ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 June 2025

Sec. Eating Behavior

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1588999

Beyond general food craving: sex differences in food-specific craving identified using item response theory


Daiil Jun,Daiil Jun1,2Sean JooSean Joo3Tera L. Fazzino,
Tera L. Fazzino1,2*
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States
  • 2Cofrin Logan Center for Addiction Research and Treatment, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States
  • 3Department of Educational Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States

Background: Food craving plays a significant role in food choice and excess energy intake. While prior research has predominantly examined food craving as a general construct (e.g., craving for foods overall), this approach may obscure important variability in craving for specific foods. The current study aimed to address this limitation by examining craving for specific foods (food-item level) and examining differences across sex in food item craving, while controlling for overall food craving.

Methods: The sample (N = 583) was collected via crowdsourcing. The Food Craving Inventory was used to assess craving for 28 food items. Using item response theory (IRT), a partial credit model was employed to investigate which food item was easier or more commonly craved, while holding overall food craving level constant across participants. Differential item functioning (DIF) analysis identified sex differences in craving for specific food items, with effect sizes calculated to interpret the magnitude of DIF.

Results: The partial credit model revealed that pizza and chocolate were the easiest or most commonly craved, while gravy and cornbread were the most difficult to crave. DIF analysis suggested that savory items were more difficult (or less commonly) craved among females with medium effect sizes (i.e., Cohen's D) ranging from 0.53 to 0.80, whereas sweets were more difficult for males to crave, with effect sizes ranging from 0.42 to 0.49.

Conclusion: Findings indicated that food craving may vary depending on specific foods and sex.

1 Introduction

Food craving, defined as an strong desire to consume a food (White et al., 2002), has been suggested to play a key role in disordered eating behaviors (e.g., binge eating) as well as excess energy intake and related conditions (e.g., obesity) (Boswell and Kober, 2016; Stopyra et al., 2021; Ince et al., 2021; Yu et al., 2022). In the food craving literature, food craving has mostly been measured as a general construct that represents either trait or state-based craving for foods in general (Taylor, 2019; Meule, 2020). Trait-based food craving represents how individuals tend to respond to or experience food craving and is hypothesized to be an individual-level difference, with some individuals experiencing higher craving overall compared to others (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000; Meule, 2020). In contrast, state-based food craving represents momentary or situation-based food craving, which is hypothesized to vary over time and is assumed to be influenced by environment/situational contexts (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000; Richard et al., 2017; Pannicke et al., 2022). Trait and state-based craving constructs have contributed to identifying the role of elevated food craving as an individual and state-level risk factor in disordered eating behaviors (Leslie et al., 2018; Reents and Pedersen, 2021; Schaefer et al., 2023), which has been important in identifying intervention targets for craving-induced overeating and binge eating (Sun and Kober, 2020).

Despite the usefulness of prior work in identifying high general food craving as an individual-level vulnerability, research characterizing food craving as a general construct has employed an assumption that measured craving may be similarly applicable to all types of foods. However, research focusing on aggregate food craving may overlook important variability in craving for specific foods that may contribute to disordered eating and excess energy intake. Previous studies have suggested that foods with specific nutrient combinations (e.g., high fat and sodium; fat and sugar; carbohydrates and sodium), termed hyper-palatable foods (HPF), may have particularly strong reinforcing properties (DiFeliceantonio et al., 2018; Epstein et al., 2012; Fazzino et al., 2019), and thus may more easily trigger food wanting and craving relative to whole, fresh foods that come from nature (Fazzino, 2022). Although there have been limited studies examining how craving differs across specific food items, some empirical evidence has suggested that HPF may be more easily craved than non-HPF. For example, one prior study measured craving for specific food items and found that foods that would be considered HPF (e.g., hamburgers; pizza) were more strongly craved than non-HPF (e.g., fruits and vegetables) among adults with obesity and binge eating disorder (Reents and Pedersen, 2021). Also, a study using ecological momentary assessment found that craving for HPF (e.g., hamburgers) was more frequently reported than craving for fresh foods (e.g., vegetables) among adults with elevated BMIs relative to adults with lower BMIs (Roefs et al., 2019). These findings highlight the importance of measuring cravings for specific food items, as some types of food may be more strongly craved than others and may differentially influence food seeking and intake behavior.

Measuring general food craving may also obscure the heterogeneity of food cravings across different contexts and for different groups of people. For example, some prior literature has suggested that cravings among males may differ significantly from females, with hypothesized reasons spanning from physiological differences to potential differences in food effects due to sociocultural and/or psychological reasons (Hallam et al., 2016). In general in the craving literature, the prevailing view has been that that females may exhibit higher cravings for sweets (e.g., chocolate) whereas males may exhibit greater craving for savory foods (Asarian and Geary, 2013; Hallam et al., 2016; Meule, 2020). Specifically, previous studies typically compared males' and females' average scores for a single item or closely related food items, which prevented them from disentangling whether observed sex differences were due to craving for the particular food item assessed (e.g., chocolate) or reflective of higher general craving in one sex compared to the other (Anton et al., 2012; Chao et al., 2016; Imperatori et al., 2013; Meule and Hormes, 2015). However, few studies have directly investigated sex differences in cravings for specific foods in the past decade, and thus, the methodological limitations of these studies have remained largely unaddressed.

To address the limitations of the prior literature, the present study sought to examine differences in cravings for different types of foods among a general sample of adults. The study used the Food Craving Inventory (FCI), which assesses food craving across different types of foods (White et al., 2002) and facilitated the examination of food craving at the item-level using item response theory (IRT) analysis. The study also sought to examine differences in food craving across food items by sex using differential item functioning (DIF) analysis.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study procedures and participants

The study was reviewed and approved by the host university's Institutional Review Board. The study used Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online crowdsourcing platform, and collected data in the spring of 2020. Participant recruitment was conducted in six batches to accommodate individuals with varying schedule availabilities. To be eligible, participants had to be aged 18–65, reside in the United States, possess an MTurk approval rating of ≥99%, and have completed ≥1,000 studies on MTurk. These criteria were selected to adhere to MTurk's data quality standards and to align with the study's focus on US food measures. All participants provided informed consent (N = 602). Ten participants provided poor-quality data (i.e., failed at least one of three attention check questions), and seven participants had missing values in the FCI, and were thus removed from the data prior to analysis.

Participants were asked to select from the following options: male; female; transgender, male to female; transgender, female to male; intersex; and prefer not to answer. Unfortunately, sex and gender identity were both included in the response options. To recognize this limitation and to best reflect participant responses, we included participants who selected the male (n = 308) or female (n = 275) sex options in the current analyses. We did not have sufficient statistical power to analyze the other groupings (n = 2 selected transgender) and therefore did not include these participants in the analyses. Therefore, the final sample that we analyzed was N = 583.

2.2 Measures

The Food Craving Inventory (FCI) was employed to assess participants' craving for specific foods (White et al., 2002). The FCI is a widely used self-report measure designed to capture the intensity and frequency of cravings for specific foods. The FCI consists of 28 items, with each item corresponding to a particular food (e.g., chocolate, fried chicken). Participants assessed the frequency of their cravings for each item over the past month using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always/almost every day). Generally in the literature, the FCI has been used to measure overall craving for food items (aggregated), or craving for specific categories of foods (e.g., fast foods) (White et al., 2002; Anton et al., 2012; Chao et al., 2016). In this study, the FCI was administered once to all study participants. We employed IRT to examine how craving differed across the 28 individual food items in the FCI. The FCI has demonstrated good internal consistency (α = 0.93), acceptable test-retest reliability (α = 0.86), concurrent validity, and discriminant validity (White et al., 2002), suggesting it is a robust measure for assessing food cravings. In this study, the FCI showed a good internal consistency (α = 0.93).

2.3 Data analysis

IRT analysis was employed to examine the characteristics of items and psychometric properties. Furthermore, DIF analysis was carried out to examine potential item bias against sex for the FCI. Analyses were conducted using the “mirt” package in R (Chalmers, 2012). IRT uses response to all FCI items to estimate individuals' latent food craving level while simultaneously determining the unique characteristics of each item (e.g., item difficulty).

Specifically, the partial credit model (PCM) was used to estimate item difficulty parameters. The PCM was specifically developed to analyze polytomous response data including Likert scale (Masters, 2016). The item difficulty parameter reflected how easy or difficult an item was to be endorsed by individuals with an average level of the trait. Overall craving, aggregated across all food items in the FCI, was calculated to account for in analyses, and may be considered to represent trait-level craving. The PCM was conducted to determine which food items in the FCI were more easily or commonly craved than other food items when individuals had the same level of overall food craving. The IRT parameters were estimated using marginal maximum likelihood (MML) with the expected and maximization (EM) algorithm (Lord, 1986).

Before estimating item parameters, the unidimensionality assumption of the construct was examined using principal component analysis (PCA). Next, item parameters (i.e., item difficulty) were estimated. Finally, DIF analysis was conducted to assess whether there were systematic differences in item responses between male and female participants while accounting for their underlying levels of food craving. The results of the DIF analysis provide insights into whether certain food items from the FCI function differently males and females, which can more accurately reflect sex differences in craving for specific food items. The results also inform whether higher latent (overall) craving may be related to differences in craving for different food items across males and females.

DIF analysis was conducted based on the multigroup IRT approach (Bock and Zimowski, 1997) and DIF detection procedure was conducted with a sequential-free baseline procedure (Chun et al., 2016) (detailed in the Supplementary material). Lastly, the effect sizes of true DIF items were calculated following Meade's (2010) approach, which provides a standardized item characteristic difference between two groups for DIF items and is comparable to conventional effect size measures (i.e., Cohen's D). Applied to our study, the effect size for DIF items indicated which food items showed greater ease to crave across males and females, given the same level of overall food craving. More details on the DIF detection procedure are provided in the Supplementary material.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive statistics

Demographic information for the sample is presented in Table 1. The average age of the sample was 37.9 years, and the average BMI was 26.5, calculated from participants' self-report of height and weight. The sample was comprised of 52.7% males. Descriptive statistics for each item in the FCI are displayed in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics (N = 583).

3.2 Unidimensionality assumption check for the FCI

According to the results from PCA, the first principal component explained 34.62% of the total variance, and the second principal component explained 8.77%. The first component accounted for around 4 times more of the total variance than the second component, which supports the unidimensionality of the FCI (Reckase, 1997).

3.3 Item characteristics of the FCI

The PCM model results facilitated a ranking of FCI items from the easiest to most difficult to crave as assessed across all participants in the sample (see Supplementary Table 2). Across participants, the easiest items to crave were pizza, French fries, and chocolate (item difficulty parameters ranged from 1.85 to 0.77), whereas the least craved food items were gravy, corn bread, and sausage (item difficulty parameters ranged from 2.36 to 3.01). Item score plots, test information plots, and measurement error plot are presented in Supplementary Figures 1, 2. The information and measurement error plots indicated that the FCI more accurately measured overall food craving for individuals with relatively higher food craving levels compared to those with lower levels.

3.4 Differential item functioning by sex

According to the DIF analysis, the following items were identified as DIF: item 1 (fried chicken), item 2 (sausage), item 7 (hot dog), item 8 (steak), item 12 (chocolate), item 14 (cake), and item 25 (hamburger). The findings suggested that fried chicken, sausage, hot dogs, and hamburgers were harder for females to crave, whereas chocolate and cake were harder for males to crave, despite the groups having the same overall level of food craving (Table 2). DIF items identified as being easier for males to crave (savory items) had medium effect sizes ranging from 0.53 (hot dog) to 0.80 (hamburger). DIF items that were easier for females to crave (sweet items) had effect sizes ranging from 0.42 (cake) to 0.49 (chocolate). Regarding latent (overall) food craving, DIF analysis indicated that females needed a higher level of latent food craving to report cravings for savory items identified as DIF compared to males. In contrast, males required a higher level of food craving to endorse cravings for chocolate and cake compared to females. Thus, findings suggested that females who craved savory items had higher average craving, and males who craved sweet items had higher average craving. Item scale plots for each DIF item depending on sex are presented in Figure 1. For more detailed results of DIF detection, please see Supplementary Table 3.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results from the free base line models for DIF in sex.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Item characteristic curves for items identified as differential functioning by sex. y-axis represents predicted score of craving for the specific food item. x-axis represents overall food craving. Higher values on the y-axis represents a greater expected score of craving for a specific food item at the respective θ levels on the x-axis (overall food craving). Yellow curve presents male's expected score along the overall food craving levels. Blue curve denotes female's expected score along the overall food craving levels.

4 Discussion

Traditionally in the literature, food craving has been studied as a general construct and characterized by aggregating craving ratings across different types of foods to obtain a total score. However, specific foods may differently influence craving intensity across individuals and among different groups (e.g., males and females), which is important to understand from a risk and prevention perspective. The current study evaluated variability in craving across different types of foods among a general sample of adults. Additionally, the study overcame the limitations of previous studies on sex differences in craving by examining differences in craving for specific foods across males and females, while holding average level of craving constant. The results revealed substantial variability in the degree of craving intensity for different foods, suggesting that individuals may differ in craving for different food types. Furthermore, findings revealed differences in the types of foods craved across males and females, even when holding average craving level constant; savory food items were more easily (commonly) craved by males, whereas sweet items were more easily craved by females. Taken together, this study highlighted that food craving may vary depending on the type of food and sex.

One key finding of the study was that food craving varied substantially when examined at the food item-level. Specifically, pizza, French fries, and chocolate were the easiest (most common) to crave, whereas gravy, cornbread, and sausage were the hardest. The items that were most commonly craved are foods that are commonly consumed among the US population, whereas the foods that were the hardest to crave are less commonly consumed nationally (U. S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, 2023). Thus, the findings appear to reflect common dietary intake of the US population. The findings may also reflect the wide availability of some HPF in the US food environment, which could contribute to cue-induced cravings and subsequent intake. Pizza and French fries are widely available and commonly consumed in the US food environment across various settings, from fast food restaurants to frozen grocery options (Zenk et al., 2015). Similarly, chocolate is widely available and heavily marketed in a variety of contexts in the US (Sebastian et al., 2010). Thus, the high availability and marketing of HPF in the US food supply may contribute to cue-induced cravings through repeated exposure and classical conditioning that associate these foods with rewarding experiences (Hill, 2007; Rejeski et al., 2010; Boyland et al., 2024). In contrast, food items like gravy may be less commonly encountered at the national scale, and may be more event- or culture-specific (e.g., gravy served with meat at holidays), and thus may not trigger cravings as readily. Overall, our results suggested that the most commonly craved food items were HPF that are popular and commonly consumed in the US, and thus may reflect their extensive availability in the US food environment.

The DIF analysis revealed moderate to large effect size differences in cravings for specific foods by sex, with males finding certain savory foods easier to crave and females finding some sweet foods easier to crave. Importantly, these findings emerged despite overall food craving across males and females being held constant in analyses; therefore, the findings were able to disentangle the effects of specific food items from overall craving level across males and females. Previous studies have attempted to test sex differences in craving intensity using single food items, with chocolate being a common food stimulus. Studies typically observed higher craving intensity ratings for chocolate among females relative to males (Hormes et al., 2014; Meule and Hormes, 2015), which appear to have influenced the focus on female samples in research on chocolate craving (Tapper and Turner, 2018; Meule et al., 2019; Richard et al., 2019) and overall craving research (Richard et al., 2017; Zorjan and Schienle, 2023). However, our findings suggest that these studies may have selected a food stimulus that females tend to crave more commonly or easily despite having relatively lower overall food craving than males, which may have impacted the findings and the interpretations of sex differences. In contrast, if a study had selected a savory item such as hamburger, the findings may have indicated higher craving intensity among males and may have resulted in an interpretation that males experience more craving relative to females. Thus, our finding highlights the importance of considering the item-specific nature of cravings when examining differences in craving intensity across groups.

The results of the DIF analysis may provide nuanced insights into craving across sexes, both regarding types of foods craved and also level of overall craving. Our study was conducted in a general sample of adults and therefore we cannot make inferences regarding clinical risk. However, our study identified distinct patterns of craving across sexes that also coincided with higher overall craving. Future research replicating this work among clinical samples, including individuals with elevated craving, obesity, and/or disordered eating could inform whether the sex-specific food item cravings identified in the study, combined with higher overall craving, may be a risk factor for clinical conditions.

The study had several limitations. First, the sample may not be representative of the broader US population, particularly in terms of race and ethnicity. Future studies should aim to include more racially and ethnically diverse populations to improve the generalizability of the findings. Second, the study did not assess where participants lived in the US, which could have influenced the results, especially given that cornbread was identified as particularly difficult to crave. Future studies should examine the potential effects of region on cravings for different types of foods. Third, the study measured sex (male, female) but also included an option for transgender, which conflated gender identity in the survey item. Although most participants (99.5%; 583/585) endorsed sex as male or female, n = 2 individuals selected the option for transgender. Because the sample that endorsed transgender was too small to analyze as a group, we did not include the participants in analyses. However, this approach may limit the generalizability of our findings to more diverse populations, including individuals who identify as transgender. Future studies should clearly distinguish questions for biological sex and gender identity to improve measurement of these constructs and to examine potential differences in food item level craving. Lastly, our study did not include eating-related variables such as dietary intake, which limits the contextualization of our findings within broader patterns of eating behavior. Future studies should examine how sex differences in cravings for specific food items relate to eating behaviors and dietary intake.

5 Conclusions and implications

The study findings highlight the nuances of food cravings, which differed by food items and sex, underscoring the need for more refined approaches in craving assessment research. The study identified distinct patterns of craving across sexes that also occurred with higher overall craving, and future work is needed to understand whether such presentations could be considered as risk factors for disordered eating and related clinical conditions.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon reasonable request and approval of the institutional review board, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by University of Kansas Human Subjects Research Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DJ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. SJ: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. TF: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The study was funded by internal funds from the University of Kansas. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1588999/full#supplementary-material

References

Anton, S. D., Gallagher, J., Carey, V. J., Laranjo, N., Cheng, J., Champagne, C. M., et al. (2012). Diet type and changes in food cravings following weight loss: findings from the pounds lost trial. Eat Weight Disord. 17, e101–e108. doi: 10.1007/BF03325333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asarian, L., and Geary, N. (2013). Sex differences in the physiology of eating. Am J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 305, R1215–R1267. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00446.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bock, R. D., and Zimowski, M. F. (1997). “Multiple group IRT,” in Handbook of Modern Item Response Theory, eds. W. J. van der Linden and R. K. Hambleton (New York, NY: Springer), 433-448. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-2691-6_25

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boswell, R. G., and Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes. Rev. 17, 159–177. doi: 10.1111/obr.12354

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyland, E., Spanakis, P., O'Reilly, C., and Christiansen, P. (2024). Associations between everyday exposure to food marketing and hunger and food craving in adults: an ecological momentary assessment study. Appetite 196:107241. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2024.107241

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cepeda-Benito, A., Gleaves, D. H., Williams, T. L., and Erath, S. A. (2000). The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav. Ther. 31, 151–173. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80009-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalmers, R. P. (2012). mirt: a multidimensional item response theory package for the R environment. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, A. M., Grilo, C. M., and Sinha, R. (2016). Food cravings, binge eating, and eating disorder psychopathology: exploring the moderating roles of gender and race. Eat. Behav. 21, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.12.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chun, S., Stark, S., Kim, E. S., and Chernyshenko, O. S. (2016). MIMIC methods for detecting DIF among multiple groups: exploring a new sequential-free baseline procedure. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 40, 486–499. doi: 10.1177/0146621616659738

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DiFeliceantonio, A. G., Coppin, G., Rigoux, L., Thanarajah, S. E., Dagher, A., Tittgemeyer, M., et al. (2018). Supra-additive effects of combining fat and carbohydrate on food reward. Cell Metab. 28, 33–44.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, L. H., Lin, H., Carr, K. A., and Fletcher, K. D. (2012). Food reinforcement and obesity. Psychological moderators. Appetite 58, 157–162. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazzino, T. L. (2022). The reinforcing natures of hyper-palatable foods: behavioral evidence for their reinforcing properties and the role of the US food industry in promoting their availability. Curr. Addict. Rep. 9. doi: 10.1007/s40429-022-00417-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazzino, T. L., Rohde, K., and Sullivan, D. K. (2019). Hyper-palatable foods: development of a quantitative definition and application to the US food system database. Obesity 27, 1761–1768. doi: 10.1002/oby.22639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallam, J., Boswell, R. G., DeVito, E. E., and Kober, H. (2016). Gender-related differences in food craving and obesity. Yale J. Biol. Med. 89, 161–173.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hill, A. J. (2007). The psychology of food craving. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 66, 277–285. doi: 10.1017/S0029665107005502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hormes, J. M., Orloff, N. C., and Timko, C. A. (2014). Chocolate craving and disordered eating. Beyond the gender divide? Appetite 83, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Imperatori, C., Innamorati, M., Tamburello, S., Continisio, M., Contardi, A., Tamburello, A., et al. (2013). Gender differences in food craving among overweight and obese patients attending low energy diet therapy: a matched case-control study. Eat Weight Disord. 18, 297–303. doi: 10.1007/s40519-013-0054-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ince, B., Schlatter, J., Max, S., Plewnia, C., Zipfel, S., Giel, K. E., et al. (2021). Can we change binge eating behaviour by interventions addressing food-related impulsivity? A systematic review. J. Eat. Disord. 9:38. doi: 10.1186/s40337-021-00384-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leslie, M., Turton, R., Burgess, E., Nazar, B. P., and Treasure, J. (2018). Testing the addictive appetite model of binge eating: tHE importance of craving, coping, and reward enhancement. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 26, 541–550. doi: 10.1002/erv.2621

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, F. M. (1986). Maximum likelihood and bayesian parameter estimation in item response theory. J. Educ. Meas. 23, 157–162. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-3984.1986.tb00241.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Masters, G. N. (2016). “Partial credit model,” in Handbook of Item Response Theory (New York, NY: Chapman and Hall/CRC).

Google Scholar

Meade, A. W. (2010). A taxonomy of effect size measures for the differential functioning of items and scales. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 728–743. doi: 10.1037/a0018966

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A. (2020). Twenty years of the food cravings questionnaires: a comprehensive review. Curr. Addict. Rep. 7, 30–43. doi: 10.1007/s40429-020-00294-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., and Hormes, J. M. (2015). Chocolate versions of the food cravings questionnaires. Associations with chocolate exposure-induced salivary flow and ad libitum chocolate consumption. Appetite 91, 256–265. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Richard, A., Dinic, R., and Blechert, J. (2019). Effects of a smartphone-based approach-avoidance intervention on chocolate craving and consumption: randomized controlled trial. JMIR mHealth and uHealth 7:e12298. doi: 10.2196/12298

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pannicke, B., Blechert, J., Reichenberger, J., and Kaiser, T. (2022). Clustering individuals' temporal patterns of affective states, hunger, and food craving by latent class vector-autoregression. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 19:57. doi: 10.1186/s12966-022-01293-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reckase, M. D. (1997). The past and future of multidimensional item response theory. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 21, 25–36. doi: 10.1177/0146621697211002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reents, J., and Pedersen, A. (2021). Differences in food craving in individuals with obesity with and without binge eating disorder. Front. Psychol. 12:660880. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.660880

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rejeski, W. J., Blumenthal, T. D., Miller, G. D., Lobe, M., Davis, C., Brown, L., et al. (2010). State craving, food availability, and reactivity to preferred snack foods. Appetite 54, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, A., Meule, A., and Blechert, J. (2019). Implicit evaluation of chocolate and motivational need states interact in predicting chocolate intake in everyday life. Eat. Behav. 33, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, A., Meule, A., Reichenberger, J., and Blechert, J. (2017). Food cravings in everyday life: an EMA study on snack-related thoughts, cravings, and consumption. Appetite 113, 215–223. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.02.037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roefs, A., Boh, B., Spanakis, G., Nederkoorn, C., Lemmens, L. H. J. M., and Jansen, A. (2019). Food craving in daily life: comparison of overweight and normal-weight participants with ecological momentary assessment. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 32, 765–774. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12693

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, L. M., Forester, G., Burr, E. K., Laam, L., Crosby, R. D., Peterson, C. B., et al. (2023). Examining the role of craving in affect regulation models of binge eating: Evidence from an ecological momentary assessment study. J. Psychopathol. Clin. Sci. 132, 725–732. doi: 10.1037/abn0000839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, R. S., Enns, C. W., Martin, C. L., Goldman, J. D., and Moshfegh, A. J. (2010). “Sweet foods consumption by adults in the U.S. what we eat in america, NHANES 2015-2018,” in FSRG Dietary Data Briefs (Beltsville, MD: United States Department of Agriculture (USDA)).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Stopyra, M. A., Friederich, H-.C., Lavandier, N., Mönning, E., Bendszus, M., Herzog, W., et al. (2021). Homeostasis and food craving in obesity: a functional MRI study. Int. J. Obes. 45, 2464–2470. doi: 10.1038/s41366-021-00920-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, W., and Kober, H. (2020). Regulating food craving: From mechanisms to interventions. Physiol. Behav. 222, 112878. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapper, K., and Turner, A. (2018). The effect of a mindfulness-based decentering strategy on chocolate craving. Appetite 130, 157–162. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M. (2019). A review of food craving measures. Eat. Behav. 32, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

U. S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service (2023). What We Eat in America, NHANES August 2021 - August 2023. Beltsville, MD: U. S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service.

Google Scholar

White, M. A., Whisenhunt, B. L., Williamson, D. A., Greenway, F. L., and Netemeyer, R. G. (2002). Development and validation of the food-craving inventory. Obes. Res. 10, 107–114. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.17

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, Y., Miller, R., and Groth, S. W. (2022). A literature review of dopamine in binge eating. J. Eat. Disord. 10:11. doi: 10.1186/s40337-022-00531-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zenk, S. N., Powell, L. M., Isgor, Z., Rimkus, L., Barker, D. C., Chaloupka, F. J., et al. (2015). Prepared food availability in U.S. food stores: a national study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 49, 553–562. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.02.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zorjan, S., and Schienle, A. (2023). Temporal dynamics of mental imagery, craving and consumption of craved foods: an experience sampling study. Psychol. Health 38, 1443–1459. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2022.2033239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: food craving, sex difference, item response theory, differential item functioning, chocolate, Food Craving Inventory

Citation: Jun D, Joo S and Fazzino TL (2025) Beyond general food craving: sex differences in food-specific craving identified using item response theory. Front. Psychol. 16:1588999. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1588999

Received: 06 March 2025; Accepted: 08 May 2025;
Published: 17 June 2025.

Edited by:

Boris C. Rodríguez-Martín, International University of La Rioja, Spain

Reviewed by:

Hui-Ting Cai, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
Eram Albajri, King Abdulaziz University, Saudi Arabia

Copyright © 2025 Jun, Joo and Fazzino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tera L. Fazzino, ZmF6emlub0BrdS5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.