, each subject evaluated the faces of 35 women and 35 men (both the gray hair and non-gray hair versions of each face, for a total of 140 faces) based on social perceptions (attractiveness, social status, aggressiveness, or trustworthiness) and perceived age. After finishing the experiment, each subject competed the Aging Perceptions Questionnaire (APQ; Barker et al., 2007). The APQ evaluates aging perceptions along seven domains. An overall score on the APQ is calculated by summing the scores across all domains (after reverse scoring ‘Consequences Positive’ and ‘Control Positive’; Fawsitt et al., 2022), with higher scores indicating more negative perceptions of aging.

2.3 Measurements and statistical analysis

We analyzed our assessment data using linear mixed-effects models with repeated measures in SAS (PROC MIXED; Version 9.4; SAS Institute Inc.). The dependent variables were the perceived age or social perception scores. The independent variables were the hair color of the people in the stimuli (‘stimuli hair color’: non-gray or gray), gender of the face stimuli (‘stimuli gender’), gender of the subject (‘subject gender’), and their interactions as well as the subjects’ overall score on the APQ (‘perception of aging’), the interaction between their overall score on the APQ and hair color of the people in the stimuli, and subject age. Subject identity was included within the models to account for repeated measures. Stimuli identity was also included within the models to account for repeated measures because the models used the raw scores from each subject for each stimuli rather than averages. We also performed four comparisons within each model of social perception to compare the influence of gray hair on both face stimuli and subject gender; we used a Bonferroni correction to evaluate statistical significance. Raw data are included in the Supplementary material. We also reran the above statistical models on a dataset that excluded short and long reaction times (we excluded any stimuli in which the subject took less than 1.01 s or more than 4.27 s to indicate their score, which represent the lower and upper 5% of the latency data); the results were qualitatively the same as the full dataset and are reported in the Supplementary Tables S1, S2.

3 Results

Gray hair color increased the perceived age of male and female faces (p < 0.001; Table 2 and Figure 2). Faces with gray hair were, on average, evaluated as being 1.2× older than faces without gray hair. For example, women assessed the perceived ages of the male faces with non-gray hair as 30 years old (SE: 0.15 years), on average, and assessed the perceived ages of the male faces with gray hair as 36 years old (SE: 0.18 years); men assessed the perceived ages of the female faces with non-gray hair as 33 years old (SE: 0.19 years), on average, and assessed the perceived ages of the female faces with gray hair as 38 years old (SE: 0.24 years). Gray hair color decreased the attractiveness of male and female faces (F1,28 = 4.73, p = 0.038) but had no influence on their social status (F1,28 = 0.70, p = 0.41) or aggressiveness (F1,28 = 1.21, p = 0.28). Gray hair also decreased the trustworthiness of faces when evaluated by men (male faces: t1,28 = 4.48, p = 0.0001; female faces: t1,28 = 4.50, p = 0.0001); gray hair had no impact on the trustworthiness of faces evaluated by women (p > 0.52; Table 3 and Figure 3). Means, standard deviations, 95% confidence intervals, and effect sizes are reported in Table 4.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The effect of stimuli hair color, stimuli gender, subject gender, perception of aging, and subject age on perceived age in each experiment.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The effect of stimuli hair color on perceived age in the (A) attractiveness, (B) social status, (C) aggression, and (D) trustworthiness experiments based on the gender of the face stimuli and subjects.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The effect of stimuli hair color, stimuli gender, subject gender, perception of aging, and subject age on social perceptions.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The effect of stimuli hair color on (A) attractiveness, (B) social status, (C) aggression, and (D) trustworthiness based on the gender of the face stimuli and subjects.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The mean, standard deviation, 95% confidence interval, and effect size (Cohen’s d) of non-gray hair versus gray hair on assessments of perceived age and social perceptions.

4 Discussion

Both men and women perceived faces with gray hair as older and less attractive than faces without gray hair. Men (but not women) perceived faces with gray hair as less trustworthy than faces without gray hair. Social status and aggressiveness perceptions were similar irrespective of whether the faces had gray hair or not. These results support our predictions regarding perceived age and attractiveness and partially support our predictions about trustworthiness, social status and aggression.

Previous studies have found that people with gray hair are perceived as older. When evaluating images of men and women with varying levels of gray hair, people estimated both men and women as being older when they had more gray hair (Bulpitt et al., 2001; Gunn et al., 2009). Interestingly, women that dyed their hair (likely to non-gray natural colors) were perceived as appearing younger for their age compared to woman that did not dye their hair (Gunn et al., 2009). Our experimental results confirm these correlational studies, indicating a strong link between gray hair and perceived age. Given the relatively strong association between chronological age and gray hair (Keogh and Walsh, 1965; Panhard et al., 2012), gray hair is a relatively reliable index of age and may be used as an honest indicator of age (Jawor and Breitwisch, 2003).

Both male and females faces with gray hair were perceived as less attractive by men and women. Since faces with gray hair were perceived as older, these attractiveness judgments may be driven by perceived age of the faces. It is also possible that the attractiveness judgments were influenced by the relatively young age of the subjects or the modified stimuli (potentially appearing unnatural to the subjects). Many previous studies have found that older faces are considered less attractive than younger faces (Foos and Clark, 2011; Furnham et al., 2004; He et al., 2021; Kissler and Bäuml, 2000; Kwart et al., 2012; Wernick and Manaster, 1984). Women generally prefer romantic partners who are similar in age to themselves and men tend to prefer romantic partners who are within their reproductive years (Buunk et al., 2001; Kenrick and Keefe, 1992). Given that most of our women subjects were below 30 years old, the perceived ages of the male faces were closer to their own ages when the faces had non-gray hair versus gray hair. As such, the male faces with non-gray hair were closest in age to the women subjects, potentially explaining why they rated those faces as most attractive. Similarly, men perceived the female faces with non-gray hair as younger compared to the faces with gray hair, and therefore potentially perceived the faces with non-gray hair as more fertile (Berkowitz et al., 1990). It is also possible that faces with graying hair are perceived as less attractive because graying hair can be a sign of disease, such as Alzheimer’s disease (Mendelsohn and Larrick, 2020), Parkinson’s disease (Jucevičiūtė et al., 2019) or cardiovascular disease (Elfaramawy et al., 2018).

Gray hair had no impact on the perception of social status or aggression. Faces of men are perceived as reaching their peak dominance around 35 years old and then plateauing (Batres et al., 2015). We did not find that the male faces with non-gray hair were perceived as having lower social status, despite these faces being perceived as younger (around 30 years old) than the dominance plateau while the faces with gray hair were above the dominance plateau (around 36 years old). Similarly, even though male faces that are older are perceived as less physically dominant (Richardson et al., 2020), we did not find that male faces with gray hair (and thus perceived as being older) were considered less aggressive. These results suggest that traits aside from gray hair (such as wrinkles, baldness, facial hair, or masculinity; Batres et al., 2015; Bulpitt et al., 2001; Dixson and Vasey, 2012) are more important to assessments of social status and aggression.

Gray hair lowered the trustworthiness of faces when men assessed them. Previous studies have found that younger faces are considered more trustworthy than older faces but the gender of the faces and participants were either not considered or not found to have an effect (Pehlivanoglu et al., 2022; Zebrowitz et al., 2013). It is possible that men considered faces with gray hair less trustworthy because those faces were perceived as older. Other studies have found the opposite: older people are generally considered highly trustworthy (Cuddy and Fiske, 2002; Fiske, 1998). In contrast, women’s evaluation of trustworthiness was unaffected by gray hair. This suggests that other factors are more important in driving trustworthiness assessments in women, such as facial shape (Kleisner et al., 2013; Rezlescu et al., 2012; Siddique et al., 2022; Stirrat and Perrett, 2010; Todorov et al., 2008;), self-resemblance (DeBruine, 2002; Farmer et al., 2013), and typicality (Sofer et al., 2014). The finding that perceptions of trustworthiness was influenced by gray hair for men, but not women, might also be attributable to different sex-based priorities in social perceptions related to mate-choice (Buss, 1996). For example, from an evolutionary perspective, men tend to seek partners exhibiting youthful characteristics, while this tends not to be as strong a priority for women in mate-seeking (Buss and Schmitt, 2019). Youthful facial characteristics are generally perceived as more trustworthy (Zebrowitz, 2017), so it is possible that the increased perceptions of age due to gray hair detrimentally impacted perceptions of trustworthiness moreso for men than women, given the sex differences in ecological weight of youthful characteristics. Trustworthiness may also be especially important evolutionarily to men to ensure paternity (Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Apicella and Marlowe, 2004). Additional studies examining the links between gray hair, age, and trustworthiness as a function of sex would be valuable.

4.1 Limitations and future directions

We note several limitations in this study that could be explored in future research. First, the perceived age of the faces (likely Caucasian) with non-gray hair color used in this study was relatively young (around 30 years old). While Caucasian people usually begin to get gray hair in their mid-thirties (Keogh and Walsh, 1965), the percentage of gray hair is relatively low (most people below 40 years old have less than 20% gray hair; Kwon et al., 2012; Panhard et al., 2012). Due to the methodological approach of creating natural appearing gray hair while holding all other facial features constant, the gray hair that we simulated in this study was complete (100% gray hair). Therefore, it would be informative to evaluate how the degree of graying impacts perceived age and social perceptions. In addition, it is possible that subjects perceived the gray hair in these faces of relatively young people as prematurely gray, which can be associated with additional physiological and psychological conditions beyond natural aging processes (Thompson et al., 2019). Furthermore, it would be valuable to determine whether the faces of older people (those that are more likely to have higher levels of gray hair) would be evaluated differently. It would be particularly interesting to assess whether older men with gray hair are considered more attractive than those with less gray hair, as suggested by the vernacular term “silver fox.” Similarly, the perception of gray hair may vary based on the subjects’ age. Most of our subjects were relatively young and it is possible that older subjects would evaluate the faces with gray hair differently. Second, we modified hair color by simulating gray hair only on the head. As people age, the hair color of their eyebrows, eye lashes and facial hair can also turn gray (Senanayake and Wickramasinghe, 2016). We used male faces with minimal to no facial hair but some male faces did have some stubble. It would therefore be informative to assess how changes in hair color on these other facial regions impacts perceived age and social perceptions. Third, the face stimuli were likely Caucasian and evaluated by primarily Caucasian and Asian participants. Because the prevalence of gray hair varies by ethnic group (Panhard et al., 2012), further studies that examine the perception of gray hair by ethnic group could indicate ethnic differences in the perception of gray hair. And fourth, the face stimuli were assessed by subjects that were mostly likely from Western culture. Because social perceptions can vary across cultures (Tovée et al., 2006), it would be interesting to determine how the perception of gray hair varies by culture, especially in cultures that attribute high esteem to the elderly (North and Fiske, 2015).

Future studies could also consider modifying other aspects of this study. Other social perceptions (such as likability) may also be linked to gray hair and would be fruitful targets for future study. The faces in this study were displayed for 5 s each and it would be interesting to assess whether varying this viewing duration influences social perceptions. Previous work has demonstrated that people can assess the gender, age, and identity of faces in less than a second (Dobs et al., 2019) but viewing time can influence social perceptions (Rashidi et al., 2012). Because we modified only the hair color of the faces in our stimuli (and not other features often associated with aging such as gray eyebrows and wrinkles), it is possible that the face stimuli appeared unnatural or artificial to the subjects. Experiments with crossed designs that vary multiple traits (e.g., faces with gray hair, faces with wrinkles, and faces with both gray hair and wrinkles) could tease apart the importance of each feature. Future studies confirming that the face stimuli appeared natural to the subjects would also be valuable. Artificial faces, such as the faces of avatars, are not always perceived in the same way as real faces at the behavioral or neural level (Moser et al., 2007). Similarly, it is possible that subjects may have been biased to assess the faces with gray hair as older because of experimental expectations (e.g., if the subjects noted that the hair color was the only trait being modified and they assume that gray hair is usually associated with older age, they may have assessed the faces as older). This could explain why the faces with gray hair were only rated as 1.2× older than the faces with non-gray hair, rather than being rated as much older. In addition, hair turns gray in other species as well as they age, such as chimpanzees (Tapanes et al., 2020) and dogs (Pati et al., 2015). Future studies that examine whether gray hair in these non-human animals affects how conspecifics perceive them would be interesting.

By better understanding perceived age and social perceptions of faces, we can potentially use this knowledge to overcome implicit biases (Haselton et al., 2016). Given that faces with gray hair are perceived as older, it is possible that people with gray hair are discriminated against, as older individuals are often discriminated against in the workplace and other settings (reviewed in Marques et al., 2020). For example, people who appear older are less likely to be hired than younger-looking people (Kaufmann et al., 2016). Ageism is a form of conscious or unconscious discrimination against older people in which others alter their feelings, beliefs or behaviors because of an individual’s real or perceived age (Iversen et al., 2009; Levy and Banaji, 2002). This discrimination may result from an evolutionary mismatch such that behaviors that were adaptive in our ancestral environment are no longer adaptive today (Li et al., 2018). Being aware of biases is often among the first steps to overcoming them (Lee, 2017), suggesting that being aware of how gray hair color impacts age and social perceptions can be a first step to reducing biases against those with gray hair in contexts ranging from the workplace to other social situations. Interventions that aim to promote positive attitudes toward older people have some success in overcoming these biases (Sinclair et al., 2023). Among different types of interventions, age diversity workshops are among the most promising tools for reducing bias toward older people. For example, a workshop focused on reducing bias of older workers by awareness-raising and intergroup contact; biases toward older workers were reduced after the workshop, with positive effects persisting 6 months afterwards (Schloegel et al., 2016).

5 Conclusion

People frequently and rapidly make inferences about a person’s age, which can meaningfully inform social judgments and decision making in social interactions. Graying hair is an observable characteristic that can convey age information, and indirectly inform other relevant social perceptions. The current work conducted an experiment that independently manipulated hair color while assessing perceptions of age and related social evaluations. The results demonstrated that gray hair increased perceptions of age, decreased perceptions of attractiveness, decreased perceptions of trustworthiness (rated by men, but not women), and did not impact perceptions of social status or aggressiveness. These results suggest that gray hair can independently be used as an indicator of some important social evaluations (age, attractiveness, trustworthiness), while others (social status, aggression) may be better informed by other visible characteristics. The current work highlights the role of observable characteristics in informing social inferences, the utility of experimental approaches toward separating the unique contributions of independent visible signals of social states and traits, and potentially informs approaches to minimize social biases related to aging.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Review Board of Texas A&M University (#2022-0935D). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

KN: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. CT: Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Software. JY: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the College of Agriculture and Life Sciences at Texas A&M University and Texas A&M AgriLife Research to JY.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1541836/full#supplementary-material

References

Aberg, E., Kukkonen, I., and Sarpila, O. (2020). From double to triple standards of ageing. Perceptions of physical appearance at the intersections of age, gender and class. J. Aging Stud. 55:100876. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2020.100876

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Apicella, C. L., and Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Perceived mate fidelity and paternal resemblance predict men's investment in children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 371–378. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.06.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, M., O’Hanlon, A., McGee, H. M., Hickey, A., and Conroy, R. M. (2007). Cross-sectional validation of the aging perceptions questionnaire: a multidimensional instrument for assessing self-perceptions of aging. BMC Geriatr. 7, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-7-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, A. E. (2003). Socioeconomic status and age identity: the role of dimensions of health in the subjective construction of age. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 58, S101–S109. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Batres, C., Re, D. E., and Perrett, D. I. (2015). Influence of perceived height, masculinity, and age on each other and on perceptions of dominance in male faces. Perception 44, 1293–1309. doi: 10.1177/0301006615596898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beddow, M. (2011). Hair color stereotypes and their associated perceptions in relationships and the workplace. Psi Chi J Psychol Res 16, 12–19. doi: 10.24839/1089-4136.JN16.1.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, G. S., Skovron, M. L., Lapinski, R. H., and Berkowitz, R. L. (1990). Delayed childbearing and the outcome of pregnancy. N. Engl. J. Med. 322, 659–664. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199003083221004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowdring, M. A., Sayette, M. A., Girard, J. M., and Woods, W. C. (2021). In the eye of the beholder: a comprehensive analysis of stimulus type, perceiver, and target in physical attractiveness perceptions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 45, 241–259. doi: 10.1007/s10919-020-00350-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulpitt, C. J., Markowe, H., and Shipley, M. (2001). Why do some people look older than they should? Postgrad. Med. J. 77, 578–581. doi: 10.1136/pmj.77.911.578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (1996). “The evolutionary psychology of human social strategies” in Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. eds. E. T. Higgins and A. W. Kruglanski (New York: Guilford), 3–38.

Google Scholar

Buss, D. M., and Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychol. Rev. 100, 204–232.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Buss, D. M., and Schmitt, D. P. (2019). Mate preferences and their behavioral manifestations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 77–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103408

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Kenrick, D. T., and Warntjes, A. (2001). Age preferences for mates as related to gender, own age, and involvement level. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 241–250. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00065-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carré, J. M., McCormick, C. M., and Mondloch, C. J. (2009). Facial structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1194–1198. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02423.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelnokova, O., Laeng, B., Eikemo, M., Riegels, J., Løseth, G., Maurud, H., et al. (2014). Rewards of beauty: the opioid system mediates social motivation in humans. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 746–747. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, L. H., and Korotchenko, A. (2010). Shades of grey: to dye or not to dye one’s hair in later life. Ageing Soc. 30, 1011–1026. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X1000036X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conroy-Beam, D., and Buss, D. M. (2019). Why is age so important in human mating? Evolved age preferences and their influences on multiple mating behaviors. Evol. Behav. Sci. 13, 127–157. doi: 10.1037/ebs0000127

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuddy, A. J. C., and Fiske, S. T. (2002). “Doddering, but dear: process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons” in Ageism. ed. T. D. Nelson (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 3–26.

Google Scholar

DeBruine, L. M. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proc. Biol. Sci. 269, 1307–1312. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., and Vasey, P. L. (2012). Beards augment perceptions of men’s age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness. Behav. Ecol. 23, 481–490. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobs, K., Isik, L., Pantazis, D., and Kanwisher, N. (2019). How face perception unfolds over time. Nat. Commun. 10:1258. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09239-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elfaramawy, A. A. A., Hanna, I. S., Darweesh, R. M., Ismail, A. S., and Kandil, H. I. (2018). The degree of hair graying as an independent risk marker for coronary artery disease, a CT coronary angiography study. Egypt. Heart J. 70, 15–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ehj.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairchild, M. D. (2013). Color appearance models. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Farmer, H., McKay, R., and Tsakiris, M. (2013). Trust in me. Psychol. Sci. 25, 290–292. doi: 10.1177/0956797613494852

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawsitt, F., Dockray, S., and Setti, A. (2022). Regulatory focus and perceptions of ageing: exploring the connections. Aging Ment. Health 26, 1451–1459. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2021.1952550

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinman, S., and Gill, G. W. (1978). Sex differences in physical attractiveness preferences. J. Soc. Psychol. 105, 43–52. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1978.9924089

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Hufschmidt, C., Hirn, T., Will, S., McKelvey, G., and Lankhof, J. (2016). Age, health and attractiveness perception of virtual (rendered) human hair. Front. Psychol. 7:1893. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01893

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T. (1998). “Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination” in The handbook of social psychology. eds. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey. 4th ed (Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill).

Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foos, P. W., and Clark, M. C. (2011). Adult age and gender differences in perceptions of facial attractiveness: beauty is in the eye of the older beholder. J. Genet. Psychol. 172, 162–175. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2010.526154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Mistry, D., and McClelland, A. (2004). The influence of age of the face and the waist to hip ratio on judgements of female attractiveness and traits. Pers. Individ. Differ. 36, 1171–1185. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(03)002095

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goetz, S. M. M., Shattuck, K. S., Miller, R. M., Campbell, J. A., Lozoya, E., Weisfeld, G. E., et al. (2013). Social status moderates the relationship between facial structure and aggression. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2329–2334. doi: 10.1177/0956797613493294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunn, D. A., Rexbye, H., Griffiths, C. E. M., Murray, P. G., Fereday, A., Catt, S. D., et al. (2009). Why some women look young for their age. PLoS One 4:e8021. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., and Murray, D. R. (2016). “The evolution of cognitive bias” in The handbook of evolutionary psychology: integrations. ed. D. M. Buss. 2nd ed (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 968–987.

Google Scholar

He, D., Workman, C. I., Kenett, Y. N., He, X., and Chatterjee, A. (2021). The effect of aging on facial attractiveness: an empirical and computational investigation. Acta Psychol. 219:103385. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinsz, V. B., Matz, D. C., and Patience, R. A. (2001). Does women's hair signal reproductive potential? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37, 166–172. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2000.1450

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Isopahkala-Bouret, U. (2016). “It’s a great benefit to have gray hair!”: the intersection of gender, aging, and visibility in midlife professional women’s narratives. J. Women Aging 29, 267–277. doi: 10.1080/08952841.2016.1142773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iversen, T. N., Larsen, L., and Solem, P. E. (2009). A conceptual analysis of ageism. Nord. Psychol. 61, 4–22. doi: 10.1027/1901-2276.61.3.4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jawor, J., and Breitwisch, R. (2003). Melanin in ornaments, honesty, and sexual selection. Auk 120, 249–265. doi: 10.1093/auk/120.2.249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Flake, J. K., Liuzza, M. T., Antfolk, J., Arinze, N. C., et al. (2021). To which world regions does the valence–dominance model of social perception apply? Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 159–169. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01007-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jucevičiūtė, N., Banaitytė, I., Vaitkus, A., and Balnytė, R. (2019). Preclinical signs of Parkinson’s disease: a possible association of Parkinson’s disease with skin and hair features. Med. Hypotheses 127, 100–104. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2019.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, M. C., Krings, F., and Sczesny, S. (2016). Looking too old? How an older age appearance reduces chances of being hired. Br. J. Manag. 27, 727–739. doi: 10.1111/1467-8551.12125

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenrick, D. T., and Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 75–91. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00067595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keogh, E. V., and Walsh, R. J. (1965). Rate of greying of human hair. Nature 207, 877–878. doi: 10.1038/207877a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kissler, J., and Bäuml, K.-H. (2000). Effects of the beholder’s age on the perception of facial attractiveness. Acta Psychol. 104, 145–166. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00018-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleisner, K., Priplatova, L., Frost, P., and Flegr, J. (2013). Trustworthy-looking face meets brown eyes. PLoS One 8:e53285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwart, D. G., Foulsham, T., and Kingstone, A. (2012). Age and beauty are in the eye of the beholder. Perception 41, 925–938. doi: 10.1068/p7136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, O., Jo, S., Paik, S., Choi, J., Lee, J., Cho, S., et al. (2012). Hair graying pattern depends on gender, onset age and smoking habits. Acta Dermato Venereol. 92, 160–161. doi: 10.2340/00015555-1181

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyle, D. J., and Mahler, H. I. M. (1996). The effects of hair color and cosmetic use on perceptions of a female’s ability. Psychol. Women Q. 20, 447–455. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00311.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C. (2017). “Awareness as a first step toward overcoming implicit bias” in Enhancing justice: reducing bias, eds. R. Sarah, et al. GWU Law School Public Law Research Paper No. 2017-56, GWU Legal Studies Research Paper No. 2017-56. 289. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3011381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B., and Banaji, M. R. (2002). “Implicit agesim” in Ageism. stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. ed. T. D. Nelson (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Li, N. P., Van Vugt, M., and Colarelli, S. M. (2018). The evolutionary mismatch hypothesis: implications for psychological science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 38–44. doi: 10.1177/0963721417731378

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. Biol. Sci. 366, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: a peek inside the cognitive toolbox. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 37–47. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.37

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, S., Mariano, J., Mendonça, J., De Tavernier, W., Hess, M., Naegele, L., et al. (2020). Determinants of ageism against older adults: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2560. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072560

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matlab (2022). R2022a. Natick, Massachusetts: The MathWorks Inc.

Google Scholar

Mendelsohn, A. R., and Larrick, J. W. (2020). The danger of being too sympathetic: norepinephrine in Alzheimer’s disease and graying of hair. Rejuvenation Res. 23, 68–72. doi: 10.1089/rej.2020.2309

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesko, N., and Bereczkei, T. (2004). Hairstyle as an adaptive means of displaying phenotypic quality. Hum. Nat. 15, 251–270. doi: 10.1007/s12110-004-1008-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitra, B., Webb, M., and Wolfe, C. (2014). Audience responses to the physical appearance of television newsreaders. Participat. J. Aud. Recept. Stud. 11, 45–57.

Google Scholar

Moser, E., Derntl, B., Robinson, S., Fink, B., Gur, R. C., and Grammer, K. (2007). Amygdala activation at 3T in response to human and avatar facial expressions of emotions. J. Neurosci. Methods 161, 126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.10.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, U., and Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of west point cadets as a predictor of later military rank. Soc. Forces 74, 823–850. doi: 10.1093/sf/74.3.823

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niehorster, D. C., Andersson, R., and Nyström, M. (2020). Titta: a tool-box for creating PsychToolbox and PsychoPy experiments with Tobii eye trackers. Behav. Res. Methods 52, 1970–1979. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01358-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishimura, E. K. (2011). Melanocyte stem cells: a melanocyte reservoir in hair follicles for hair and skin pigmentation. Pigment Cell Melanoma Res. 24, 401–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-148X.2011.00855.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

North, M. S., and Fiske, S. T. (2015). Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: a cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 141, 993–1021. doi: 10.1037/a0039469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Sullivan, J. D. B., Nicu, C., Picard, M., Chéret, J., Bedogni, B., Tobin, D. J., et al. (2020). The biology of human hair greying. Biol. Rev. 96, 107–128. doi: 10.1111/brv.12648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandhi, D., and Khanna, D. (2013). Premature graying of hair. Indian J. Dermatol. Venereol. Leprol. 79:641. doi: 10.4103/0378-6323.116733

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panhard, S., Lozano, I., and Loussouarn, G. (2012). Greying of the human hair: a worldwide survey, revisiting the “50” rule of thumb. Br. J. Dermatol. 167, 865–873. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2133.2012.11095.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pati, S., Panda, S. K., Acharya, A. P., Senapati, S., Behera, M., and Behera, S. S. (2015). Evaluation of geriatric changes in dogs. Vet. World 8:273278, 273–278. doi: 10.14202/vetworld.2015.273-278

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pehlivanoglu, D., Lin, T., Lighthall, N. R., Heemskerk, A., Harber, A., Wilson, R. C., et al. (2022). Facial trustworthiness perception across the adult life span. J. Gerontol. Series B 78, 434–444. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbac166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rashidi, M., Pazhoohi, F., and Hosseinchari, M. (2012). Effect of facial stimuli exposure time on evaluation of facial attractiveness. Aust. J. Psychol. 64, 164–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00050.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezlescu, C., Duchaine, B., Olivola, C. Y., and Chater, N. (2012). Unfakeable facial configurations affect strategic choices in trust games with or without information about past behavior. PLoS One 7:e34293. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034293

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, T., Waddington, M., and Gilman, R. T. (2020). Young, formidable men show greater sensitivity to facial cues of dominance. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roll, S., and Verinis, J. S. (1971). Stereotypes of scalp and facial hair as measured by the semantic differential. Psychol. Rep. 28, 975–980. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1971.28.3.975

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarin, K. Y., and Artandi, S. E. (2007). Aging, graying and loss of melanocyte stem cells. Stem Cell Rev. 3, 212–217. doi: 10.1007/s12015-007-0028-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schloegel, U., Stegmann, S., Maedche, A., and van Dick, R. (2016). Reducing age stereotypes in software development: the effects of awareness- and cooperation-based diversity interventions. J. Syst. Softw. 121, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jss.2016.07.041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Senanayake, H. M. K., and Wickramasinghe, N. D. (2016). Age estimation based on appearance of gray hair in different body sites of Sri Lankan autopsy cases. J. Forensic Sci. 62, 1075–1079. doi: 10.1111/1556-4029.13375

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siddique, S., Sutherland, C. A. M., Palermo, R., Foo, Y. Z., Swe, D. C., and Jeffery, L. (2022). Development of face-based trustworthiness impressions in childhood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cogn. Dev. 61:101131. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101131

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, C., Joffe, T., Ginnivan, N., Parker, S. K., and Anstey, K. J. (2023). A scoping review of workplace interventions to promote positive attitudes toward older workers and reduce age-based discrimination. Work Aging Retire. 10, 61–76. doi: 10.1093/workar/waad013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sofer, C., Dotsch, R., Wigboldus, D. H. J., and Todorov, A. (2014). What is typical is good. Psychol. Sci. 26, 39–47. doi: 10.1177/0956797614554955

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorokowski, P. (2008). Attractiveness of blonde women in evolutionary perspective: studies with two polish samples. Percept. Mot. Skills 106, 737–744. doi: 10.2466/pms.106.3.737-744

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stirrat, M., and Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychol. Sci. 21, 349–354. doi: 10.1177/0956797610362647

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutherland, C. A., Oldmeadow, J. A., Santos, I. M., Towler, J., Burt, D. M., and Young, A. W. (2013). Social inferences from faces: ambient images generate a three-dimensional model. Cognition 127, 105–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Barrett, S. (2011). British men’s hair color preferences: an assessment of courtship solicitation and stimulus ratings. Scand. J. Psychol. 52, 595–600. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00911.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapanes, E., Anestis, S., Kamilar, J. M., and Bradley, B. J. (2020). Does facial hair greying in chimpanzees provide a salient progressive cue of aging? PLoS One 15:e0235610. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235610

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, K. G., Marchitto, M. C., Ly, B. C. K., and Chien, A. L. (2019). Evaluation of physiological, psychological, and lifestyle factors associated with premature hair graying. Int. J. Trichology 11, 153–158. doi: 10.4103/ijt.ijt_43_19

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., and Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trustworthiness: a model based approach. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 119–127. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovée, M. J., Swami, V., Furnham, A., and Mangalparsad, R. (2006). Changing perceptions of attractiveness as observers are exposed to a different culture. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 443–456. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walla, P., Chang, M., Schaefer, K., and Windhager, S. (2020). Social perception of faces: brain imaging and subjective ratings. Brain Sci. 10:861. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10110861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, R., and Holland, C. (2010). “If I look old, I will be treated old”: hair and later-life image dilemmas. Ageing Soc. 31, 288–307. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X10000863

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wernick, M., and Manaster, G. J. (1984). Age and the perception of age and attractiveness. Gerontologist 24, 408–414. doi: 10.1093/geront/24.4.408

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorzinski, J. L., and Platt, M. L. (2010). Same-sex gaze attraction influences mate-choice copying in humans. PLoS One 5:e9115. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A. (2017). First impressions from faces. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 237–242. doi: 10.1177/0963721416683996

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G., Hillman, S., and Boc, H. (2013). Older and younger adults’ first impressions from faces: similar in agreement but different in positivity. Psychol. Aging 28, 202–212. doi: 10.1037/a0030927

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ageism, aggressiveness, attractiveness, gray hair, mate choice, sexual selection, social status, trustworthiness

Citation: Nutt KM, Thorstenson CA and Yorzinski JL (2025) Gray hair influences perceived age and social perceptions. Front. Psychol. 16:1541836. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1541836

Received: 08 December 2024; Accepted: 27 March 2025;
Published: 07 May 2025.

Edited by:

T. Joel Wade, Bucknell University, United States

Reviewed by:

Norbert Meskó, University of Pécs, Hungary
Felipe Novaes, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Copyright © 2025 Nutt, Thorstenson and Yorzinski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jessica L. Yorzinski, amVzc2ljYS55b3J6aW5za2lAYWcudGFtdS5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.