Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 21 July 2025

Sec. Educational Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1611145

School-based randomized controlled trials for ADHD and accompanying impairments: a systematic review and meta-analysis

  • Department of Education, The University of York, York, United Kingdom

Introduction: Children and young people with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) face disproportionate challenges due to core symptoms of ADHD (i.e., inattention and hyperactivity) interfering with academic, social, and behavioural functioning. The significant rise in the prevalence of ADHD and difficulties experienced by children and young people over recent years has put a lot more emphasis on school-based interventions. Despite this, and the tremendous amount of randomised controlled trials (RCTs) reporting school-based interventions for ADHD over the past decades, there has been no systematic reporting of the pooled effects of such trials in the literature.

Methods: This study seeks, for the first time, to report the effects of school-based RCTs on the core symptoms of ADHD and other difficulties, i.e., academic, social, emotional and behavioural (i.e., externalising). Search was performed in three journals, for interventions from 1980 to 2024 that targeted school-aged children (4 to 18 years old) with ADHD.

Results: In total, 26 randomised controlled trials met the inclusion criteria, 22 (n = 1962) of which were included in our meta-analyses. Our findings showed that school-based based randomised controlled trials were effective in improving combined ADHD (d = −0.28, p < 0.0001), inattention (d = −0.33, p < 0.0001), academic performance (d = 0.37, p < 0.0001), and social skills (d = 0.28, p < 0.001), and reducing externalising problems (d = −0.32, p = 0.001). There was no significant effect for hyperactivity/impulsivity (d = −0.09, p = 0.22)

Discussion: Our findings showed that school-based randomised controlled trials improve a range of difficulties experienced by children and young people with ADHD, but lack focus on hyperactivity/impulsivity. Future interventions would benefit from a more comprehensive focus on hyperactivity/impulsivity for system-wide improvement. Issues related to high levels of heterogeneity and potential reporter bias are further discussed.

Introduction

Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is one of the most prevalent neurodevelopmental conditions, with latest figures showing a global rate of 8% in children and adolescents (Ayano et al., 2023). It is characterised by two core difficulties: inattentiveness, which are difficulties with sustaining attention, organisational skills, and following instructions; and hyperactivity/impulsivity, which presents as acting before thinking, excessive talking, and fidgeting (National Institute of Mental Health, 2024). Among the presentation types of ADHD, combined inattention and hyperactivity is the most prevalent (Ayano et al., 2020; Wilens et al., 2009). Most children and young people (CYP) with ADHD have at least one of the comorbid behavioural (e.g., conduct problems), emotional (e.g., anxiety, depression), or neurodevelopmental (e.g., autism) conditions (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2024; Gillberg et al., 2004). Such difficulties, alongside core symptoms of ADHD, often present additional negative consequences in other areas of functioning, such as social and academic skills (Mak et al., 2022; Wehmeier et al., 2010; Jensen et al., 2001).

In terms of social difficulties, CYP with ADHD experience higher levels of peer rejection and generally have less friends than their neurotypical peers (Ferretti et al., 2019; Singh et al., 2015; Mrug et al., 2012). This may be due to several reasons, such as difficulties in taking turns in games or in conversations, or in making and maintaining friendships; disruptive and impulsive behaviours, and having poor emotion regulation and self-esteem (Faraone et al., 2021; Ferretti et al., 2019; Van Stralen, 2016). Children with ADHD also tend to struggle with their interactions and relationships with adults. They appear to have more conflict with their parents and perceive less social support from their teachers (Faraone et al., 2021; Ferretti et al., 2019; Singh et al., 2015; Mrug et al., 2012). Such social experiences may be worsened due to co-occurring difficulties, such as autism spectrum conditions.

Children with ADHD also struggle academically, usually due to the core symptoms of ADHD interfering with skills that require sustained attention and behavioural control, such as executive function skills, inhibition and attentional control, and working memory (Fan and Wang, 2022; Roselló et al., 2020; Schreiber et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2011; Brocki et al., 2010). These are essential for academic tasks that require high attentional skills, such as maths and reading, also in keeping attendance and managing homework (Faraone et al., 2021; Loe and Feldman, 2007). Hence, CYP often perform below average in academic attainment and are at high risk for school dropout (Arnold et al., 2020; Fredriksen et al., 2014), which can lead to difficulties with occupational performance, job stability, and career development (Gordon and Fabiano, 2019). Taken together, compared to their non-ADHD peers, children and adolescents with ADHD are at a greater risk for poor academic outcomes and future achievement.

ADHD and accompanying difficulties often become more evident once children reach school age (Salari et al., 2023) even though the symptoms can emerge earlier (Miller et al., 2023). One reason for this is because, during preschool years, children are not expected to deal with complex daily tasks and social interactions as they are when they start school. Therefore, being presented with additional requirements in educational settings tends to reveal the difficulties associated with ADHD. Another reason is that teachers might be better equipped to identify children that significantly differ from their non-ADHD peers than parents (Nafi et al., 2020) and in general, teachers play a key role in recognising children’s difficulties (Akdağ, 2023; Daley and Birchwood, 2010). Given that core ADHD symptoms tend to unfold within school settings where children spend a substantial amount of time, schools are often expected to play a central role in supporting these children and adolescents.

A wide range of school-based interventions has been developed and implemented to support various areas of functioning in CYP with ADHD (Daley and Birchwood, 2010; DuPaul et al., 2012; DuPaul and Eckert, 1997), one of which is multimodal treatments with a direct focus on the core difficulties of ADHD. Multimodal approaches also target other areas of functioning, such as cognitive skills (Capodieci et al., 2017; Egeland et al., 2013), auditory and visual attention (Chacona, 2008), organisation and planning (Langberg et al., 2012), academic (Owens et al., 2020) and social skills (Evans et al., 2023; Lan et al., 2018), which would indirectly promote improvements in the core difficulties of ADHD. These interventions often include behavioural components, such as antecedent and consequent-based strategies, which manipulate the environment to reduce or encourage a certain behaviour (DuPaul et al., 2011). There are also some interventions that aim to create a bridge between school and home environment (e.g., Pfiffner et al., 2016), some of which include components like psychoeducation for parents and teachers, where they are taught behavioural modification techniques (Hornstra et al., 2023). Similarly, interventions focusing on cognitive training (e.g., neurofeedback, executive functions), therapeutic approaches (e.g., mindfulness therapies), and educational programs in schools (e.g., individualised programs, accommodations) are also common (Nazarova et al., 2022; Dahl et al., 2020; Ferrin et al., 2016; Fabiano et al., 2009).

School-based ADHD interventions have many advantages, such as accessibility, cost, and feasibility as they can be embedded into daily classroom activities and delivered at a large scale. Classroom-based psychosocial interventions are found to be beneficial for improving ADHD symptoms, with effects transcending to conduct problems, social skills, and on-task behaviour (Fabiano et al., 2015; Daley et al., 2014; DuPaul et al., 2012; Miranda et al., 2006). They are promising to improve certain outcomes especially where clinical interventions (e.g., medication use) have very small effects, such as academic performance (Fabiano et al., 2009). However, research shows that the scope and mechanisms of interventions for ADHD vary significantly, and more research is needed to understand if and how these interventions work to tackle difficulties experienced by children with ADHD.

Rationales for the current study

There have been many attempts to systematically investigate the effectiveness of school-based interventions on core ADHD symptoms and accompanying impairments. While the earliest form of such meta-analyses showed that school-based interventions are somewhat beneficial for improving core symptoms of ADHD and academic skills (DuPaul and Eckert, 1997), later evidence with control conditions found no significant effects of such interventions (DuPaul et al., 2012). A more up-to-date evaluation (Moore et al., 2018) has also shown inconsistent findings for the effectiveness of school-based ADHD interventions, alongside reporting rater bias. While these reviews have provided insights into the effectiveness of such interventions for ADHD, several important gaps remain. For instance, Moore et al. (2018) reported the efficacy of randomised trials by intervention type which considerably reduced the sample power and lacked a general picture of overall effectiveness of such interventions. Additionally, DuPaul et al. (2012) had methodological limitations such as reporting trials with various designs, including those with single-subject. Hence, despite an abundance of research trials delivering school-based interventions to improve the core difficulties of ADHD and accompanying impairments, the overall efficacy of such trials has remained unclear as past evidence appears inconclusive.

To close this gap in the literature and provide up-to-date evidence, we conducted the most comprehensive review to date, investigating the efficacy of school-based randomised controlled trials (RCTs) targeting core difficulties of ADHD and accompanying impairments published between 1980 and 2024. In doing this, we sought to address the following research questions: (1) What are the key characteristics of school-based randomised controlled trials targeting core difficulties of ADHD and accompanying impairments? (2) How effective, if at all, are school-based randomised controlled trials in improving core difficulties of ADHD and accompanying impairments? (3) Does the type of reporter (e.g., teacher, parent) moderate the pooled effect size of the school-based interventions for ADHD and accompanying impairments? (4) Does the efficacy of school-based interventions change depending on school age (i.e., primary vs. secondary)? By combining data from high quality RCTs, this study seeks a more cumulative approach in answering whether school-based interventions work for children and young people with ADHD and aims to inform school practices, policy decisions, and future interventions.

Methods

Target design

This review targeted randomised controlled trials. Hence, studies with all other experimental designs (e.g., quasi-experimental, within/between-subject, pre- and post-test) were kept out of the scope of the current study.

Target sample

The target sample was school-aged children (4–18 years old; age of compulsory education in the UK), who had a formal diagnosis of ADHD or those who met diagnostic cut-off scores on a standardised ADHD scale.

Outcome variables

The target primary outcomes of the current review were core ADHD difficulties (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and combined ADHD). The secondary target outcomes were behavioural difficulties accompanying ADHD, i.e., difficulties with academic and social skills and externalising problems. In this review, externalising problems are conceptualised as the symptoms of Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and Conduct Disorder (CD). Studies that did not report any of the primary outcomes were excluded from the current review.

Intervention characteristics

The target interventions were those that took place in school settings and targeted ADHD and accompanying difficulties of school-aged children with ADHD. Interventions that were outside school settings (e.g., home) and those that were focused on other target groups (e.g., parents) were excluded, unless they had a child component at school and the child outcomes were reported separately (e.g., interventions with a component of parental training).

Study selection procedure

This review followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA, Shamseer et al., 2015) guideline. Following the PRISMA principles, the first author BY screened the ERIC, PsycINFO, and PubMed databases using Boolean search strings. To increase the likelihood of the identification of the relevant studies, following search strings were defined based on the target sample and intervention: “adhd” AND “school” AND “intervention.” The following synonymous terms were also searched for maximising the study identification results: For “school”; school-based OR classroom OR classroom-based; for “ADHD”; attention deficit*, OR hyperactiv*; and for “intervention”; treatment OR education OR training (see Table 1). Finally, all search strings were screened in all fields (i.e., title, abstract, and full text). All study titles and abstracts were screened and organised systematically in Endnote. If needed, any discrepancies or uncertainties regarding the eligibility of a paper were resolved through a discussion between the first and third author. To be included, a study had to meet the following criteria: (a) reported a randomised controlled trial, (b) delivered a child-focused intervention in a school setting, (c) consisted of children clinically diagnosed with ADHD or used a valid ADHD assessment tool, (d) reported core ADHD difficulties (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, ADHD), and (e) published in English between 1980 and 2024.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Study selection key term strings.

Data extraction

We extracted two types of data, descriptive and quantified. In terms of descriptive data, we extracted information related to the following: sample characteristics (including gender distribution and medication use), experimental design, intervention type, intervention duration, intervention setting/school type, intervention administrator, outcome reporter, and intervention components. In terms of quantified data, we extracted sample sizes, means and standard deviations, and, where necessary, standard errors and reported effect sizes. Descriptive information and quantified data for studies that were published pre-2018 were obtained from the most up-to-date systematic review and meta-analysis (i.e., Moore et al., 2018). For post-2018 studies, the first author extracted relevant descriptive information and quantified data for each target outcome. In case there were multiple reported outcomes per domain that were reported by different reporters (e.g., parent and teacher reports on academic skills), we extracted all outcomes and conducted the meta-analyses with first reported outcomes per rater in each domain. However, in cases where there were multiple reported outcomes per domain by the same reporter (e.g., parental reports on two academic scales), we only extracted the first reported outcome to prevent selective outcome reporting; an approach that has been consistently applied in previous studies (i.e., Francis et al., 2022; Deniz et al., 2024).

Quality appraisal

The methodological quality of the included studies, published post-2018, was evaluated by the first author. For this we used the adapted Cochrane risk of bias tool (Higgins et al., 2011). The answers included Yes, No, Not applicable, and No response in the following categories: randomisation and allocation concealment (selection bias), blinding (detection bias), response rate and fidelity (attrition bias), follow up results, missing outcome data and selective reporting. For pre-2018 studies, we extracted quality appraisals from Moore et al.’s paper (2018).

Data analysis

All analyses were performed in R (Version 4.2.1). Due to high heterogeneity, which is a common issue in the field (Deniz et al., 2024), a random effect model with inverse-variance weighted was utilised. Furthermore, standardised mean differences were reported using Cohen’s d and heterogeneity was reported using I2 statistics. Scales for ADHD and externalising problems were coded in the same direction with lower scores indicating lower levels of difficulties. That is, a negative effect size indicates reduced level of problems for ADHD and externalising problems. For academic and social skills, a positive score means improvement in the treatment group. Regarding the outcome type, some studies reported core difficulties of ADHD separately, that is, inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity. In cases where a study did not report an overall ADHD outcome, we combined inattention and hyperactivity subscales and derived combined ADHD scores to boost the sample power. Additionally, we performed meta-regression analyses to report whether the efficacies of trials were moderated by the type of reporter (i.e., child, parent, teacher, and observer) or school type (i.e., primary school: 4–11 years old, secondary school: 12–17 years old). Finally, publication bias was reported by visually inspecting funnel plot asymmetry and using Egger’s test.

Results

Study selection results

Boolean search strings identified 11479 studies since 2018 in the ERIC, PsycINFO, and PubMed databases. As the first step, a filter on PubMed, along with a title and abstract screening on ERIC and PsycINFO to eliminate non-randomised studies were applied, which resulted in the exclusion of 10932 studies. Duplicates (k = 9) were also removed. Hence, 538 studies were title screened. Of these, 154 studies were carried into abstract screening, 97 of which were further excluded due to irrelevance. Therefore, remaining 57 studies were full-text screened which resulted in the exclusion of 50 studies due to trials not meeting the inclusion criteria for the following reasons: intervention setting (k = 11), sample characteristics (k = 7), study aims (k = 6), study design (k = 4), target group (k = 9), target outcome (k = 9), and confounding factors (k = 2). Finally, two studies reported follow-up results of their precedents, hence, were excluded due to lack of follow up analysis in this paper. Additionally, one study (Capodieci et al., 2017) was excluded due to significant baseline inequivalence between the intervention and control groups but was kept for systematic review. Finally, 28 studies that were included in Moore et al. (2018) were further screened against our eligibility criteria, nine of which were excluded, while three of these were only included in the systematic review, due to inadequate information on interventions (k = 1), pre-test differences between groups (k = 1) (Seeley et al., 2009), and small sample size (k = 1). Hence, 26 studies (pre-2018 = 19, post-2018 = 7) were included in this systematic review, 22 of which were carried into the meta-analysis. Detailed information on database screening can be seen in the PRISMA flow diagram (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flowchart of a systematic review process. Identification phase starts with 11,479 articles; 10,941 removed as non-randomized or duplicates. Screening involves 154 records; 57 full-text articles assessed. Exclusions include various reasons like different target groups and irrelevant outcomes. Seven articles proceed. Inclusion adds 19 from a previous dataset, totaling 26 for review and 22 for meta-analysis.

Figure 1. Prisma flow diagram.

Study characteristics

The characteristics of the included studies are summarised here in brief. Detailed information is shown in Table 2. The included studies (k = 26) had a total sample of 2102 participants. The majority of studies (N = 24) were journal articles, while two studies were parts of dissertations, of which 18 were conducted in the USA, and the rest were from Iran (k = 2), China (k = 1), Italy (k = 1), Norway (k = 1), USA & Canada (k = 1), USA & China (k = 1), and USA and Mexico (k = 1). Finally, most interventions (k = 19) were conducted in primary schools, highlighting their focus on early intervention strategies.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intervention demographics of included studies.

Intervention characteristics

Below information is further included in Table 3. Of 26 included studies, 21 reported intervention durations, of which, students received 38.87 hours of treatment on average. However, the total intervention length drastically varied, where the shortest intervention was 25 minutes, and the longest was 360 hours. There were six studies that conducted more than one form of intervention, and the type of interventions was also significantly different, which were: Multimodal (k = 11), Cognitive training (k = 6), Neurofeedback (k = 4), Self-monitoring (k = 2), Study and Organisational skills (k = 2), Daily report card (k = 2), Relaxation Training (k = 1), Social Skills Training (k = 1), and Task modification (k = 1) (Table 3). It is important to highlight that post-2018 studies delivered interventions that were not captured by pre-2018 studies (e.g., CLS- FUERTE in Mexico). In terms of who delivered the interventions, 24 out of 26 studies reported this information, where the majority (k = 17) were delivered by school staff, e.g., teachers and school mental health practitioners, while the rest was delivered by others, such as doctoral students (k = 4), and trained coaches, therapists or researchers (k = 3). Finally, in terms of control conditions, the most common control type was community care (k = 9), followed by treatment as usual (k = 8), waitlist control (k = 6), and placebo (k = 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Intervention characteristics of included studies.

Some core practices repeatedly emerged across interventions, highlighting a shared approach in addressing ADHD difficulties in children. As discussed in the literature (Richardson et al., 2015), intervention components varied greatly, combining both classroom management strategies and skills training. The majority of the interventions in our review adopted a skills-based model that aimed to equip children with specific cognitive, organisational, behavioural, academic, and social competencies to manage ADHD symptoms and related difficulties. Providing children with guided feedback, prompting, encouraging active participation and role-playing, setting and monitoring target behaviours, and reflection were key components across interventions. Classroom-based strategies (e.g., daily report card, preferential seating, and use of praise) and parent training were also commonly incorporated. Interventions were typically delivered in a structured format, through didactic teaching, reinforcement (e.g., token economies or point-based reward systems), and practice. Some cognitive interventions also emphasised performance-based progression, emphasising developmental sensitivity and tailored approach to learning and change. Centered around social skills, organisation and planning, and neurocognition, strategies were predominantly child-contingent for enduring and transferable skill-building. More detailed information on intervention components is included in Table 3.

Study outcomes

All included studies reported core ADHD outcomes; ADHD (k = 22), inattention (k = 16), and hyperactivity/impulsivity (k = 16). Some studies also reported ADHD-accompanying impairments such as difficulties in academic skills (k = 14), social skills (k = 13), and externalising problems (k = 9). Measurements for each outcome are summarised here in brief and detailed information is provided in Table 4. Any measurement taken from Moore et al. (2018) was retained for consistency.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Scales per study per outcome.

ADHD

A wide range of scales was used to assess ADHD: Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale-4 (k = 2), Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale-5 (k = 1), Child Symptom Inventory-4 (k = 3), Conners Rating Scale (k = 6), Disruptive Behaviour Disorders (k = 5), Matching Familiar Figures Test (k = 2), ADHD-5 (k = 1), Clinical Global Impression Scale (k = 1), Continuous Performance Test (k = 1), Early Identification of ADHD (k = 1); Swanson, Kotkin, Agler, M-Flynn, and Pelham Scale (k = 1); Swanson, Nolan and Pelham Scale (k = 1), Social Skills Rating System (k = 1), Test of Variables of Attention (k = 1), and Vanderbilt (k = 1).

A higher number of studies (k = 8) had ADHD-combined as the dominant subtype within their samples, followed by inattention (k = 3) and hyperactivity (k = 2). However, the majority of studies (k = 13) did not report this finding. For the use of ADHD stimulants across control and treatment groups, of 26 included studies, only 4 explicitly excluded children on medication, 6 did not report medication status, and 20 included children on medication at varying levels.

Academic skills

Academic skills were measured using a wide range of scales including standardised tests, grades, and non-standardised measures. Cognitive skills, e.g., executive function and working memory, were also categorised under the academic skills as they highly positively correlated. In terms of standardised measures, the following scales were used: Academic Competence Evaluation Scale-short form (k = 1), Academic Performance Rating Scale (k = 1), Adolescent Academic Problems Checklist (k = 1), Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (k = 2), California Verbal Learning Test (k = 1), Conners’ Classroom Performance Survey (k = 1), Continuous Performance Test (k = 1), Early Identification of ADHD-Working memory subscale (k = 1), Homework Problems Checklist (k = 1), Impairment Rating Scale (k = 1), Social Skills Rating Scale-Academic subscale (k = 1), and Children’s Organisational Skills Scales (k = 1). Additionally, the following non-standardised measures and school grades were used: percentage of assignments turned in or work completed (k = 3), math and reading scores (k = 2), executive function (k = 1) and working capacity (k = 1).

Social skills

While some studies reported negative aspects of social skills (e.g., social skills impairments), some others focused on positive aspects (e.g., social skills improvement). Measures reporting children’s social skills impairments were as follows: Impairment Rating Scale (k = 3), Children’s Organisational Skills Scale-Impairment-Family conflict (k = 1), Classroom Performance Survey-Interpersonal performance subscore (k = 1), Classroom Life Measure-Teacher support (k = 1), Conflict Behaviour Questionnaire–20–Parent-teen conflict (k = 1), Conners–Peer Relations (k = 1) and Asocial subscore (k = 1). Scales that focused on social skills improvement were: Social Adjustment Inventory for Children and Adolescents-Interaction with peers subscore (k = 1), Social Skills Improvement System (k = 3), Social Skills Rating System (k = 1), Social preference sociometric measures (k = 1), Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (k = 1) and Walker-McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment (k = 1).

Externalising problems

Standardised measures were used to assess varying aspects of externalising problems such as aggression, disruptive behaviours, conduct problems and oppositional defiant disorder. Specific measures to report children’s externalising problems were as follows: Conners Teacher Rating Scale (k = 3), Disruptive Behaviours Disorder Rating Scale (k = 3), Child Symptom Inventory-4 (k = 2), Children’s Organisational Skills Scale-Impairment-Life Interference (k = 1), Social Skills Rating Scale (k = 1) and Teachers’ Report Form (k = 1).

Quality appraisal

Quality appraisal of pre-2018 studies were gathered from Moore et al. (2018) and post-2018 studies were evaluated by the first author. Of the included studies, 6 showed high risk of bias, 12 showed some concerns, and 8 showed low risk of bias. There were some concerns regarding randomisation procedure and allocation concealment as only few studies reported these. In regards to blinding, there were again some concerns as in most studies (k = 20) the assessors were aware of participants’ assigned groups. Furthermore, intervention fidelity appeared to be good across most of the included studies as 19 out of 24 studies had above 85% intervention fidelity, while two papers did not report this finding or students’ attendance. Additionally, only a small number of included studies (k = 9) shared follow-up results. Finally, only 12 out of 26 studies reported missing data and how it was handled. Detailed information regarding quality appraisal can be seen in Table 5.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Risk of bias analysis.

Meta-analytic findings

In total, 22 unique studies were meta-analysed. Some studies were included more than once for certain outcomes due to having different reporters (e.g., parents, teachers, self-reports). Hence, total number of studies included in each meta-analysis were as follows: ADHD (k = 35), inattention (k = 25), hyperactivity/inattention (k = 25), academic skills (k = 22), social skills (k = 19), externalising problems (k = 12). For each meta-analysis, we checked the presence of publication bias. Visual inspection of funnel plot asymmetry raised concerns over potential publication bias for ADHD and inattention outcomes which was also confirmed by Egger’s test findings: ADHD (t = –3.12, df = 33, p < 0.01), inattention (t = –2.41, df = 23, p = 0.024). Detailed findings for publication bias can be seen in Supplementary Figures S2–S7.

ADHD

Pooling effect sizes across studies reporting ADHD outcomes (Figure 2, n = 4256) resulted in a small to medium and significant negative effect size (d = –0.28, 95% CI [–0.39, –0.17], p < 0.0001) suggesting that children with ADHD who received a school-based intervention showed significantly lower levels of ADHD than those in the control conditions. Meta-regression analysis revealed that while the type of rater did not explain a significant amount of variance in the pooled effect size (QM = 3.03, p > 0.05), interventions were more effective in primary than secondary school settings (QM = 5.34, 𝑝 = 0.021). However, the pooled effect size suffered from significant heterogeneity for ADHD outcomes (I2 = 61%, τ2 = 0.06, p < 0.01) which may suggest these interventions may not share a common effect size.

Figure 2
Forest plot summarizing the standardized mean differences from various studies. Each study is listed with its effect size, confidence interval, and weights. The plot includes both fixed and random effects models, depicted by a diamond at the bottom. Heterogeneity details are shown with I-squared, tau-squared, and p-value indicating significant heterogeneity.

Figure 2. Forest plot for ADHD.

Inattention

Studies reporting inattention outcomes (Figure 3, n = 1933) resulted in a medium negative effect size (d = –0.33, 95% CI [–0.48, –0.19], p < 0.0001) suggesting improvements in inattention in children who were exposed to a school-based intervention than those who were not. However, meta-regression findings revealed that reporter type significantly moderated the effectiveness of interventions (QM = 27.16, p < 0.001). Specifically, compared to the child reports (i.e., reference category), teachers (β = 0.47, p = 0.014, 95% CI [0.10, 0.84]) and parents (β = 0.41, p = 0.030, 95% CI [0.04, 0.79]) perceived significantly less improvements in children's inattention symptoms post-intervention. On the contrary, observers reported significantly higher gains in inattention symptoms than child reports (β = −1.17, p = 0.002, 95% CI [−1.91, −0.43]), although this suffered from power issues as only one outcome was observer-reported. Similar to the combined ADHD outcome, interventions targeting primary school-aged children were significantly more effective to improve attention skills than those delivered in secondary school settings (QM = 4.55, 𝑝 = 0.03). There was also significant heterogeneity (I2 = 52%, τ2 = 0.06, p < 0.01) which may indicate that such trials may not share a common effect size on inattention.

Figure 3
Forest plot comparing standardized mean differences across multiple studies, depicting individual study results as squares with confidence intervals as horizontal lines. Weights for common and random effects models are shown, alongside pooled estimates depicted as diamonds, suggesting overall negative effects. Heterogeneity statistics are included below.

Figure 3. Forest plot for inattention.

Hyperactivity/Impulsivity

Studies reporting hyperactivity/impulsivity (Figure 4, n = 1933) showed no significant effects (d = –0.09, 95% CI [–0.22,0.05], p = 0.22) meaning that individuals who received school-based interventions did not improve their hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms compared to those who did not. Similar to inattention, observers reported significantly greater treatment effects for hyperactivity symptoms (QM = 15.42, p < 0.01) compared to the reference group (i.e., child outcomes) (β = −1.39, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−2.09, −0.69]). However, it is important to bear in mind that there was only one observer-reported hyperactivity/impulsivity outcome included in the meta-regression analysis of hyperactivity, and parental or teacher reports did not statistically differ from the reference group.

Figure 4
Forest plot showing standardized mean differences (SMD) from multiple studies with confidence intervals. Each study is listed with its SMD and corresponding weights for common and random effects. The plot indicates variability in study results, with a summary effect size shown at the bottom. Heterogeneity is reported as I squared equals forty-eight percent.

Figure 4. Forest plot for hyperactivity/impulsivity.

Academic skills

For studies reporting academic skills (Figure 5, n = 2138), we found a significant moderate positive effect size (d = 0.37, 95% CI [0.23 0.50], p < 0.0001), that is, individuals who received school-based interventions improved their academic skills beyond those who did not. Similar to the ADHD combined, reporter type did not moderate the pooled effect size (QM = 2.78, p > 0.05) and interventions appeared to be more effective in primary than secondary school settings (QM = 5.48, p = 0.01). Like other outcomes, the pooled effect size suffered from significant heterogeneity (I2 = 52%, τ2 = 0.05, p < 0.01).

Figure 5
Forest plot showing standardized mean differences for various studies, each represented by a square (size indicates study weight) and horizontal line (confidence interval). A red diamond represents the overall random effects model result (0.37, confidence interval 0.23 to 0.50). Common effect model result is slightly lower (0.32, confidence interval 0.23 to 0.41). Heterogeneity is moderate with \(I^2 = 52\%\). Studies include Egeland 2013, multiple Evans studies, Fabiano 2010, Lan 2018, Langberg 2012, Mikami 2021, Nejati 2023, Pfiffner 2016, Sibley 2018, Smith 2017, with varying confidence intervals and weights.

Figure 5. Forest plot for academic skills.

Social skills

Studies reporting social skills (Figure 6, n = 1991) revealed a significant small to medium positive effect size (d = 0.28, 95% CI [0.13 0.43], p < 0.001) meaning that individuals who received a school-based intervention improved their social skills more than those who did not. Neither the type of reporter (QM = 3.31, p > 0.05) nor the school-level (QM = 0.29, p > 0.05) moderated the effectiveness of school-based interventions on social skills. As others, trials reporting social skills outcomes also suffered from significantly high heterogeneity (I2 = 57%, τ2 = 0.06, p < 0.01).

Figure 6
Forest plot showing standardized mean differences (SMD) for multiple studies with 95% confidence intervals. The plot includes a summary diamond for both common and random effects models. Weights for common and random models are provided. The heterogeneity statistics are I-squared at 57%, tau-squared at 0.0594, with a p-value less than 0.01.

Figure 6. Forest plot for social skills.

Externalising problems

Finally, studies reporting externalising problems (Figure 7, n = 1217) resulted in a negative, medium and significant effect size (d = –0.32, 95% CI [–0.51, –0.13], p = 0.001). That is, individuals who received a school-based intervention had lower levels of externalising problems than their peers who did not. Similar to social skills outcomes, the type of reporter did not moderate the pooled effect size (QM = 0.79, p > 0.05), same as the school type (QM = 1.19, p > 0.05). Finally, the pooled effect size for externalising problems also suffered from statistically high heterogeneity (I2 = 59%, τ2 = 0.06, p < 0.01).

Figure 7
Forest plot illustrating the standardized mean differences (SMD) for various studies. Each study is listed with its SMD, 95% confidence interval, and weights for common and random effects. The diamond shapes represent the pooled effect size for common and random effects models, indicating a negative overall effect. Heterogeneity is noted with I-squared at fifty-nine percent and a p-value less than 0.01, suggesting moderate variability among study results.

Figure 7. Forest plot for externalising problems.

Discussion

The current paper systematically reviewed and conducted a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of school-based interventions (1980-2024) on core ADHD symptoms and accompanying impairments, including academic, social and externalising problems. Overall, 26 studies met the eligibility criteria, 22 of which were included in the meta-analyses. We found that school-based interventions were effective in reducing symptoms related to ADHD (combined), inattention, and externalising problems and improving academic and social skills in school-aged children with ADHD. Such interventions appeared to be more effective if delivered in primary school settings compared to secondary school settings, except for social skills and externalising problems, where school type did not make a difference on the observed effects. Additionally, school-based interventions appeared to be ineffective in reducing hyperactivity/impulsivity. To note, our findings suffer from high heterogeneity and some level of potential reporter-bias, which are two common issues in the field.

Interventions in this review involved a combination of school adjustments and skill-training for children, latter of which was emphasised more frequently across studies. That is, interventions prioritised and targeted equipping children with social, organisational, academic and behavioural strategies, encouraging child’s agency in managing ADHD difficulties through reinforcing feedback and reflection, setting and evaluating goals, providing clear instructions and prompting. Furthermore, our meta-analyses statistically indicated that school-based interventions are effective in improving core ADHD difficulties of school-aged children with ADHD, mainly the symptoms of ADHD (combined) and inattention. These findings align with previous reviews and meta-analyses, which also reported significant positive effect sizes for school-based interventions targeting core ADHD difficulties (Moore et al., 2018; DuPaul et al., 2012; DuPaul and Eckert, 1997). An interesting observation from our review, however, was that the level of change in hyperactivity for children in the intervention groups was not statistically lower than their peers in the control groups. A previous meta-analysis presented similar results, arguing that school-based psychosocial interventions might be more effective in addressing inattention problems than hyperactivity (Richardson et al., 2015).

There may be several reasons for why school-based interventions appear to be effective in improving inattention symptoms but not hyperactivity. It is recognised that inattention and hyperactivity are pathophysiologically distinct constructs (Willcutt et al., 2012). On this ground, we could argue that school-based interventions may be explicitly, or more so, focused on improving attention-related skills (e.g., organisational, homework) than hyperactivity difficulties. Findings from a recent meta-analysis also supports this argument by showing varied strength levels for the interventions’ effectiveness on hyperactivity across studies, such as social skill or cognitive/neurofeedback interventions that had limited power on improving impulsivity (Sadr-Salek et al., 2023). Perhaps, inclusion of more physical and relaxation-based components in the intervention could make management of hyperactive/impulsive symptoms easier. Another explanation for this might be the distribution of ADHD types in study samples, where hyperactivity always had the smallest percentage, except the two studies with 100% hyperactive ADHD groups. Naturally, the preponderance of inattention would mean there were more opportunities to notice the changes in these symptoms than hyperactivity. It is also plausible that children with predominantly hyperactive symptoms may be more likely to receive medication earlier compared to their peers with inattentive presentation, due to the visibility and disruptiveness of impulsive behaviours in the classroom, some of which might have already been addressed by medication. Therefore, introducing a school-based intervention might have only produced small gains for this group, resulting in a possible ceiling effect. Our analyses would not have captured the within-group differences in these children if there were any.

Combined effects of school-based interventions also appear to improve the studied ADHD-accompanying difficulties. The significant gains in ADHD-related impairments in our paper reinforces the earlier findings, where psychosocial classroom interventions were effective in enhancing academic performance and social skills and reducing conduct problems, including off-task behaviour (Huang et al., 2021; De Siqueira et al., 2019; Harrison et al., 2019; Daley et al., 2014; DuPaul et al., 2012; Fabiano et al., 2009; Miranda et al., 2006). Comparatively, Moore et al. (2018) showed mixed results for ADHD-related impairment outcomes based on the intervention types and reporters. For instance, there was a positive impact of multimodal interventions on academic skills across all raters, on conduct problems for parents’ ratings (included under externalising problems in our paper), but no statistically meaningful improvement in social skills of children in the treatment groups compared to control groups. Our meta-analyses revealed similar results for academic skills, as we found significant benefits of interventions regardless of the type of reporter. Our meta-analytic findings of externalising skills were significant too, yet independent of the reporter type. Finally, our meta-analysis of social skills also yielded significant results, contrary to Moore et al. (2018)‘s findings on this outcome, although the effect size was slightly lower than the other two outcomes in this domain. Taken together, our results indicate that the pooled school-based ADHD interventions from 1980 to 2024 are promising in addressing academic and social skills as well as externalising problems of children with ADHD.

We also conducted meta-regression analyses for school types. Our findings highlighted the importance of early interventions for ADHD combined, inattention symptoms, and academic skills, as school-based interventions for primary school-aged children were better at targeting these domains than the ADHD interventions in secondary schools. Additionally, meta-regression analyses based on each rater (i.e., teacher, parent, child, observer) were performed to evaluate the potential influence of the reporter type for all measured outcomes. While there was no significant moderator effect of the outcome reporter on ADHD combined, which is similar to Moore et al. (2018), there was noticeable influence of raters on the observed effects for inattention and hyperactivity. However, it is important to note that for inattention, parents and teachers did not report as much impact of interventions as children observed. For hyperactivity, none of the reporters perceived differences between treatment and control groups after the interventions, only that one observer moderated the overall effects by reporting lower gains from the intervention compared to the child reports. Past research, including the precedent meta-analysis, underlined the dependence of interventions’ effectiveness on the type of intervention and reporter (Moore et al., 2018; Watabe et al., 2013). While the problem of reporter-bias is prevalent in the field, inconsistency in reporters’ understanding of the problem and focus on different target behaviours might explain the disagreement (Hartman et al., 2007). This would mean that the observed tasks and behaviours to report on ADHD symptoms differ among raters, as shown in our results, where teachers and parents perceived less improvement in inattention following the interventions compared to the child. Combined with this finding, the majority of the measures in the included studies relied on subjective ratings, it might be that the interventions are teaching children how to compensate for their symptoms to meet the demands and expectations set by the school, parents, and peers, especially when they are unblinded to the intervention conditions. Interventions that enforce compliance, classroom behaviour management, or external reinforcement systems may inadvertently teach children to mask overt signs of inattentiveness without addressing underlying neurocognitive difficulties. As the efficacy of school-based interventions on academic, social, and conduct problems of ADHD-related impairment appeared to be stable and consistent across different raters, it is likely that interventions are equipping children with strategies to apply in the classroom that effectively target these skills, which are equally visible to others, such as parents and teachers. As a final note, while some studies included observational and experimental tasks, more so in academic and social skills than the core ADHD symptoms, the majority of data in our analyses were drawn from subjective assessments. As such, the differences in raters’ observations, especially in inattention ratings, should reflect the difference in reporters’ subjective measures. It is important for future studies to incorporate multi-informant and objective measures, such as neurocognitive testing alongside standardised, self-report data to support observed effects and reduce bias.

We should also consider the high levels of statistical heterogeneity for all ADHD and ADHD-related impairment outcomes. This remains to be a common issue in the field, as seen in previous reviews focused on ADHD (Moore et al., 2018; DuPaul et al., 2012; DuPaul and Eckert, 1997) and in other neurodevelopmental subgroups of the population (e.g., autism; Deniz et al., 2022, 2024; Francis et al., 2022). Taken together, while present school-based interventions are effective in improving ADHD accompanying difficulties, the variance between study or intervention characteristics in meta-analyses, such as the outcome reporter, outcome measurement, intervention provider, and duration of the intervention (e.g., the shortest intervention lasted 25 minutes), complicates the interpretation of the pooled effects. This complexity may also partly reflect the heterogeneity inherent to ADHD itself, which presents differently across individuals. As such, it is possible that tailored or individualised interventions are better suited to meet the diverse needs of children with ADHD, and that variability across interventions highlight the need to personalise support. This flexibility in practice, however, also contributes to inconsistencies in intervention designs that may limit the interpretability and generalisability of meta-analytic findings. Balancing the individualisation in practice with standardisation in research might be key to advancing both the effectiveness and applicability of school-based ADHD interventions.

Strengths and limitations

This review and meta-analysis hold several strengths. First, following the PRISMA guideline in reviewing and reporting the existing evidence ensures no selective-reporting bias, which was further strengthened by taking the first-reported outcome by each rater per domain per study. Furthermore, inclusion of multiple reporters per outcome, wherever available, and performing meta-regression analyses also unpacked potential reporter-related bias, especially given that reporters were not blinded to child’s assigned study conditions in most cases, which triggers observer-expectancy effect. Third, while the inclusion of the grey literature is a clear strength, as it reduces the possibility of publication bias, it may as well introduce other types of bias due to inclusion of data that have not been rigorously peer reviewed. Fourth, inclusion of new interventions that were not studied by the previous meta-analysis (e.g., IBBS–Integrated Brain, Body and Social Intervention) as well as new cultural adaptations of some interventions (e.g., CLS-FUERTE in Mexico) enabled a cumulative and up-to-date presentation of the effectiveness of school-based ADHD interventions, even though the novel studies (i.e., 2018 onwards) couldn’t be statistically compared to the previous dataset due to the small sample size. Finally, considering the emergence of ADHD symptoms as early as age 4 and their strong link to the symptoms later in life (Lahey et al., 2016), for which it is important to design and evaluate early interventions to mitigate later academic and behavioural difficulties as shown in our subgroup analysis based on school type, we defined the age range of school-aged children to allow for a comprehensive understanding of ADHD interventions across developmental stages.

There were also some caveats. For example, we detected high heterogeneity in the meta-analyses of included studies across all outcomes. As discussed previously, this is a major problem and is prevalent in the meta-analysis of interventions that target such neurodiverse subgroups of the population, specifically autism and ADHD (Deniz et al., 2022, 2024; Francis et al., 2022; DuPaul et al., 2012). This may indicate that the included studies may not share a common effect size for the reported outcomes. Additionally, there was a considerable variation in intervention dosage, ranging from as little as 25 minutes to over 360 hours. Therefore, it was not possible to categorise interventions under few subgroups, in terms of their dosages, as any cut-off point or categorisation would have been arbitrary, and suffer from measurement error. Future reviews are strongly encouraged to investigate this matter, where possible. Moreover, our approach to choosing measurements for the meta-analyses aimed at continuity with a prior systematic review (Moore et al., 2018), as well as to increase validity of each studied domain, which meant that we included measurements in the same domains that Moore et al. (2018) reported. However, we recognise that the scopes of these scales were wide, including clinical performance measures, behavioural rating scales, and observational measures, which are different in terms of which aspect of the construct they measure. We therefore acknowledge that the included rating scales capture related constructs (e.g., core symptoms of ADHD) but different underlying mechanisms of these constructs (e.g., cognitive and behavioural aspects of ADHD). Combined with high heterogeneity, these findings should be interpreted cautiously. Another limitation is the lack of control for children who are prescribed a medicine for ADHD, as the majority of studies did not report this information. Nevertheless, by applying only between-group differences in this study, we aimed at omitting any positive/negative effects created by the medication use. Additionally, our approach to taking the first-reported outcome per rater in each target domain per study, although is a common practice in the field to reduce selective reporting bias (Deniz et al., 2022, 2024; Francis et al., 2022) does not rule out the possibility that applying a different data extraction strategy could provide different findings. Finally, unblinding and the lack of information on this matter in some of the included studies may mean that they may have promoted individuals’ masking behaviours instead of driving a meaningful change in core ADHD symptoms.

Conclusion and implications

Overall, we found that school-based interventions are effective in reducing symptoms of ADHD, potentially only inattention, and externalising problems, and in improving academic and social skills. We found no evidence that such interventions also improve hyperactivity/impulsivity. Based on this, we suggest that school-based interventions are designed in a way that they target individuals’ attention deficits more than their hyperactivity/impulsivity. It is not clear, from our findings, why children with hyperactivity/impulsivity did not respond to such school-based interventions. We suggest, for the first time, such interventions should be more inclusive to target children who mainly have hyperactivity/impulsivity difficulties. Additionally, stronger effect sizes found in primary school settings, compared to secondary schools for inattention and academic outcomes further highlighted the importance of early intervention strategies, for children with ADHD. High heterogeneity remains to be an important issue, hence, more consistent practices and clear guidelines are needed to understand what intervention works, for whom, and in what dosage. Our review also showed that schools and teachers might benefit from interventions that directly build students’ cognitive, behavioural, and social skills through structured routines, explicit instructions, and consistent reinforcement.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

BY: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Investigation. BB: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ED: Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

BY would like to thank Matthew Carter MSc, BSc, for his continuous support, insightful feedback, and dedicated contribution throughout the development of this work. His expertise and encouragement were invaluable at every stage of the process.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1611145/full#supplementary-material

References

Akdağ, B. (2023). Exploring teachers’ knowledge and attitudes toward attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment in a district of Turkey. Cureus 15, 1–10. doi: 10.7759/cureus.45342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, L. E., Hodgkins, P., Kahle, J., Madhoo, M., and Kewley, G. (2020). Long-term outcomes of ADHD: academic achievement and performance. J. Attention Disord. 24, 73–85. doi: 10.1177/1087054714566076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayano, G., Demelash, S., Gizachew, Y., Tsegay, L., and Alati, R. (2023). The global prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: An umbrella review of meta-analyses. J. Affect. Disord. 339, 860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.071

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayano, G., Yohannes, K., and Abraha, M. (2020). Epidemiology of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents in Africa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann. General Psychiatry 19, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12991-020-00271-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloomquist, M. L., August, G. J., and Ostrander, R. (1991). Effects of a school-based cognitive-behavioral intervention for ADHD children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 19, 591–605. doi: 10.1007/BF00925822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brocki, K. C., Eninger, L., Thorell, L. B., and Bohlin, G. (2010). Interrelations between executive function and symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention in preschoolers: a two year longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 38, 163–171. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9354-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Capodieci, A., Gola, M. L., Cornoldi, C., and Re, A. M. (2017). Effects of a working memory training program in preschoolers with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 17–29. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1307946

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, (2024). Data and statistics on ADHD. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/adhd/data/index.html (Accessed June 03, 2025).

Google Scholar

Chacona, S.M. (2008). Effect of world music drumming on auditory and visual attention skills of ADHD elementary students. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 68, 2817.

Google Scholar

Cloward, R.D. (2003). Self-monitoring increases time-on-task of attention deficit hyperactivity disordered students in the regular classroom. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 64, 1524.

Google Scholar

Dahl, V., Ramakrishnan, A., Spears, A. P., Jorge, A., Lu, J., Bigio, N. A., et al. (2020). Psychoeducation interventions for parents and teachers of children and adolescents with ADHD: A systematic review of the literature. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 32, 257–292. doi: 10.1007/s10882-019-09691-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, D., and Birchwood, J. (2010). ADHD and academic performance: why does ADHD impact on academic performance and what can be done to support ADHD children in the classroom? Child Care Health Dev. 36, 455–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01046.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, D., van der Oord, S., Ferrin, M., Danckaerts, M., Doepfner, M., Cortese, S., et al. (2014). Behavioral interventions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials across multiple outcome domains. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 835–847.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Siqueira, A. R. C., Silva, M. M. M., de Paula, E. T., de Araújo, M. V., Teixeira, M. C. T. V., and Carreiro, L. R. R. (2019). Effects of a behavioral intervention in a school setting on hyperactivity and inattention. Psicologia 21, 102–118.

Google Scholar

Deniz, E., Francis, G., Torgerson, C., and Toseeb, U. (2022). Parent-mediated play-based interventions to improve social communication and language skills of preschool autistic children: A systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. PLoS One 17:e0270153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0270153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deniz, E., Francis, G., Torgerson, C., and Toseeb, U. (2024). Parent-mediated play-based interventions to improve social communication and language skills of preschool autistic children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Rev. J. Autism Develop. Disord. 17. doi: 10.1007/s40489-024-00463-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., and Eckert, T. L. (1997). The Effects of School-based Interventions for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analysis. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 26, 5–27. doi: 10.1080/02796015.1997.12085845

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., Eckert, T. L., and Vilardo, B. (2012). The effects of school-based interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis 1996–2010. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 41, 387–412. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087496

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., Weyandt, L. L., and Janusis, G. M. (2011). ADHD in the classroom: effective intervention strategies. Theory Into Practice 50, 35–42. doi: 10.1080/00405841.2011.534935

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Egeland, J., Aarlien, A. K., and Saunes, B. K. (2013). Few effects of far transfer of working memory training in ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 8:e75660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075660

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. W., DuPaul, G. J., Benson, K., Owens, J. S., Qiong, F. U., Cleminshaw, C. L., et al. (2023). Social functioning outcomes of a high school-based treatment program for adolescents with ADHD. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol., 53, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2023.2235693

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., Schultz, B. K., Vaughn, A., Altaye, M., Marshall, S. A., et al. (2016). Evaluation of a school-based treatment program for young adolescents with ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 84, 15–30. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. W., Schultz, B. K., and DeMars, C. E. (2014). High school-based treatment for adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from a pilot study examining outcomes and dosage. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 43, 185–202. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2014.12087444

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, S. W., Schultz, B. K., DeMars, C. E., and Davis, H. (2011). Effectiveness of the challenging horizons after-school program for young adolescents with ADHD. Behav. Ther. 42, 462–474. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiano, G. A., Pelham, W. E., Coles, E. K., Gnagy, E. M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., and O’Connor, B. C. (2009). A meta-analysis of behavioral treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiano, G. A., Schatz, N. K., Aloe, A. M., Chacko, A., and Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2015). A systematic review of meta-analyses of psychosocial treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 18, 77–97. doi: 10.1007/s10567-015-0178-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiano, G. A., Vujnovic, R. K., Pelham, W. E., Waschbusch, D. A., Massetti, G. M., Pariseau, M. E., et al. (2010). Enhancing the effectiveness of special education programming for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using a daily report card. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 39, 219–239.

Google Scholar

Fan, L., and Wang, Y. (2022). The relationship between executive functioning and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children: A cross-lagged study. Curr. Psychol. 42, 21375–21383. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03233-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Banaschewski, T., Coghill, D., Zheng, Y., Biederman, J., Bellgrove, M. A., et al. (2021). The World Federation of ADHD International Consensus Statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferretti, N. M., King, S. L., Hilton, D. C., Rondon, A. T., and Jarrett, M. A. (2019). Social Functioning in Youth with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Sluggish Cognitive Tempo. Yale J. Biol. Med. 92, 29–35.

Google Scholar

Ferrin, M., Perez-Ayala, V., El-Abd, S., Lax-Pericall, T., Jacobs, B., Bilbow, A., et al. (2016). A Randomized Controlled Trial Evaluating the Efficacy of a Psychoeducation Program for Families of Children and Adolescents With ADHD in the United Kingdom: Results After a 6-Month Follow-Up. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 768–779. doi: 10.1177/1087054715626509

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, G., Deniz, E., Torgerson, C., and Toseeb, U. (2022). Play-based interventions for mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis focused on children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language disorder. Autism Develop. Lang. Impair. 7. doi: 10.1177/23969415211073118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredriksen, M., Dahl, A. A., Martinsen, E. W., Klungsoyr, O., Faraone, S. V., and Peleikis, D. E. (2014). Childhood and persistent ADHD symptoms associated with educational failure and long-term occupational disability in adult ADHD. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 6, 87–99. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0126-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I. C., Rasmussen, P., Kadesjö, B., Söderström, H., Råstam, M., et al. (2004). Co-existing disorders in ADHD -- implications for diagnosis and intervention. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 13, 180–192. doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-1008-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, C. T., and Fabiano, G. A. (2019). The Transition of Youth with ADHD into the Workforce: Review and Future Directions. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 22, 316–347. doi: 10.1007/s10567-019-00274-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haack, L. M., Araujo, E. A., Meza, J., Friedman, L. M., Spiess, M., Alcaraz Beltrán, D. K., et al. (2021). Can School Mental Health Providers Deliver Psychosocial Treatment Improving Youth Attention and Behavior in Mexico? A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial of CLS-FUERTE. J. Atten. Disord. 25, 2083–2097. doi: 10.1177/1087054720959698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, J. R., Soares, D. A., Rudzinski, S., and Johnson, R. (2019). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorders and Classroom-Based Interventions: Evidence-Based Status, Effectiveness, and Moderators of Effects in Single-Case Design Research. Rev. Educ. Res. 89, 569–611. doi: 10.3102/0034654319857038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartman, C. A., Rhee, S. H., Willcutt, E. G., and Pennington, B. F. (2007). Modeling Rater Disagreement for ADHD: Are Parents or Teachers Biased? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 536–542. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9110-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Altman, D. G., Gotzsche, P. C., Juni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The Cochrane Collaboration’s Tool for Assessing Risk of Bias in Randomised Trials. BMJ 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornstra, R., Groenman, A. P., Oord, S., Luman, M., Dekkers, T. J., Veen-Mulders, L., et al. (2023). Review: Which components of behavioral parent and teacher training work for children with ADHD – a metaregression analysis on child behavioral outcomes. Child Adolesc. Mental Health 28, 258–268. doi: 10.1111/camh.12561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y.-S., Yeh, C. B., Chen, C. H., Shang, C. Y., and Gau, S. S. F. (2021). A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Two-Way Crossover Clinical Trial of ORADUR-Methylphenidate for Treating Children and Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 31, 164–178. doi: 10.1089/cap.2020.0104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Kraemer, H. C., Lenora, N., Newcorn, J. H., Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2001). ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: comparing comorbid subgroups. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 147–158. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurbergs, N., Palcic, J. L., and Kelley, M. L. (2010). Daily behavior report cards with and without home-based consequences: improving classroom behavior in low income, African American Children with ADHD. Child Family Behav. Ther. 32, 177–195. doi: 10.1080/07317107.2010.500501

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khilnani, S., Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., and Schanberg, S. (2003). Massage therapy improves mood and behavior of students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Adolescence 38, 623–638

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lahey, B. B., Lee, S. S., Sibley, M. H., Applegate, B., Molina, B. S. G., and Pelham, W. E. (2016). Predictors of adolescent outcomes among 4–6-year-old children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 168–181. doi: 10.1037/abn0000086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, Y. T., Liu, X. P., and Fang, H. S. (2018). Randomized control study of the effects of executive function training on peer difficulties of children with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder C subtype. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 9, 41–55. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2018.1509003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Becker, S. P., Girio-Herrera, E., and Vaughn, A. J. (2012). Evaluation of the Homework, Organization, and Planning Skills (HOPS) intervention for middle school students with ADHD as implemented by school mental health providers. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 41, 342–364. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087514

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loe, I. M., and Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 32, 643–654. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsl054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Looyeh, M. Y., Kamali, K., and Shafieian, R. (2012). An exploratory study of the effectiveness of group narrative therapy on the school behavior of girls with attention- deficit/hyperactivity symptoms. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 26, 404–410. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, A. D. P., Lee, S., Sampson, N. A., Albor, Y., Alonso, J., Auerbach, R. P., et al. (2022). ADHD Comorbidity Structure and Impairment: Results of the WHO World Mental Health Surveys International College Student Project (WMH-ICS). J. Atten. Disord. 26:108705472110572. doi: 10.1177/10870547211057275

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikami, A. Y., Owens, J. S., Evans, S. W., Hudec, K. L., Kassab, H., Smit, S., et al. (2021). Promoting classroom social and academic functioning among children at risk for ADHD: The MOSAIC Program. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 51, 1039–1052. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2021.1929250

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Arnett, A. B., Shephard, E., Charman, T., Gustafsson, H. C., Joseph, H. M., et al. (2023). Delineating early developmental pathways to ADHD: Setting an international research agenda. JCPP Advances 3, 1–14. doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12144

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2011). Childhood executive function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood-diagnosed ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 657–668. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9599-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, A., Jarque, S., and Tárraga, R. (2006). Interventions in school settings for students with ADHD. Exceptionality. 14, 35–52. doi: 10.1207/s15327035ex1401_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, D. A., Russell, A. E., Matthews, J., Ford, T., Rogers, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., et al. (2018). School-based interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review with multiple synthesis methods. Rev. Educ. 6, 209–263. doi: 10.1002/rev3.3149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mrug, S., Molina, B. S. G., Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Hechtman, L., et al. (2012). Peer Rejection and Friendships in Children with Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Contributions to Long-Term Outcomes. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 1013–1026. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9610-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nafi, O., Shahin, A., Tarawneh, A., and Samhan, Z. (2020). Differences in identification of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children between teachers and parents. East Mediterr. Health J. 26, 834–838. doi: 10.26719/emhj.20.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute of Mental Health (2024). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Available online at: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/attention-deficit-hyperactivity-disorder-adhd (Accessed June 11, 2025).

Google Scholar

Nazarova, V. A., Sokolov, A. V., Chubarev, V. N., Tarasov, V. V., and Schiöth, H. B. (2022). Treatment of ADHD: Drugs, Psychological therapies, devices, Complementary and Alternative Methods as Well as the Trends in Clinical Trials. Frontiers in Pharmacology, [online] 13, 1–19. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2022.1066988

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nejati, V., Derakhshan, Z., and Mohtasham, A. (2023). The effect of comprehensive working memory training on executive functions and behavioral symptoms in children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Asian J. Psychiatr. 81:103469. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2023.103469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Omizo, M. M., and Michael, W. B. (1982). Biofeedback-induced relaxation training and impulsivity, attention to task, and locus of control among hyperactive boys. J. Learn. Disabil. 15, 414–416. doi: 10.1177/002221948201500708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, J. S., Allan, D. M., Kassab, H., and Mikami, A. Y. (2020). Evaluating a Short Form of the Academic Competence Evaluation Scales: Expanded Examination of Psychometric Properties. Sch. Ment. Heal. 12, 38–52. doi: 10.1007/s12310-019-09347-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfiffner, L. J., Rooney, M., Haack, L., Villodas, M., Delucchi, K., and McBurnett, K. (2016). A randomized controlled trial of a school-implemented school–home intervention for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and impairment. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 762–770. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Moore, D. A., Gwernan-Jones, R., Thompson-Coon, J., Ukoumunne, O. C., Rogers, M., et al. (2015). Non-pharmacological interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) delivered in school settings: systematic reviews of quantitative and qualitative research. Health Technol. Assess. 19, 1–470. doi: 10.3310/hta19450

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera, E., and Omizo, M. M. (1980). The effects of relaxation and biofeedback on attention to task and impulsivity among male hyperactive children. The Exceptional Child 27, 41–51. doi: 10.1080/0156655800270104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roselló, B., Berenguer, C., Baixauli, I., Mira, Á., Martinez-Raga, J., and Miranda, A. (2020). Empirical examination of executive functioning, ADHD associated behaviors, and functional impairments in adults with persistent ADHD, remittent ADHD, and without ADHD. BMC Psychiatry 20:02542-y. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadr-Salek, S., Costa, A. P., and Steffgen, G. (2023). Psychological Treatments for Hyperactivity and Impulsivity in Children with ADHD: A Narrative Review. Children, [online] 10:1613. doi: 10.3390/children10101613

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salari, N., Ghasemi, H., Abdoli, N., Rahmani, A., Shiri, M. H., Hashemian, A. H., et al. (2023). The global prevalence of ADHD in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ital. J. Pediatr. 49, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s13052-023-01456-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiber, J. E., Possin, K. L., Girard, J. M., and Rey-Casserly, C. (2014). Executive Function in Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: The NIH Examiner Battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 41–51. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, J. R., Small, J. W., Walker, H. M., Feil, E. G., Severson, H. H., Golly, A. M., et al. (2009). Efficacy of the first step to success intervention for students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sch. Ment. Heal. 1, 37–48. doi: 10.1007/s12310-008-9003-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ 349:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, M. H., Coxe, S. J., Campez, M., Morley, C., Olson, S., Hidalgo-Gato, N., et al. (2018). High versus low intensity summer treatment for ADHD delivered at secondary school transitions. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 248–265. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2018.1426005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A., Yeh, C. J., Verma, N., and Das, A. K. (2015). Overview of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young children. Health Psychol. Res. 3, 23–35. doi: 10.4081/hpr.2015.2115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. D., Vitulano, L. A., Katsovich, L., Li, S., Moore, C., Li, F., et al. (2017). A randomized controlled trial of an integrated brain, body, and social intervention for children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 780–794. doi: 10.1177/1087054716647490

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, N. J., Frenette, E. C., Rene, K. M., Brennan, R. T., and Perrin, E. C. (2014). Neurofeedback and cognitive attention training for children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in schools. J. Develop. Behav. Pediatrics 35, 18–27. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, N. J., Sheldrick, R. C., Gotthelf, D., and Perrin, E. C. (2011). Computer-based attention training in the schools for children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary trial. Clin. Pediatr. 50, 615–622. doi: 10.1177/0009922810397887

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Stralen, J. (2016). Emotional dysregulation in children with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Attention Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 8, 175–187. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0199-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watabe, Y., Stewart, J. L., Owens, J. S., Andrews, N., and Griffeth, J. C. (2013). Effectiveness and sustainability of school-based interventions for youth with or at risk for ADHD. Sch. Ment. Heal. 5, 83–95. doi: 10.1007/s12310-012-9094-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., and Barkley, R. A. (2010). Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J. Adolesc. Health 46, 209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Martelon, M., Westerberg, D., and Spencer, T. J. (2009). Presenting ADHD symptoms, subtypes, and comorbid disorders in clinically referred adults with ADHD. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70, 1557–1562. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04785pur

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 991–1010. doi: 10.1037/a0027347

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, school, intervention, systematic review, meta-analysis

Citation: Yegencik B, Bell BT and Deniz E (2025) School-based randomized controlled trials for ADHD and accompanying impairments: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 16:1611145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1611145

Received: 13 April 2025; Accepted: 19 June 2025;
Published: 21 July 2025.

Edited by:

Matteo Angelo Fabris, University of Turin, Italy

Reviewed by:

Lawrence Maayan, New York State Psychiatric Institute (NYSPI), United States
Ka Po Wong, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China
Emily McDougal, Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, United Kingdom
Fabrizio Turiaco, University of Messina, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Yegencik, Bell and Deniz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Beliz Yegencik, YmVsaXoueWVnZW5jaWtAZ21haWwuY29t; Emre Deniz, ZW1yZS5kZW5pekB5b3JrLmFjLnVr

ORCID: Beliz Yegencik, http://orcid.org/0009-0008-0645-2127
Beth Bell, http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6587-0336
Emre Deniz, http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7083-5562

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.