Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 25 September 2025

Sec. Human Developmental Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1659576

The contribution of executive functions and emotion comprehension skills to the development of pragmatic competence in 5–8-year-old children

  • Faculty of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia

A child’s pragmatic competence reflects both their social and communicative abilities, as well as their understanding of indirect meaning in words, utterances or discourse. This has led to a growing interest in the development of pragmatics in children. While the contribution of cognitive and emotional developmental aspects to pragmatic competence in general has been explored, the role of the emotion comprehension (EC) and executive functions (EF) in different pragmatic skills is still insufficiently studied. The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of children’s EF, and EC skills, on four core aspects of pragmatic competence (understanding, production, nonverbal means and communication). Participants were children (N = 1,842) aged 59–96 months (M = 73.51, SD = 9.0) and their teachers. Children completed tests assessing their EF (NEPSY-II) and understanding of emotions (Test of Emotion Comprehension), and their teachers completed a questionnaire of the children’s pragmatic competence. Through comparison of baseline and extended regression models, it was shown that although EF contribute significantly to all aspects of pragmatic competence, the connections with EC remain at a correlational level. Their contribution to pragmatic competence is not confirmed. These findings can support the development of programs to enhance children’s pragmatic competence, targeting educators, parents, and the children themselves.

Introduction

Pragmatics, or language in social context, is a construct that has been studied for more than half a century and is analyzed, among other things, from the perspective of ontogenetic development (Pearson and de Villiers, 2006). The development of pragmatic competence (the ability to use language appropriately in a social context (Taguchi, 2009)) in ontogeny is an essential component of a child’s overall social-communicative development. When examining the relationships between a child’s pragmatic skills and other aspects of their cognitive and emotional development, it becomes apparent that pragmatic skills are heterogeneous and their interrelations vary depending on the aspect considered. In this study, according to the instrument used (PCQCh), the child’s pragmatic skills include: (1) the ability to understand language in context; (2) communication skills; (3) the use of nonverbal means of communication; (4) narrative skills in different genres.

The relationship between children’s pragmatic competence and the executive functions

By executive functions (EF) we follow the concept of Friedman and Miyake (2017), understanding them as high-level cognitive processes that control lower-level processes. According to this model, EF include cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory. Research shows that EF regulate a wide range of mental functions in children and make a significant contribution to their development in preschool age. The relationship between pragmatic competence and the development of EF has primarily been studied in samples of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Filipe et al., 2020). These studies found that well-developed EF mitigate the negative impact of ASD on the pragmatic aspect of language in children from 8 to 18 years (Cardillo et al., 2021). In particular, a strong positive influence of well-developed working memory and attention on pragmatic skills in children aged 2–8 years with autism has been noted (Howard et al., 2023). Studies on this relationship in normatively developing populations remain relatively rare, and the available findings are often inconsistent (Blain-Brière et al., 2014). Nevertheless, several studies have emphasized the leading contribution of working memory to higher levels of pragmatic competence in adults and children aged 5–9 years (Bambini et al., 2021; Filippova and Astington, 2008). Also noted is the contribution of inhibitory control (Chen et al., 2025) to the comprehension of non-literal language and irony, as well as the role of cognitive flexibility (Hung and Loh, 2020) in meaning interpretation, which also pertains to pragmatic competence. Thus, despite numerous studies and the general conclusion about the contribution of EF to socio-communicative skills, their contribution to the development of pragmatics and its individual aspects in preschoolers remains insufficiently studied.

The relationship between children’s pragmatic competence and emotion comprehension

By emotion comprehension (EC) we follow the concept of Pons and Harris (2005). EC is the ability to understand “the nature, causes, and consequences of the emotional experience in the self and others” (Pons and Harris, 2019, p. 431). Children’s EC development passes through several stages or levels (Pons and Harris, 2005; Pons et al., 2004). These levels are: (1) the first “External” level at 3–5 years old. This level is associated with the formation of ideas about the external causes of emotions, that is, recognition of others’ emotions from facial expressions and understanding the influence of external circumstances on emotions. (2) The second is the “Mental” level between the ages of 4–5 and 6–7. Children begin to understand that personal beliefs, desires, and memories can trigger different emotions and that certain emotions can be hidden. (3) The third or” Reflective” level occurs at 6–7 to 9–10 years. Children learn to regulate their emotions, begin to better understand ambivalent (mixed) and hidden emotions, and discover the influence of moral norms on emotions (Pons and Harris, 2005; Pons et al., 2003). Pragmatic competence includes communicative skills, that is, the ability and willingness to engage in communication and interaction with others. It would be logical to assume that understanding the interlocutor’s emotions makes a significant contribution to the pragmatic competence. However, there are relatively few studies directly investigating the relationship between EC and pragmatic skills. In particular, Veraksa et al. (2019) provides clear evidence of a positive relationship between pragmatic narrative ability and EC in typical preschoolers. Curenton’s (2015) work shows how an emotion-focused task can reveal pragmatic competence. Pronina et al. (2021) suggest that these skills can be dissociated in intervention. In sum, pragmatic skill appears intertwined with EC during preschool (Spackman et al., 2006): children who are better at social language tend to understand emotions better, whereas those with pragmatic impairments (from ASD, ADHD, or DLD) often show social–emotional weaknesses as well (Carruthers et al., 2022; Wong et al., 2022). Several studies have shown that pragmatic skills contribute to EC, but not vice versa (Farina et al., 2007; Giménez-Dasí et al., 2013). It has also been shown that work with 3- to 4-year-old children focused on expanding their vocabulary with terms related to mental states and emotion labels significantly improved their emotion understanding, as well as their false-belief understanding (Ornaghi et al., 2011). As for precise studies of contribution of EC in children in their pragmatic skills, they are missing.

Research relevance and aim

Thus, the impact of EF and EC on pragmatic skills in children remains understudied. This research question is very important not only for designing intervention programs for children with ASD and communication disorders but also for enhancing the social-communicative and pragmatic skills of typically developing children. The aim of the present study is to assess the contribution of EF and EC to key aspects of pragmatic competence in children aged 5–8 years.

Methods and sample

Sample

The sample included typically developing children aged 5–8 years from middle-class families, encompassing both monolingual Russian speakers and Russian-dominant bilinguals. Children who did not complete both testing sessions, as well as those whose parents did not provide written informed consent, were excluded from the study. Initially the sample included more than 2000 children from six regions of the Russian Federation. However, after excluding irrelevant teacher responses to the PCQCh, valid assessments remained for 1,842 children, including 910 boys and 932 girls. The children were aged between 59 and 96 months (M = 73.51, SD = 9.0).

Instruments and procedure

To assess the pragmatic competence of children, the Pragmatic Competence Questionnaire for Children aged 5–8 years (PCQCh) (Oshchepkova et al., 2024) was used. This questionnaire was completed by the educators of the children attending their classes. It includes 20 items grouped into four scales: Scale 1. The ability to understand language in context (e.g., “The child understands from your intonation and facial expression that you are dissatisfied, demanding something, or, conversely, that you are pleased,” 6–30 points, Cronbach’s α = 0.811); Scale 2. Communication skills (e.g., “When communicating with relatives and familiar adults, the child shows interest and friendliness,” 5–25 points, Cronbach’s α = 0.795); Scale 3. Use of nonverbal means of communication (e.g., “The child uses gestures appropriate to the situation,” 3–15 points, Cronbach’s α = 0.845); Scale 4. Narrative skills in different genres (e.g., “The child invents his/her own coherent stories, with a beginning, a sequence of events, and an ending,” 3–15 points, Cronbach’s α = 0.856). Teachers evaluate how characteristic this behavior is for the child, from 1 (this behavior is not characteristic at all) to 5 (this behavior is always characteristic of the child).

To assess the level of EC, the TEC (Test of Emotion Comprehension) (Pons and Harris, 2004) was used. TEC is a tool that allows to capture child emotion understanding and measure nine components of EC: (1) emotion recognition, (2) external cause, (3) desire, (4) belief, (5) reminder, (6) regulation, (7) hidden, (8) mixed, and (9) morally based emotions. At the same time, it enables the distinction of three levels of emotion understanding: the external level, the mental level, and the reflective level. The test consists of 22 tasks. An accurate answer receives 1 point, and a wrong answer receives a zero point. The scores obtained for individual tasks are combined into four main indicators: three of them (External, Mental, and Reflective) correspond to the three levels of EC per the authors’ (Pons et al., 2004) theory, and the fourth indicator (Total score) is the sum of the points obtained from the first three indicators. The score can range from 0 to 3 for each of the three indicators. Accordingly, the total score for EC can range between 0 and 9. The total raw score is the sum of points for all 22 tasks, ranging from 0 to 22 points. The reliability of this instrument for Russian-speaking samples has been demonstrated previously (Bukhalenkova et al., 2024).

To assess EF, the NEPSY-II assessment battery was used (Korkman et al., 2007). Using this instrument, the following components were assessed in children: cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory (both visual and verbal). To assess inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, the Inhibition subtest (NEPSY-II) was used: in the task, the child first had to correctly name circles and squares, as well as upward and downward arrows, and then, following the rule, name them in reverse—circles as squares, squares as circles, and arrows in the opposite direction. Tasks performance correctness and time were assessed. Working memory was assessed through Sentence repetition (verbal working memory, maximum score—34) and Memory for designs (visual working memory, maximum score—120). The test had been translated into Russian, adapted for use with Russian-speaking children aged 5–8 years and showed high reliability (Veraksa et al., 2020).

Procedure

The assessment was conducted individually with each child in a quiet and bright room of the kindergarten attended by the children. Two sessions were organized with each child, lasting 15–20 min. Children were free to stop the test at any time. All methods were presented to children in the same established order: at the first session, the TEC, and Memory for Designs subtest; at the second session, the Inhibition and the Sentence repetition subtests were carried out. Prior to the study, the testers completed specialized training sessions on the administration and scoring procedures of the specified methods. Subsequently, they were required to pass a certification exam, which involved submitting a video recording of themselves conducting and processing the tests. Their scores were compared against those assigned by experienced experts. Only testers who achieved an inter-rater reliability score of at least Cronbach’s α = 0.81 were certified to conduct further testing.

All the parents were informed about the study goals and gave written consent for their children’s participation in the research. The study was carried out in those educational institutions with which cooperation agreements were concluded, and parental consent was collected with the help of teachers working in the groups attended by the children.

Statistical analysis

Preliminary analyses included simple Pearson and Spearman correlation analyses for main study variables. Additional Mann–Whitney test was then conducted to assess the significance of differences in pragmatic competence scales between boys and girls.

To identify the most significant predictors of pragmatic competence, we adopted a block-wise regression approach, ensuring that all necessary assumptions for linear regression were met. In our analysis, each of the four pragmatic competence scales was, in turn, used as the dependent variable. The predictors were organized into three distinct blocks: one for sex and age, one for EF components, and one for EC variables. In the first stage, we constructed a baseline Model 1 that included only the demographic predictors (sex and age). In the second stage, we developed two alternative extended models: in Model 2.1, the baseline model was supplemented with EF components, while in Model 2.2, predictors for EC were added. In the final stage, we built Model 3 by integrating both the EF and EC blocks along with the original demographic predictors.

After constructing all models for each of the four pragmatic competence scales, Model 2 (in two versions, Model 2.1 and Model 2.2) was compared with the baseline model using R2, and Model 3 was compared with Model 2. Based on these comparisons, the best-fitting model was selected. All analyses were conducted in Jamovi (version 2.3.16).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Supplementary Table 1 displays Pearson and Spearman correlations among study variables. The correlational analysis revealed significant (at the p < 0.05 level) associations between the pragmatic competence and age, sex (except for “Narrative skills”), Mental level, External level and Reflective levels of EC (except for “Use of nonverbal means of communication”), Cognitive flexibility, Inhibition (except for “Narrative skills”), Verbal working memory, and Visual working memory.

The Mann–Whitney test for identifying sex differences in the pragmatic competence scales revealed significant differences for three of the scales. Differences were significant in favor of girls for “Ability to understand language in context” (U = 379,769, p < 0.001, Rank biserial correlation = 0.1044), “Communication skills” (U = 379,024, p < 0.001, Rank biserial correlation = 0.1062), and “Use of nonverbal means” (U = 380,505, p < 0.001, Rank biserial correlation = 0.1027). For “Narrative skills,” no significant differences were found (U = 404,272, p = 0.081, Rank biserial correlation = 0.0467).

Regression models for pragmatic competence scales

Preliminary checks of the predictors did not reveal any multicollinearity issues (VIF statistics ranged from 1.01 to 1.60). We conducted separate block-wise linear regression models for each of the four scales of pragmatic competence. The models provide standardized estimates.

Regression models for scale “the ability to understand language in context”

At the first stage, the baseline Model 1, which included sex and age as predictors, was found to be significant (adjusted R2 = 0.05, F = 40.6, df₁ = 2, df₂ = 1,567, p < 0.001). Both sex (β = 0.20, t = 4.18, p < 0.001) and age (β = 0.20, t = 8.19, p < 0.001) made a significant contribution.

At the second stage, the baseline model was first supplemented first with a block of EF components (Model 2.1). This resulted in a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.118, F = 36.1, df₁ = 6, df₂ = 1,563, p < 0.001), with significant predictors being visual working memory (β = 0.13, t = 4.38, p < 0.001), verbal working memory (β = 0.20, t = 7.52, p < 0.001), and sex.

Next, Model 2.2 was constructed, which consisted of sex, age, and components of EC (adjusted R2 = 0.06, F = 21.4, df₁ = 5, df₂ = 1,564, p < 0.001). The significant predictors of this version of the model turned out to be sex, age, external level of EC (β = 0.08, t = 3.13, p = 0.002), and reflective level of EC (β = 0.07, t = 2.76, p = 0.006).

Finally, at the third stage, the model was expanded to include all three blocks: sex, age, EF components, and EC (Model 3). The resulting model was significant (adjusted R2 = 0.119, F = 24.5, df₁ = 9, df₂ = 1,560, p < 0.001). Significant predictors are visual working memory (β = 0.12, t = 3.91, p < 0.001), verbal working memory (β = 0.2, t = 7.26, p < 0.001), and sex.

A comparison between the baseline model and Model 2.1 showed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.07, F = 32.24, p < 0.001). A comparison between the baseline model and Model 2.2 showed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.01, F = 8.22, p < 0.001). A comparison between Model 2.1 and Model 3 showed no significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.002, F = 1.21, p = 0.304). A comparison between Model 2.2 and Model 3 showed significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.06, F = 26.59, p < 0.001).

Therefore, Model 3 was selected as the best-fitting model (see Table 1). Thus, the most significant predictors of the ability to understand language in context are verbal and visual working memory. However, without taking into account the EF components, the level of EC (external and reflective) also contribute to this aspect of pragmatic competence.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Regression model of scale 1: the ability to understand language in context.

Regression models for scale “communication skills”

At the first stage, the baseline model, which included sex and age as predictors, was found to be significant (adjusted R2 = 0.03, F = 26.6, df₁ = 2, df₂ = 1,567, p < 0.001). Both sex (β = 0.186, t = 3.74, p < 0.001) and age (β = 0.161, t = 6.45, p < 0.001) made significant contributions.

At the second stage, the baseline model was first supplemented first with a block of EF components (Model 2.1), resulting in a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.079, F = 23.5, df₁ = 6, df₂ = 1,563, p < 0.001). The significant predictors were visual working memory (β = 0.114, t = 3.81, p < 0.001), verbal working memory (β = 0.187, t = 6.76, p < 0.001), and sex.

Next, Model 2.2 was constructed, which consisted of sex, age, and components of EC (adjusted R2 = 0.04, F = 14, df₁ = 5, df₂ = 1,564, p < 0.001). The significant predictors of this version of the model turned out to be sex, age, and mental level of EC (β = 0.06, t = 2.62, p = 0.009).

Finally, at the third stage, the model was expanded to include all three blocks: sex, age, EF components, and EC (Model 3). The resulting model was significant (adjusted R2 = 0.080, F = 16.2, df₁ = 9, df₂ = 1,560, p < 0.001). Significant predictors in Model 3 included visual working memory (β = 0.102, t = 3.34, p < 0.001), verbal working memory (β = 0.185, t = 6.39, p < 0.001), the mental level of EC (β = 0.05, t = 1.98, p = 0.048), and sex.

A comparison of the baseline model and Model 2.1 revealed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.049, F = 21.28, p < 0.001). A comparison of the baseline model and Model 2.2 revealed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.01, F = 5.5, p < 0.001). The comparison between Model 2.1 and Model 3 showed no significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.003, F = 1.54, p = 0.203). The comparison between Model 2.2 and Model 3 showed significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.04, F = 18.1, p < 0.001).

Based on the largest R2, the best model turned out to be the Model 3 (Table 2). Thus, the most significant predictors of communication skills are visual and verbal working memory, and the mental level of EC.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression model of scale 2: communication skills.

Regression models for scale “use of nonverbal means of communication”

At the first stage, the baseline model, which included sex and age as predictors, was significant (adjusted R2 = 0.0204, F = 17.32, df₁ = 2, df₂ = 1,567, p < 0.001). Both sex (β = 0.198, t = 3.95, p < 0.001) and age (β = 0.114, t = 4.57, p < 0.001) made significant contributions.

At the second stage, the baseline model was first supplemented first with a block of EF components (Model 2.1), resulting in a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.047, F = 13.93, df₁ = 6, df₂ = 1,563, p < 0.001). Significant predictors included visual working memory (β = 0.09, t = 3.02, p = 0.003), verbal working memory (β = 0.132, t = 4.7, p < 0.001), and sex.

Next, Model 2.2 was constructed, which consisted of sex, age, and scores for EC (adjusted R2 = 0.0203, F = 7.5, df₁ = 5, df₂ = 1,564, p < 0.001). The significant predictors of this version of the model turned out to be sex and age.

Finally, at the third stage, the model was expanded to include all three blocks: sex, age, EF components, and level of EC (Model 3), yielding a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.045, F = 9.32, df₁ = 9, df₂ = 1,560, p < 0.001). Significant predictors are visual working memory (β = 0.09, t = 2.97, p = 0.003), verbal working memory (β = 0.131, t = 4.72, p < 0.001), and sex.

A comparison of the baseline model and Model 2.1 showed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.029, F = 11.98, p < 0.001). A comparison of the baseline model and Model 2.2 did not reveal a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.001, F = 0.95, p = 0.416). Comparing Model 2.1 and Model 3 did not reveal any significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.000, F = 0.149, p = 0.930). Comparing Model 2.2 and Model 3 revealed significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.03, F = 11.4, p < 0.001).

Based on the largest R2, Model 2.1 was selected as the best-fitting model (see Table 3). Thus, the most significant predictors of nonverbal means of communication are verbal and visual working memory.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression model of scale 3: use of nonverbal means of communication.

Regression models for scale “narrative skills in different genres”

At the first stage, the baseline model, which included sex and age as predictors, was significant (adjusted R2 = 0.032, F = 27.6, df₁ = 2, df₂ = 1,567, p < 0.001). Only age was a significant predictor (β = 0.18, t = 7.29, p < 0.001), while sex was not (β = 0.09, t = 1.82, p = 0.069).

At the second stage, the baseline model was first supplemented with a block of EF components (Model 2.1), resulting in a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.064, F = 18.8, df₁ = 6, df₂ = 1,563, p < 0.001). Significant predictors included verbal working memory (β = 0.176, t = 6.29, p < 0.001) and age.

Next, Model 2.2 was constructed, which consisted of sex, age, and scores of EC (adjusted R2 = 0.034, F = 12.1, df₁ = 5, df₂ = 1,564, p < 0.001). The significant predictors of this version of the model turned out to be age.

Finally, at the third stage, the model was expanded to include all three blocks: sex, age, EF components, and EC scores (Model 3), yielding a significant model (adjusted R2 = 0.063, F = 12.7, df₁ = 9, df₂ = 1,560, p < 0.001). The significant predictors are verbal working memory (β = 0.176, t = 6.24, p < 0.001) and age.

A comparison between the baseline model and Model 2.1 showed a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.03, F = 13.9, p < 0.001). A comparison of the baseline model and Model 2.2 did not reveal a significant improvement in model quality (ΔR2 = 0.003, F = 1.81, p = 0.144). A comparison between Model 2.1 and Model 3 revealed no further improvement (ΔR2 = 0.000, F = 0.341, p = 0.796). Comparing Model 2.2 and Model 3 revealed significant improvement (ΔR2 = 0.03, F = 12.8, p < 0.001).

Based on the largest R2, Model 2.1 was selected as the best-fitting model (see Table 4). Thus, the most significant predictor of narrative skills in different genres is verbal working memory.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regression model of scale 4: narrative skills in different genres.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to assess the contribution of EF and EC to key aspects of pragmatic competence in children aged 5–8 years. Our data showed that the result depended on EF aspect. Working memory in children from 5 to 8 years old was proved to be the strongest predictor for all the aspects of pragmatic competence. It corresponds with data, reported by V. Bambini and her colleagues in aged people (Bambini et al., 2021). Cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control had significant correlations with pragmatic competence in children, however they did not enhance it. As shown in previously cited studies (Bambini et al., 2021), verbal working memory makes the greatest contribution to pragmatic skills. The impact of EF on such pragmatic ability as narrative construction was also proved in longitudinal study (Oshchepkova and Shatskaya, 2023). The current study demonstrated the same effect on other aspects of pragmatic skills. The comprehension of figurative meaning requires children to retain both instructions and what was said in memory. This is especially important for interpreting the figurative meanings of words and phrases. For the communicative aspect of pragmatics, working memory also proved to be the most significant predictor. In our view, this can be explained by the fact that communication requires holding in memory what exactly the interlocutor has said and constructing one’s response according to his/her remarks. Why both verbal and visual working memory turned out to be the most significant predictors for the use of nonverbal means of communication is less clear to us. It could be explained by the fact, that the production of gestures implies the ability to coordinate their execution with the meaning of the discourse: children with higher WM could be better able to coordinate the two levels. Furthermore, maintaining the motor plan in mind requires WM. We may assume that this is a matter of working memory in general, which allows one to retain the elements of a communicative situation (both verbal and nonverbal) and respond appropriately to this situation. For Scale 4, verbal working memory made the most significant contribution. This confirms previous findings that working memory influences narrative production in preschool children (Veraksa et al., 2019). We can explain this primarily by the fact that verbal working memory makes a substantial contribution both to children’s language development in general and to the development of narrative skills (Gago-Galvagno et al., 2024; Ryabikina and Vasilchenko, 2023). Therefore, verbal working memory remains the strongest predictor of all aspects of pragmatic competence.

As for the impact of EC on pragmatic competence in children with normative development, our data showed that though EC skills correlated significantly with all the aspects of pragmatic competence in children, the impact of EC skills on pragmatics was statistically less significant then the impact of EF. The contribution of EC to pragmatic competence remains debatable, as noted in the literature we reviewed (Beck et al., 2012; Derakhshan et al., 2021). Since significant correlations (p < 0.001) were found between all components of EC and pragmatic competence, we may assume that this relationship reflects a specific directionality. It is pragmatic competence—defined as the child’s ability and skill to communicate and engage in discussions with close others—that may shape their ability to recognize and understand emotions (Guseva et al., 2025; Morozova et al., 2025; Pronina et al., 2021; Veraksa et al., 2023). Thus, a correlational study design can only establish a significant relationship between EC and pragmatic competence, whereas the direction of this relationship requires separate investigation and, in this case, cannot be determined.

It was noteworthy that sex differences were revealed for all the aspects of pragmatic skills. It corresponds with the data received for general verbal abilities (Rzhanova et al., 2023) and may respond to educators’ beliefs about communicative strategies of boys vs. girls or reflect social norms and stereotypes about sex differences in communication (Rudnova et al., 2024).

Conclusion

The children’s pragmatic competence is significantly influenced by EF, particularly verbal and visual working memory. This reflects the importance of these executive aspects for remembering instructions, rules of games, and what a conversation partner said, especially when figurative meanings are involved. To understand indirect meanings accurately, it is essential not only to retain the literal utterance but also to keep the context in memory. The same mechanisms, namely, the integration of verbal content and social context, are also supported, albeit to a lesser degree, by cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control.

One notable limitation is our inability to isolate and assess the specific role of Theory of Mind in shaping pragmatic competence. This remains an important direction for future studies. Furthermore, we could cite the correlational nature of the design, which prevents the assessment of the direction of relations.

Data availability statement

Data supporting the findings of this study are available from the authors upon reasonable request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Ekaterina Oshchepkova, bWFwb3N0ZTA2QHlhbmRleC5ydQ==.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the study and consent procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of Faculty of Psychology at Lomonosov Moscow State University (the approval No: 2023/18). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

EO: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft. AS: Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. KT: Data curation, Investigation, Software, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The research was supported by the Russian Science Foundation (number 23-78-30005).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1659576/full#supplementary-material

References

Bambini, V., Van Looy, L., Demiddele, K., and Schaeken, W. (2021). What is the contribution of executive functions to communicative-pragmatic skills? Insights from aging and different types of pragmatic inference. Cogn. Process. 22, 435–452. doi: 10.1007/s10339-021-01021-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, L., Kumschick, I. R., Eid, M., and Klann-Delius, G. (2012). Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion 12, 503–514. doi: 10.1037/a0026320

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blain-Brière, B., Bouchard, C., and Bigras, N. (2014). The role of executive functions in the pragmatic skills of children age 4–5. Front. Psychol. 5:240. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukhalenkova, D., Veraksa, A., Guseva, U., and Oshchepkova, E. (2024). The relationship between vocabulary size and emotion understanding in children aged 5–7 years. Lomonosov Psychol. J. 47, 150–176. doi: 10.11621/LPJ-24-44

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardillo, R., Mammarella, I. C., Demurie, E., Giofrè, D., and Roeyers, H. (2021). Pragmatic language in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: do theory of mind and executive functions have a mediating role? Autism Res. 14, 932–945. doi: 10.1002/aur.2423

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carruthers, S., Taylor, L., Sadiq, H., and Tripp, G. (2022). The profile of pragmatic language impairments in children with ADHD: a systematic review. Dev. Psychopathol. 34, 1938–1960. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421000328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Lv, J., and Chen, B. (2025). Crossing the cultural bridge: the role of inhibitory control during second language metaphor comprehension. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1–17. doi: 10.1017/S1366728924001081

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curenton, S. M. (2015). African American preschoolers' emotion explanations can provide evidence of their pragmatic skills. Top. Lang. Disord. 35, 46–60. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Derakhshan, A., Eslami, Z., and Ghandhari, N. (2021). Investigating the interplay of emotional intelligence and interlanguage pragmatic competence in Iranian lower-intermediate EFL learners. Issues Lang. Teach. 10, 37–66. doi: 10.22054/ilt.2020.54334.527

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, E., Albanese, O., and Pons, F. (2007). Making inferences and individual differences in emotion understanding. Psychol. Lang. Commun. 11, 3–19. Available at: https://hdl.handle.net/10281/142042

Google Scholar

Filipe, M. G., Veloso, A., Frota, S., and Vicente, S. G. (2020). Executive functions and pragmatics in children with high-functioning autism. Read. Writ. 33, 859–875. doi: 10.1007/s11145-019-09975-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippova, E., and Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Dev. 79, 126–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01115.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 86, 186–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gago-Galvagno, L. G., Castillo, M. P., Fernandez, M. A., Tabullo, A. J., Miller, S. E., Elgier, A. M., et al. (2024). Dyadic interactions, communication and regulation skills: associations with screen use in toddlers from Buenos Aires. Psychol. Russ. 17, 39–59. doi: 10.11621/pir.2024.0403

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giménez-Dasí, M., Quintanilla, L., and Daniel, M. F. (2013). Improving emotion comprehension and social skills in early childhood through philosophy for children. Child. Philos. 9, 63–89. Available at: https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/5120/512051608005.pdf

Google Scholar

Guseva, U., Bukhalenkova, D., and Morozova, O. (2025). Relationship between value orientations and emotion understanding in Preschoolers. New Ideas Child Educ. Psychol. 1–2, 67–83. doi: 10.11621/nicep.2025.0504

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, J., Herold, B., Major, S., Leahy, C., Ramseur, K. II, Franz, L., et al. (2023). Associations between executive function and attention abilities and language and social communication skills in young autistic children. Autism 27, 2135–2144. doi: 10.1177/13623613231154310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, C. O. Y., and Loh, E. K. Y. (2020). Examining the contribution of cognitive flexibility to metalinguistic skills and reading comprehension. Educ. Psychol. 41, 712–729. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2020.1734187

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korkman, M., Kirk, U., and Kemp, S. L. (2007). NEPSY II. Administrative manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Morozova, M., Nikonova, E., Rupchev, G., and Burminskiy, D. S. (2025). Emotional attribution in a social context in stable patients with schizophrenia with residual psychotic symptoms. Natl. Psychol. J. 20, 92–105. doi: 10.11621/npj.2025.0207

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ornaghi, V., Brockmeier, J., and Gavazzi, I. G. (2011). The role of language games in children's understanding of mental states: a training study. J. Cogn. Dev. 12, 239–259. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.563487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oshchepkova, E., Kartushina, N., and Tarasova, K. (2024). A questionnaire for the assessment of pragmatic competence in children aged 5-8. Clin. Psychol. Spec. Educ. 13, 128–154. doi: 10.17759/cpse.2024130109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oshchepkova, E., and Shatskaya, A. (2023). Development of narratives in children aged 6–8 years depending on the level of executive functions. Lomonosov Psychol. J. 46, 261–284. doi: 10.11621/LPJ-23-25

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, B. Z., and de Villiers, J. (2006). “Pragmatics and language acquisition” in Encyclopedia of language & linguistics. ed. K. Brown (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 10–17.

Google Scholar

Pons, F., and Harris, P. L. (2004). TEC: Test of emotion comprehension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Pons, F., and Harris, P. L. (2005). Longitudinal change and longitudinal stability of individual differences in children’s emotion understanding. Cognit. Emot. 19, 1158–1174. doi: 10.1080/02699930500282108

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pons, F., and Harris, P. L. (2019). “Children’s understanding of emotions or Pascal’s “error”: Review and prospects” in Handbook of emotional development. eds. V. LoBue, K. Perez-Edgar, and K. A. Buss (Berlin: Springer), 431–449.

Google Scholar

Pons, F., Harris, P. L., and de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organization. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 1, 127–152. doi: 10.1080/17405620344000022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pons, F., Lawson, J., Harris, P. L., and de Rosnay, M. (2003). Individual differences in children’s emotion understanding: effects of age and language. Scand. J. Psychol. 44, 347–353. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00354

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pronina, M., Hübscher, I., Vilà-Giménez, I., and Prieto, P. (2021). Bridging the gap between prosody and pragmatics: the acquisition of pragmatic prosody in the preschool years and its relation with theory of mind. Front. Psychol. 12:662124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.662124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudnova, N., Kornienko, S., Gavrilova, M., Glotova, G., and Shvedchikova, Y. (2024). Parental beliefs as a factor in the cognitive and socio-emotional development of the child. Lomonosov Psychol. J. 47, 134–152. doi: 10.11621/LPJ-24-18

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryabikina, Z. I., and Vasilchenko, N. A. (2023). Friendship as a factor of transformation of self-regulatory processes of personality. Theor. Exp. Psychol. 4, 28–45. doi: 10.11621/TEP-23-27

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rzhanova, I. E., Alekseeva, O. S., Boldyreva, A. Y., Nikolaeva, A. Y., and Burdukova, Y. A. (2023). Verbal abilities: sex differences in children at different ages. Psychol. Russ. 16, 22–32. doi: 10.11621/pir.2023.0202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spackman, M. P., Fujiki, M., and Brinton, B. (2006). Understanding emotions in context: the effects of language impairment on children's ability to infer emotional reactions. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 41, 173–188. doi: 10.1080/13682820500224091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taguchi, N. (2009). Pragmatic competence. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.

Google Scholar

Veraksa, A. N., Almazova, O. V., and Bukhalenkova, D. A. (2020). Executive functions assessment in senior preschool age: a battery of methods. Psychol. J. 41, 108–118. doi: 10.31857/S020595920012593-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veraksa, A. N., Bukhalenkova, D. A., and Oshchepkova, E. S. (2019). The relationship of emotions comprehension and pragmatic aspects of the narrative in preschool age. Bull. Russ. Found. Hum. Res. 4, 76–84. doi: 10.22204/2410-4639-2019-104-04-76-84

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Veraksa, N. E., Airapetyan, Z. V., and Tarasova, K. S. (2023). Dialectical and formal thinking in the context of understanding emotions by young children. Theor. Exp. Psychol. 3, 72–91. doi: 10.11621/TEP-23-20

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, K. H. Y., Lee, K. Y. S., Tsze, S. C. Y., and Law, T. (2022). Comparing early pragmatics in typically developing children and children with neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 52, 3825–3839. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05261-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pragmatics, executive functions, test of emotion comprehension, pragmatic development, preschool age, typically developing children, regression models

Citation: Oshchepkova E, Shatskaya A and Tarasova K (2025) The contribution of executive functions and emotion comprehension skills to the development of pragmatic competence in 5–8-year-old children. Front. Psychol. 16:1659576. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1659576

Received: 07 July 2025; Accepted: 09 September 2025;
Published: 25 September 2025.

Edited by:

Ilaria Grazzani, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Pietro Spataro, Mercatorum University, Italy
Elena Cravet, University of Genoa, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Oshchepkova, Shatskaya and Tarasova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ekaterina Oshchepkova, bWFwb3N0ZTA2QHlhbmRleC5ydQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.