Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 01 October 2019
Sec. Biomechanics and Control of Human Movement

Walking Cadence Affects the Recruitment of the Medial-Lateral Balance Mechanisms

  • 1Department of Kinesiology and Applied Physiology, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States
  • 2Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, United States

We have previously identified three balance mechanisms that young healthy adults use to maintain balance while walking. The three mechanisms are: (1) The lateral ankle mechanism, an active modulation of ankle inversion/eversion in stance; (2) The foot placement mechanism, an active shift of the swing foot placement; and (3) The push-off mechanism, an active modulation of the ankle plantarflexion angle during double stance. Here we seek to determine whether there are changes in neural control of balance when walking at different cadences and speeds. Twenty-one healthy young adults walked on a self-paced treadmill while immersed in a 3D virtual reality cave, and periodically received balance perturbations (bipolar galvanic vestibular stimulation) eliciting a perceived fall to the side. Subjects were instructed to match two cadences specified by a metronome, 110 bpm (High) and 80 bpm (Low), which in this experiment, led to faster and slower gait speeds, respectively. The results indicate that subjects altered the use of the balance mechanisms at different cadences. The lateral ankle mechanism was used more in the Low condition, while the foot placement mechanism was used more in the High condition. There was no difference in the use of the push-off mechanism between cadence conditions. These results suggest that neural control of balance is altered when gait characteristics, such as cadence change, suggesting a flexible balance response that is sensitive to the constraints of the gait cycle. We speculate that the use of the balance mechanisms may be a factor resulting in well-known characteristics of gait in populations with compromised balance control, such as slower gait speed in older adults or higher cadence in people with Parkinson's disease.

1. Introduction

There is an ongoing debate about whether or not walking slower is more stable (Bruijn et al., 2009). We know certain patient populations reduce their gait speed and increase their cadence (Himann et al., 1988; Lauretani et al., 2003; Buckley et al., 2018; Duan-Porter et al., 2019), but is the motivation to improve stability? Key factors that lead to the decrease in gait speed in older adults remains unresolved, but we speculate that the control of balance plays a larger role than previously recognized. We do not intend to justify whether walking slower or faster leads to more or less stability, here we are interested in how the neural control of balance in the medial-lateral direction changes at different cadences.

Humans can adopt different combinations of step length and cadence to walk at a certain speed (Ardestani et al., 2016), creating a complex relationship when fixing a specific gait parameter. Cadence is commonly used as a gait intervention method in movement disorders, such as Parkinson's Disease, Cerebral Palsy, and stroke (Thaut and Abiru, 2010). Typically increasing cadence leads to an increase in step length and velocity, which is often the target outcome in these interventions (Hayden et al., 2009; Wittwer et al., 2013; Nascimento et al., 2015). The interplay between the adjustment of these anterior-posterior gait parameters and the control of balance in the medial-lateral direction is not well-understood.

There are a number of reports that support slower walking being a beneficial adaptation. Roos and Dingwell (2013) found that neuromuscular noise is diminished when walking slower. Less noise generally results in better estimates of a physical quantity, and thus improved control. Lyapunov exponents, a common tool used for quantifying overall stability, have been shown to be reduced at lower walking speeds, which have been interpreted as decreased susceptibility to local perturbations (Dingwell and Marin, 2006; England and Granata, 2007). Others have pointed to neurological deficiencies associated with slower walking, without referring to stability directly. Anson et al. (2019) have shown vestibular function loss causes people to walk with longer, slower steps, contradicting findings by McCrum et al. (2018) who found people with vestibular loss adapt by taking shorter, faster steps. Menz et al. (2004) have shown people with peripheral neuropathy reduced walking speed and cadence. Hsiao et al. (2017) suggested that a lateral weight shift mechanism is impaired in chronic stroke, leading to reduced gait speed.

Another body of literature suggests fast walking is beneficial. Variability, which is often attributed to greater instability, lessens with increased velocity (Donker and Beek, 2002), even in people with bilateral vestibular loss (Schniepp et al., 2012, 2017; Wuehr et al., 2016; McCrum et al., 2018). Studies have also used the mediolateral margin of stability to quantify overall stability, which increases with speed (Hof et al., 2007; Gates et al., 2013). A larger margin of stability is viewed as safer and more stable. Recent modeling results indicate that slowed gait can be explained entirely by diminished muscle strength (Song and Geyer, 2018). However, Fan et al. (2016) has shown older adults with slower gait can walk faster if instructed, suggesting that reduced locomotor capacity (i.e., force-generation) cannot account for slowed gait. This suggests a complex relationship between stability and slowed gait. Muscle strength clearly plays a role in gait characteristics but its role in the control of balance during locomotion is not well-understood. Factors, such as multisensory integration contribute to alterations in the control of balance (Oie et al., 2002; Peterka, 2002). Moreover, emotions, such as fear of falling also alter the gait parameters (Maki, 1997; Makino et al., 2017), providing further evidence that muscle strength is not the only factor leading to slowed gait.

Regardless of the walking speed, a common theme to the process of keeping a body upright is the relationship between the center of mass (CoM) and the center of pressure (CoP). The behavior of the CoM behavior, can be explained by the relationship between CoP and CoM, as in Equation (1) (Winter, 1995).

=ω2(x-p),    (1)

where p is the CoP, x the CoM position and ẍ the CoM acceleration. The CoM acceleration is proportional to the distance between the CoP and CoM. Where ω = g/l, l is the distance from the CoM to the ankle joint center and g the acceleration from gravity. This relationship holds true for both the anterior-posterior direction and the medial-lateral direction.

Applying Equation (1), a threat to balance would be an undesired movement of the CoM relative to the CoP. To correct this undesired movement, a motor action has to be taken to accelerate the CoM in the opposite direction or, rather, to decelerate the CoM, thus stopping the movement. To prevent a fall, the CoM should not be able to accelerate to the point of no return in any particular direction. So the CoP must be moved to accelerate the CoM in the opposite direction of its current travel (Hof, 2007).

In a previous study with young healthy adults, we have identified three different mechanisms of balance control in response to perceived falls (Reimann et al., 2018b). The perceived fall theoretically induces a perceived shift of the center of mass (CoM), requiring a motor action that shifts the CoM in the opposite direction. The following mechanisms are distinct balance mechanisms, observed in previous experiments, that can shift the CoP and CoM in the medial-lateral direction.

The foot placement is the most commonly reported and quantified method of balance control (Kuo, 1999; Bauby and Kuo, 2000; Vlutters et al., 2016). The foot placement mechanism consists of the swing foot moving in the direction of the perceived fall, and on heel strike, shifts the CoP in the direction of the perceived fall. In general, the foot placement mechanism refers to the modulation of step width in a particular direction (i.e., the direction of the perceived fall). Wang and Srinivasan (2014) show that 80% of the foot placement can be explained by the position and velocity of the CoM. Theoretically, the sensory input we provide to subjects alters the perceived state of the CoM, thus there should be a difference between the predicted foot placement based on the CoM behavior, and the actual foot placement. We refer to this measure as the model-corrected foot placement. The lateral ankle is another mechanism that can shift the CoP (Hof et al., 2010). The lateral ankle mechanism refers to the generation of ankle inversion/eversion torque during the single stance phase. Activating musculature to roll the ankle while the foot is on the ground shifts the CoP under the foot in the direction of the perceived fall. The goal is to accelerate the CoM away from the direction of the perceived fall, and by shifting the CoP in the direction of the perceived fall, the CoM will accelerate away from the perceived fall. We refer to the third balance mechanism as the push-off mechanism. The push-off mechanism refers to the modulation of ankle plantarflexion angle during double stance. In response to a visually perceived fall to the side, we observed a direction-dependent modulation of the stance leg ankle plantar/dorsiflexion angle. Very few studies have recognized the push-off mechanism's role in control of balance in the medial-lateral direction. Ankle plantarflexion torque has been shown to change as a result of bipolar, binaural galvanic vestibular stimulation (Iles et al., 2007). Only recently has the push-off been verified to have a functional role in balance control in the medial-lateral direction as Kim and Collins (2015) show modulation of ankle torque based on CoM behavior can reduce metabolic expenditure. Furthermore, Klemetti et al. (2014) have provided evidence that ankle plantarflexion torque induces trunk roll accelerations, providing further support for the push-off acting in the medial-lateral direction.

The need for multiple mechanisms to promote balance derives from the gait cycle, which demands different mechanisms due to the changing configuration of the body (i.e., alternation of double stance and single stance). Importantly, these mechanisms have a temporal order. The earliest response to a balance disturbance (i.e., at heel strike) is the lateral ankle mechanism, followed by a change in foot placement and push-off (Reimann et al., 2018b). Given that temporal order, we were interested in determining if gait characteristics can change how these balance mechanisms are used. Specifically, are there changes in the use of the balance mechanisms at different cadences? Modeling results suggest more frequent steps leads to more opportunities to correct undesired CoM movements with the foot placement mechanism (Reimann et al., 2018a). We hypothesized that due to the longer single stance in slower cadence, the lateral ankle mechanism would play a larger role in balance, and in faster cadence, the foot placement mechanism would provide the majority of balance control.

2. Methods

2.1. Subject Characteristics

Twenty-one healthy young subjects (15 female, 23.65 ± 4.43 years, 1.68 ± 0.11 m, 64.23 ± 14.97 kg) volunteered for the study. Subjects provided informed verbal and written consent to participate. Subjects did not have a history of neurological disorders or surgical procedures involving legs, spine or head. The experiment was approved by the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board #1125078-7. We used a statistical power analysis based on pilot data from another experiment (Reimann et al., 2018b) to determine the number of subjects required to reliably detect functionally relevant differences in the use of ankle roll and foot placement, set to 1 mm s integrated CoP-CoM displacement and 5 mm foot placement change (1−β = 0.9).

2.2. Experimental Design

Subjects walked on a split-belt, instrumented treadmill within a virtual environment projected onto a curved screen surrounding the treadmill as shown in Figure 1 (Bertec Inc., Columbus, Ohio, USA), to recreate the motion parallax that is experienced by walking over ground. We use a self-paced treadmill to allow for fluctuations of the gait parameters (gait speed, step length, cadence) as would occur over-ground. The immersive virtual reality creates a motion parallax that mimics how the real world would move on your retina as if walking over-ground. Without the virtual reality walking on a treadmill would produce no optic flow (static environment), which is critical for posture control while walking (Warren et al., 2001).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup displaying the position of the split-belt treadmill in front of the curved screen.

The treadmill was self-paced, using a non-linear PD-controller in Labview (National instruments Inc., Austin, TX, USA) to keep the middle of the posterior superior iliac spine (MPSIS) markers on the mid-line of the treadmill. The same speed command was sent to each belt of the treadmill. The virtual environment consisted of a tiled marble floor with floating cubes randomly distributed in a volume 0–10 m above the floor, 2–17 m to each side from the midline, and infinitely into the distance, forming a 4 m wide corridor for the subjects to walk through (see Figure 1), implemented in Unity3d (Unity Technologies, San Francisco, CA, USA). Perspective in the virtual world was linked to the midpoint between the two markers on the subject's temples, superposed over forward motion defined by the treadmill speed.

The subjects performed 20 2-min trials. A metronome generated by custom Labview software played throughout the trials at a frequency of 80 bpm (Low) or 110 bpm (High), which alternated on every 2 min trial. Subjects were asked to match their cadence to the metronome. After a 2-min trial there was a 15 s washout period of no metronome, followed by a 30 s adaptation period for the new metronome frequency before introducing the balance perturbations. Breaks were offered after every five 2-min trials.

During the 2-min trials, a target step number was randomized between 10 and 13. The custom Labview software counted heel strikes until the step counter matched the target step number. The leg of the heel strike that occurs on the target step number is referred to as the trigger leg. The Labview software randomized between two conditions on a trigger, either perturbation or control. Subjects were unaware if the control condition was triggered. The perturbations consisted of a 1 mA current produced by a NeuroConn DC-Stimulator Plus direct current stimulator (Ilmenau, Germany), passed between Axelgaard PALS 3.2 cm round electrodes (California, USA) placed on the mastoid processes for 1,000 ms, inducing a feeling of falling to the side, as observed in other experiments (Fitzpatrick et al., 1999; Bent et al., 2000). The polarity was chosen so that the perturbation always produced the perception of falling toward the trigger leg (i.e., Left heel strikes triggers perturbation, stimulus induced perception of falling to the left).

We identified heel strikes on-line with a manually set vertical threshold of the heel position. The vertical threshold was set to the heel marker position during quiet standing. However, in the Low condition people tended to “drag” their feet, which would cause steps to be counted both midway through swing and actual heel strike. To avoid double counting of steps, the experimenter increased the vertical threshold until double counting of steps did not occur.

2.3. Data Analysis

We recorded full-body kinematics using the Plug-in Gait marker set (Davis Iii et al., 1991), with six additional markers on the anterior thigh, anterior tibia, and 5th metatarsal of each foot. Another additional six markers were placed on the medial femoral epicondyles, medial malleolus, and the tip of the first distal phalanx of the foot for the static calibration pose at the beginning of the collection. We recorded electromyography signals from the tibialis anterior, peroneus longus, medial gastrocnemius, rectus femoris, biceps femoris, gluteus medius, tensor fascia latae, and erector spinae, bilaterally with Cometa's electromyography PicoEMG sensors (Bareggio, Italy) with hydrogel 30 × 24 mm Covidien Kendall electrodes using SENIAM guidelines for placement (Hermens et al., 2000). Marker positions were recorded at 200 Hz using a Qualisys Miqus motion capture system (Gothenburg, Sweden) with 13 cameras. Electromyography was recorded at 2,000 Hz. Ground reaction forces and moments were collected at 1,000 Hz.

We used custom Matlab scripts for data management and organization. Force plate data was low pass filtered with a 4th order Butterworth zero-phase filter at a cut-off frequency of 20 Hz. EMG data was bandpass filtered with a 4th order Butterworth filter with cutoff frequencies of 20 and 500 Hz, then rectified, then low-pass filtered with a 4th order Butterworth filter at a cut-off frequency of 6 Hz. For each subject and EMG channel, we calculated the average activation across all control strides and used this value to normalize EMG before averaging across subjects. From the marker data, we calculated joint angle data and center of mass (CoM) positions based on a geometric model with 15 segments (pelvis, torso, head, thighs, lower legs, feet, upper arms, forearms, hands) and 38 degrees of freedom (DoF) in OpenSim (John et al., 2007; Seth et al., 2018) using an existing base model (Zajac et al., 1990).

The experimental design allowed for four distinct stimulus conditions, where each leg could trigger a stimulus toward that leg during the 80 bpm metronome trials (Low cadence) or 110 bpm metronome trials (High cadence). Here we combine the conditions that are anatomically similar (i.e., [Right heel strike triggers Stimulus Right, Left heel strike triggers Stimulus Left]), and statistically supported to be symmetric (see “Trigger leg” factor in Table 1). This leaves two conditions to analyze, stimulus toward trigger leg in Low and High cadence conditions. After post-processing, we were left with 1,178 perturbations for Low cadence and 1567 perturbations for High cadence conditions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the ANOVA indicating difference between conditions and which factors have a significant effect on the magnitude of the motor response to the balance perturbation (see Table 2 for statistics on the existence of the mechanism).

We interpolated the data between heel strikes to 100 time points, representing percentage from heel strike to toe-off of the triggering leg. For the resulting interpolated trajectories for the perturbation steps, we subtracted the average of the control steps for the same stance foot. Deviation of the perturbation steps away from the average of the control steps were interpreted as the response to the perceived fall induced by the vestibular stimulus (Δ). For the model corrected foot placement, we fitted a linear regression model relating the foot placement changes for each subject to the changes of lateral position and velocity of the CoM at midstance using the control data (Wang and Srinivasan, 2014). Then for each stimulus step, we used this model to estimate the expected foot placement change based on the CoM state, and subtracted this from the observed foot placement change, resulting in an estimate of the foot placement change due to the vestibular stimulus (Reimann et al., 2017). We will refer to this model-based estimate as model corrected foot placement change.

2.4. Outcome Variables

We hypothesized the lateral ankle mechanism would play a larger role in balance control in the Low condition, while the foot placement mechanism would play a larger role in balance control in the High condition. Despite the expected difference in the use of the balance mechanisms, we also hypothesized that the overall shift of the CoM would not differ between conditions. All of the outcome variables pertain to the control of balance in the medial-lateral direction.

To test our hypothesis about the foot placement mechanism, we analyzed the following five variables that are directly or indirectly related to the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike: (i) the foot placement is defined as the Δ swing leg heel position relative to the trigger leg heel position at swing leg heel strike. (ii) the model correct foot placement is defined as the measured foot placement value and the foot placement value predicted based on the position and velocity of the CoM at mid-stance using the linear model (see above). (iii) the Δ trigger leg knee internal/external rotation angle. (iv) the Δ swing leg hip internal/external rotation angle. (v) the Δ swing leg hip abduction/adduction angle.

To test our hypothesis about the lateral ankle mechanism, we analyzed the following four variables related to the stance leg lateral ankle activation: (vi) the Δ CoP-CoM distance integrated over the time between the triggering heel strike and the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike. (vii) the Δ stance leg ankle eversion/inversion angle integrated over the time between the triggering heel strike and the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike. (viii) the Δ peroneus longus EMG of the stance leg integrated over the time between the triggering heel strike and the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike. (ix) the Δ tibialis anterior EMG of the stance leg integrated over the time between the triggering heel strike and the first post-stimulus swing leg heel strike.

To test our hypothesis about the push-off mechanism, we analyzed the following two variables related to the stance leg ankle push-off: (x) the Δ plantarflexion angle integrated over the second post-stimulus double stance phase. (xi) the Δ medial gastrocnemius EMG of the stance leg integrated over the first post-stimulus swing phase.

To test the our hypothesis about the overall balance response, we used (xii) the maximum shift of the CoM following the stimulus onset.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

We confirmed the assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity by visual inspection of the residual plots for the variables related to foot placement, lateral ankle, and push-off mechanisms. Our primary analysis is a group analysis of the kinematic, kinetic, and electromyographical basis for the three balance mechanisms. To test our hypotheses about whether the relative influence of balance mechanisms changes at different cadences, we used R (R Core Team, 2013) and lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) to perform a linear mixed effects analysis. For each outcome variable, we fitted a linear mixed model and performed an ANOVA to analyze the symmetry of the balance response and interaction of the stimulus direction. We used Satterthwaite's method (Fai and Cornelius, 1996) implemented in the R-package lmerTest (Kuznetsova, 2017). As fixed effects, we used triggering foot (left/right) to test the symmetry of the response and cadence (High/Low) to test the difference in response to the balance perturbation in the two conditions. As random effects, we used individual intercepts for subjects. To analyze whether the differences between stimulus and control steps represented by the outcome variables were statistically significant, we calculated the least squares means and estimated the 95% confidence intervals for the intercept of each outcome variable at each level of the significant factor, using a Kenward-Roger approximation (Halekoh and Højsgaard, 2014) implemented in the R-package emmeans (Lenth, 2016). We refrained from approximating p-values for the ANOVA directly in the traditional format, which can currently not be calculated reliably due to the lack of analytical results for linear mixed models (Bates et al., 2015). Results were judged statistically significant in two manners. First, the existence of the use of a variable associated with a balance mechanism in response to the perceived fall is judged statistically significant when the 95% confidence interval did not include zero (bolded in Table 2). Second, the difference in the use of the variables associated with a balance mechanism is judged statistically significant when the 95% confidence intervals for the Low and High condition did not overlap (highlighted gray in Table 2). We limited the statistical tests to the concrete hypotheses involving the use of the balance mechanisms that we had prior to performing the experiment (Brenner, 2015).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Least-squares means and upper and lower limits of the 95% confidence intervals for each outcome variable for High and Low cadence conditions using Kenward-Roger approximation.

3. Results

Subjects adjusted their stepping cadence in order to match the low or high metronome. Figure 2 shows box plots for the step time, step length and velocity for the unperturbed control steps for each cadence condition. Subjects tended to take longer, faster steps as a result of matching a higher frequency metronome (Figures 2A,B). The longer, faster steps resulted in higher overall walking velocities (Figure 2C).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Box plots of step time (A), step length (B), and velocity (C) for the unperturbed control steps. Boxes cover the first to third quartiles, whiskers show the upper and lower adjacents of the data, black lines are the means. Blue boxes correspond to High cadence, yellow boxes correspond to Low cadence.

Subjects responded to the balance perturbation by shifting their CoM in the direction opposite to the perceived fall direction, i.e., away from the triggering leg in both conditions (Figure 3), confirming a response to a perceived fall to the side. The CoM deviation peaks in the third single stance phase following the onset of the perturbation for the Low condition, and peaks in the fourth single stance phase following the onset of the perturbation for the High condition. The total CoM excursion was similar in both conditions, with the peak CoM excursion for the Low condition 116.31 and 123.06 mm for the High condition, on average. The peak occurs about the same time in both conditions, ∽2 s, despite differences in step frequency. In the following paragraphs, we will focus our analysis on the initial responses to the sensory perturbations during the first step and second double-stance period following the triggering heel-strike.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in the medial-lateral CoM position High and Low metronome conditions over 12 steps. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the twelfth heel strike post-stimulus. The gray, shaded, vertical areas correspond to double stance.

Figure 4 shows the displacement of the CoP relative to the CoM. The CoP shifts in the direction of the perceived fall (see Figure 4), i.e., toward the triggering leg, in both conditions, until the end of single stance. During the second double stance post-stimulus, the CoP moves away from the perceived fall in the Low condition, and further toward the perceived fall in the High condition.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in the medial-lateral CoP-CoM. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and end at the triggering foot toe-off. Blue curves correspond to fall stimuli toward the triggering leg during High metronome, yellow curves to fall stimuli toward the triggering leg during Low metronome. The gray vertical shaded areas correspond to double stance. DS, double stance; SS, single stance.

The shift of the CoP and CoM are generated by distinct balance mechanisms at different points of the gait cycle. The first mechanism that is able to act is the lateral ankle mechanism. The ankle of the new stance leg inverts during the first post-stimulus step relative to the unperturbed pattern (Figure 5A, Table 2). This ankle inversion change is larger in the Low cadence (Figure 5A, Table 2). Activity of the peroneus longus, an ankle everter decreases, beginning in the first double stance for both conditions. Activity of the tibialis anterior, an ankle inverter, increases, beginning in the first double stance post-stimulus for both conditions. These changes are larger in the Low condition for the tibialis anterior, but the confidence intervals overlap between the two cadence conditions for the peroneus longus (see Table 2). The timing and magnitude of these changes in kinematics and muscle activation align well with the initial CoP-CoM displacement in the direction of the perceived fall during the first step for both cadence conditions, as shown in Figure 4 and Table 2.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Variables illustrating the use of the lateral ankle mechanism. (A) The joint angle that contributes to the CoP-COM changes; and (B,C) the EMG that contributes to joint angle changes. The baseline represents the average for the control steps. Bold cures indicate the average and the light curves encasing the bold curves indicates the 95% confidence interval. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and show the subsequent two steps. Blue curves—High metronome, yellow curves—Low metronome. DS, double stance; SS, single stance.

At the first post-stimulus heel-strike, subjects shifted their foot placement in the direction of the perceived fall in the High condition (Figure 6A), as expected (see Table 2). Contrary to our expectation, subjects tended to shift their foot placement in the opposite direction in the Low condition, i.e., away from the perceived fall, although this shift was not statistically significant. Surprisingly, this general pattern was similar for the model-corrected foot placement (Figure 6B), where the shift away from the perceived fall in the Low condition is statistically significant, though smaller (see Table 2).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Variables illustrating the use of the change of the foot placement mechanism over time. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in the heel position (A), and joint angles (B–D) that contribute to change in heel position. The baseline represents the average for the control steps. Bold cures indicate the average and the light curves encasing the bold curves indicates the 95% confidence interval. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and show the subsequent two steps. Blue curves—High metronome, yellow curves—Low metronome. DS, double stance; SS, single stance.

For the High condition, the swing leg hip is slightly adducted upon heel strike (Figure 6B), though not statistically different from control steps (Table 2). A hip adduction modulation would contribute to the swing leg heel moving toward the trigger leg. The Low condition produces an abduction of the swing leg hip, allowing for movement of the heel away from the trigger leg. The combination of stance leg knee rotation and swing leg hip rotation yield produce a shift of the swing leg heel in the medial-lateral direction. For example, in the High condition, a trigger leg knee internal rotation (Figure 6C) and a swing leg hip external rotation (Figure 6D), in combination, result in a placement of the heel toward the trigger leg. The Low condition shows an internally rotated trigger leg knee, but also an internally rotated swing leg hip. The change in leg joint angles support the observed shift in the swing leg heel position.

The push-off mechanism was used for both cadence conditions (Table 2), where an increased trigger leg plantarflexion angle was observed in the second double stance following the triggering heel strike (Figure 8A). However, there was no difference in the plantarflexion modulation between cadence conditions (see Table 2). Similarly, there was no between-cadence difference in the trigger leg medial gastrocnemius EMG (see Table 2).

4. Discussion

We studied the changes of the control of balance at different cadences by constraining subjects' stepping to either High or Low cadence using a metronome and providing vestibular stimuli that induce the sensation of falling to the side. Subjects responded to the balance perturbations by accelerating their body away from the direction of the perceived fall regardless of the cadence as observed in Figure 3. We limited our analysis to the time interval from the heel-strike triggering the stimulus to the end of the second double stance post-stimulus, which encompasses the three previously identified balance mechanisms of lateral ankle roll, foot placement shift and push-off modulation. We observed changes in kinematics, ground reaction forces, and muscle activation to determine whether the neural control of balance is altered at different cadences. We found distinct changes in the use of balance mechanisms while walking at lower or higher cadences.

We found that the overall effect of the stimulus seems to be invariant of the cadence condition. Neither the time, nor the magnitude of the maximal CoM displacement in the direction of the perceived fall is significantly different between the two cadence conditions. However, the relative use of the mechanisms depends on the cadence. We found that the lateral ankle mechanism played a larger role in the Low condition, supported by differences in the CoP-CoM modulation, ankle inversion, and lateral ankle inverter (tibialis anterior) electromyography readings. The ankle inverted to a larger degree in Low, indicating that the larger shift of the CoP in the Low condition is a result of the use of the lateral ankle mechanism. The increased inversion, along with the prolonged single stance period allows the CoP and CoM to separate for a longer period of time. The combination of decreased peroneus longus muscle activity and increased activity of the tibialis anterior muscle in the Low condition compared to the High condition, yields an increased inversion angle (Figure 5C). The tibialis anterior is not only an ankle dorsiflexor, but also an ankle inverter (Gray, 1918). The combined changes responsible for ankle inversion suggests the central nervous system is actively generating a larger shift of the CoP under the stance foot in the Low condition. The increased use of the lateral ankle mechanism during the Low condition makes sense, given the increased duration of single stance in the Low condition.

We were surprised by the absence of a foot placement response to the perceived fall in the Low condition. We had expected the foot placement shift to be smaller in the Low condition, due to an increased and prolonged lateral ankle mechanism response, based on modeling results (Reimann et al., 2017). The lateral ankle mechanism accelerates the CoM, trying to stop the perceived fall, and this CoM shift is sensed by the CNS and invokes a balance response in the opposite direction, which is expected to cancel out the response to the stimulus to some degree. Following Wang and Srinivasan (2014), we fitted a regression model to the relationship between the CoM at midstance and the foot placement shift in the unperturbed reference, controlling for the different cadence conditions (Stimpson et al., 2018). We used this model to estimate the expected foot placement shift based on the normal variability of the CoM movement and calculated the model-corrected foot placement shift by subtracting the model prediction from the observed value. This model-corrected foot placement shift isolates the response to the sensory perturbation, which should be equal between the two cadence conditions. In previous experiments, this model-corrected foot placement shift showed a more consistent response to the sensory perturbation than the uncorrected value (Reimann et al., 2018b). Contrary to this expectation, the model-corrected foot placement change observed at Low cadence was significantly lower than at High cadence, and even below zero on average, though this was not statistically significant (Figure 7, Table 2).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. The foot placement response (A) and the model-corrected foot placement (B). The bar graph indicates the mean and the error bar indicates the 95% confidence interval.

The results indicate that all three previously identified balance mechanisms are used to respond to the perceived fall, but the majority of the balance response is shifted to the lateral ankle mechanism when walking with a low cadence. In contrast, when walking at a higher cadence, the foot placement mechanism dominates the balance response. The fact that the push-off mechanism does not differ between conditions indicates that the push-off may not be a critical balance mechanism, but may provide a subtle balance related adjustment at the end of stance phase. Unexpectedly, we observed a difference in the dorsiflexion angle between cadence conditions early in the gait cycle, with an increased dorsiflexion in the Low condition (see Figure 8A). Counterintuitively, the gastrocnemius EMG is also increased in the Low condition early in the single stance phase immediately following perturbation (see Figure 8B), therefore the dorsiflexion response must be attributed to the increased tibialis anterior activity (see Figure 5C). Such an observation indicates a form of “stiffening” that we plan to systematically analyze in a future publication.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Variables illustrating the use of the push-off mechanism. Changes in response to the balance perturbation in ankle dorsiflexion (A), and the medial gastrocnemius EMG (B). The baseline represents the average for the control steps. Bold cures indicate the average and the light curves encasing the bold curves indicates the 95% confidence interval. Curves start at the heel strike triggering the stimulus and show the subsequent two steps. Blue curves—High metronome, yellow curves—Low metronome. DS, double stance; SS, single stance.

These results support the idea of continuous monitoring of the balance response throughout the gait cycle, and that the balance mechanisms are in fact interdependent (Fettrow et al., 2019b). Considering that discussion of foot placement has dominated the walking literature as the primary balance response (Yiou et al., 2017; Bruijn and van Dieën, 2018), our results argue against a single balance mechanism for balance control and support the idea of a coordinated response of multiple mechanisms to achieve flexible control of balance while walking. As the vestibular perturbation elicits a perceived acceleration of the CoM in a particular direction, the CNS must shift the CoM an equal amount in the opposite direction to counter the perceived shift. The adjustment can be made continuously throughout the gait cycle, and the total balance response can be viewed as the summation of the balance mechanisms. This is not necessarily at odds with the findings of Dakin (2013) or Forbes (2017) where they found diminished vestibular reflexes in ankle muscles at higher walking velocities and cadences, but what we suggest is that the reflexive vestibular balance response may be shifted to more appropriate muscles for making use of the balance mechanism that is more readily available given the situation (i.e., foot placement at higher cadence). These results suggest that the lateral ankle and foot placement mechanisms are interdependent while interdependence between the push-off mechanism and the other mechanisms, is weak at best.

These findings will also inform our models of how the balance control system adjusts to altering gait parameters. The foot placement mechanism is the most commonly modeled balance mechanism (Townsend, 1985; Wang and Srinivasan, 2014), but to our knowledge no one has attempted to determine its use at varying cadences. Furthermore, models of human locomotion have difficulty walking at slower speeds (Song and Geyer, 2015). The difficulty with gait speed adjustment may point to limitations in the mechanisms available in these models for the control of balance. The inability for the models to walk slowly could be a result of missing the degree of freedom and control strategy to implement the lateral ankle, foot placement, and push-off mechanism in a coordinated fashion. To allow for a model to maintain walking at different gait speeds, multiple balance mechanisms may be required in order to reproduce as flexible a system as observed with human bipedal gait.

Finally, we speculate that these findings may shed light on the development of preferred walking speeds, particularly in populations that have difficulty with balance. Older adults tend to decrease gait speed with age (Himann et al., 1988; Lauretani et al., 2003), which according to this data set would result in more use of the lateral ankle mechanism. We also know that people with Parkinson's disease take shorter faster steps (Knutsson, 1972), and although their overall gait speed is diminished, an increase in cadence may shift the majority of the balance response to the foot placement compared to age matched controls. Reasons for why people with Parkinson's Disease would shift the balance response to foot placement lies in evidence that they have reduced proprioception (Hwang et al., 2016), possibly leading to an inability to sense the CoP under the stance foot. This deficit would make the use of the lateral ankle mechanism unreliable. If a particular balance mechanism is unreliable or cannot be used, the CNS must recruit the available balance mechanisms. In a hypothetical case that the lateral ankle mechanism activation is hindered, it seems logical to increase cadence to rely more on the foot placement mechanism. Thus, we speculate that balance mechanisms play a role in preferred cadence, and possibly gait speed. Future experiments will attempt to directly assess the role of cadence in balance and preferred gait speed in older adults and those with neurological conditions, such as Parkinson's disease.

4.1. Limitations

We limited our analysis to the time between triggering leg heel strike and the second double stance post-stimulus because all three balance mechanisms are used within this time frame, but also due to the difficulty interpreting subsequent steps. The response to the perturbation may continue into the next gait cycle (balance perturbation continues for 1,000 ms), but at some point the CNS responds to the self-inflated fall due to the response to the perceived fall. It is difficult to uncover when precisely the CNS determines the initial response to the balance perturbation is erroneous, but we speculate that the total balance response to the perturbation is similar between cadence conditions due to the similar overall deviation of the CoM in Figure 3. Future analyses will expand on this by incorporating global stability measures, such as margin of stability (Hof et al., 2005), to determine how overall stability may change with the relative use of the balance mechanisms.

Our current results show that higher cadence results in faster gait speed, at least for healthy young adults walking on a self-paced treadmill. The relationship between gait speed and cadence is non-trivial, making it problematic to unpack the effects of cadence vs. gait speed. The convergence to a faster gait speed when adopting a higher cadence may be related to metabolic efficiency, but this is an avenue far removed from the focus of the current work. To our knowledge this relationship has not been explored.

Our current experimental methods may lead to limitations in the presentation of the push-off mechanism. We refer to the change in ankle plantarflexion in response to the balance perturbation as push-off, because to date this is the best term to describe what we believe the change in ankle plantarflexion does. Due to the experimental design, we do not have reliable ground reaction forces and moments. Providing balance perturbations, in addition to instructions to “walk normally,” leads to many situations where one foot is on two force plates, or during double stance, two feet are on one force plate. These situations make it difficult to determine the ground reaction forces and moments associated with the change in ankle plantarflexion angle.

Another technical consideration in the current methodology is the heel strike identification method. Different walking speeds in the cadence conditions produced different vertical heel trajectory profiles which required intervention by the experimenter (see Methods). Increasing the vertical threshold created a longer period of time from threshold crossing to actual heel strike for the Low condition. Therefore, the stimulus for the Low condition was triggered on average ~175 ms prior to heel strike, and the stimulus for the High condition was triggered ~110 ms prior to heel strike, coincidentally corresponding to ~20% of the time between heel strikes for each condition. The earlier trigger in the Low condition may partially explain the earlier response time in the CoP-CoM (Figure 4) and corresponding EMG for the lateral ankle mechanism (Figures 5B,C), but not the amplitude.

5. Conclusion

We investigated whether individuals altered the control of balance if they were asked to step at different frequencies while walking on a self-paced treadmill as they periodically received a vestibularly-induced sensation of a fall to the side. The current findings support the idea that balance mechanisms are coordinated to produce an overall balance response. In the Low condition, the lateral ankle mechanism plays the primary role in the overall balance response, while the foot placement mechanism is not observed. When cadence is decreased (Low), single stance is longer, providing more opportunity to modulate the CoP through ankle roll, and diminishing the need for a change in foot placement to maintain upright posture. In the High condition, with shorter single stance duration, the lateral ankle mechanism is less effective and the balance response shifts to reliance on the foot placement mechanism. These findings suggest lateral stability is not dependent on cadence or gait speed, but the method of obtaining stability is altered with cadence, providing evidence of a flexible neural control scheme that adapts to changing constraints while walking. Moreover, such findings provide insight into adoption of preferred gait parameters, particularly in populations who have drastically altered gait speed or cadence, such as older adults or people with Parkinson's disease.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript/supplementary files.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Delaware IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TF, HR, DG, and JJ designed the experiment. TF, HR, and DG performed the experiment. TF, HR, DG, JC, JH, and JJ analyzed the data. TF, HR, DG, JC, JH, and JJ wrote the manuscript.

Funding

HR was funded from a Research Fellowship from the German Research Foundation (RE 3780/1-1). HR, TF, and JJ were funded by the National Science Foundation (NSF CRCNS 1822568).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This manuscript has been released as a Pre-Print at BioRxiv (Fettrow et al., 2019a).

References

Anson, E., Pineault, K., Bair, W., Studenski, S., and Agrawal, Y. (2019). Reduced vestibular function is associated with longer, slower steps in healthy adults during normal speed walking. Gait Posture 68, 340–345. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardestani, M. M., Ferrigno, C., Moazen, M., and Wimmer, M. A. (2016). From normal to fast walking: impact of cadence and stride length on lower extremity joint moments. Gait Posture 46, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mäechler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 1, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text

Bauby, C. E., and Kuo, A. D. (2000). Active control of lateral balance in human walking. J. Biomech. 33, 1433–1440. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(00)00101-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bent, L., McFadyen, B., Merkley, V., Kennedy, P., and Inglis, T. (2000). Magnitude effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation on the trajectory of human gait. Neurosci. Lett. 279, 157–160. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00989-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenner, E. (2015). Why we need to do fewer statistical tests. Perception 45, 489–491. doi: 10.1177/0301006616637434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruijn, S. M., and van Dieën, J. H. (2018). Control of human gait stability through foot placement. J. R. Soc. Interface 15:20170816. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruijn, S. M., van Dieën, J. H., Meijer, O. G., and Beek, P. J. (2009). Is slow walking more stable? J Biomech. 42, 1506–1512. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.03.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckley, E., Mazzà, C., and McNeill, A. (2018). A systematic review of the gait characteristics associated with cerebellar ataxia. Gait Posture 60, 154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.11.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis Iii, R. B., Õunpuu, S., Tyburski, D., and Gage, J. R. (1991). A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique. Hum. Mov. Sci. 10, 575–587. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(91)90046-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delp, S. L., Anderson, F. C., Arnold, A. S., Loan, P., Habib, A., John, C. T., et al. (2007). OpenSim: open-source software to create and analyze dynamic simulations of movement. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 54, 1940–1950. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2007.901024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delp, S. L., Loan, J. P., Hoy, M. G., Zajac, F. E., Topp, E. L., and Rosen, J. M. (1990). An interactive graphics-based model of the lower extremity to study orthopaedic surgical procedures. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 37, 757–767. doi: 10.1109/10.102791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dingwell, J. B., and Marin, L. C. (2006). Kinematic variability and local dynamic stability of upper body motions when walking at different speeds. J. Biomech. 39, 444–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donker, S. F., and Beek, P. J. (2002). Interlimb coordination in prosthetic walking: effects of asymmetry and walking velocity. Acta Psychol. 110, 265–288. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00037-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan-Porter, W., Vo, T. N., Ullman, K., Langsetmo, L., Strotmeyer, E. S., Taylor, B., et al. (2019). Hospitalization-associated change in gait speed and risk of functional limitations for older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 74, 1657–1663. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glz027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

England, S. A., and Granata, K. P. (2007). The influence of gait speed on local dynamic stability of walking. Gait Posture 25, 172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fai, A. H.-T., and Cornelius, P. L. (1996). Approximate F-tests of multiple degree of freedom hypotheses in generalized least squares analyses of unbalanced split-plot experiments. J. Stat. Comput. Simul. 54, 363–378. doi: 10.1080/00949659608811740

CrossRef Full Text

Fan, Y., Li, Z., Han, S., Lv, C., and Zhang, B. (2016). The influence of gait speed on the stability of walking among the elderly. Gait Posture 47, 31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fettrow, T., Reimann, H., Grenet, D., Crenshaw, J., and Higginson, J. (2019a). Walking cadence affects the recruitment of the medial-lateral balance mechanisms. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/658070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fettrow, T. D., Reimann, H., Thompson, E. D., Grenet, D., and Jeka, J. (2019b). Interdependence of balance mechanisms during walking. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/658120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, R. C., Wardman, D. L., and Taylor, J. L. (1999). Effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation during human walking. J. Physiol. 517(Pt 3), 931–939. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0931s.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, D. H., Scott, S. J., Wilken, J. M., and Dingwell, J. B. (2013). Frontal plane dynamic margins of stability in individuals with and without transtibial amputation walking on a loose rock surface. Gait Posture 38, 570–575. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, H. (1918). Anatomy of the Human Body. 20th Edn. New York, NY: Lea and Febiger.

Halekoh, U., and Højsgaard, S. (2014). A Kenward-Roger approximation and parametric bootstrap methods for tests in linear mixed models–the R package pbkrtest. J. Stat. Softw. 59. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayden, R., Clair, A. A., Johnson, G., and Otto, D. (2009). The effect of rhythmic auditory stimulation (ras) on physical therapy outcomes for patients in gait training following stroke: a feasibility study. Int. J. Neurosci. 119, 2183–2195. doi: 10.3109/00207450903152609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermens, H. J., Freriks, B., Disselhorst-Klug, C., and Rau, G. (2000). Hermens 2000 development of recommendations for SEMG sensors and sensor placement procedures. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 10, 361–374. doi: 10.1016/S1050-6411(00)00027-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Himann, J. E., Cunningham, D. A., Rechnitzer, P. A., and Paterson, D. H. (1988). Age-related changes in speed of walking. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 20, 161–166. doi: 10.1249/00005768-198820020-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L. (2007). The equations of motion for a standing human reveal three mechanisms for balance The equations of motion for a standing human reveal three mechanisms for balance. J. Biomech. 40, 451–457. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2005.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L., Gazendam, M. G., and Sinke, W. E. (2005). The condition for dynamic stability. J. Biomech. 38, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L., van Bockel, R. M., Schoppen, T., and Postema, K. (2007). Control of lateral balance in walking. Experimental findings in normal subjects and above-knee amputees. Gait Posture 25, 250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, A. L., Vermerris, S. M., and Gjaltema, W. A. (2010). Balance responses to lateral perturbations in human treadmill walking. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 2655–2664. doi: 10.1242/jeb.042572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, H., Gray, V. L., Creath, R. A., Binder-Macleod, S. A., and Rogers, M. W. (2017). Control of lateral weight transfer is associated with walking speed in individuals post-stroke. J. Biomech. 60, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, S., Agada, P., Grill, S., Kiemel, T., and Jeka, J. (2016). A central processing sensory deficit with Parkinson's disease. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 2369–2379. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4642-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iles, J. F., Baderin, R., Tanner, R., and Simon, A. (2007). Human standing and walking: comparison of the effects of stimulation of the vestibular system. Exp. Brain Res. 178, 151–166. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0721-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., and Collins, S. H. (2015). Once-per-step control of ankle-foot prosthesis push-off work reduces effort associated with balance during walking. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 12:43. doi: 10.1186/s12984-015-0027-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klemetti, R., Moilanen, P., Avela, J., and Timonen, J. (2014). Effects of gait speed on stability of walking revealed by simulated response to tripping perturbation. Gait Posture 39, 534–539. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutsson, E. (1972). An analysis of {Parkinsonian} gait. Brain 95, 475–486. doi: 10.1093/brain/95.3.475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, A. D. (1999). Stabilization of lateral motion in passive dynamic walking. Int. J. Robot. Res. 18, 917–930. doi: 10.1177/02783649922066655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauretani, F., Russo, C. R., Bandinelli, S., Bartali, B., Cavazzini, C., Di Iorio, A., et al. (2003). Age-associated changes in skeletal muscles and their effect on mobility: an operational diagnosis of sarcopenia. J. Appl. Physiol. 95, 1851–1860. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00246.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: the R package lsmeans. J. Stat. Softw. 69. doi: 10.18637/jss.v069.i01

CrossRef Full Text

Maki, B. E. (1997). Gait changes in older adults: indicators of fear? J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 45, 313–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1997.tb00946.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Makino, K., Makizako, H., Doi, T., Tsutsumimoto, K., Hotta, R., Nakakubo, S., et al. (2017). Fear of falling and gait parameters in older adults with and without fall history. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 17, 2455–2459. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrum, C., Lucieer, F., van de Berg, R., Willems, P., Fornos, A. P., Guinand, N., et al. (2018). Is faster always better? The walking speed-dependency of gait variability in bilateral vestibulopathy. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/413955

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menz, H. B., Lord, S. R., St. George, R., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2004). Walking stability and sensorimotor function in older people with diabetic peripheral neuropathy. Archiv. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 245–252. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimento, L. R., de Oliveira, C. Q., Ada, L., Michaelsen, S. M., and Teixeira-Salmela, L. F. (2015). Walking training with cueing of cadence improves walking speed and stride length after stroke more than walking training alone: a systematic review. J. Physiother. 61, 10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jphys.2014.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oie, K., Kiemel, T., and Jeka, J. J. (2002). Multisensory function: simultaneous re-weighting of vision and touch for the control of human posture. Cognit. Brain Res. 14, 164–176. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00071-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterka, R. J. (2002). Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1097–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

PubMed Abstract

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T., and Jeka, J. J. (2018a). Strategies for the control of balance during locomotion. Kinesiol. Rev. 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1123/kr.2017-0053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T., Thompson, E. D., and Jeka, J. J. (2018b). Neural control of balance during walking. Front. Physiol. 9:1271. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimann, H., Fettrow, T. D., Thompson, E. D., Agada, P., McFadyen, B., and Jeka, J. J. (2017). Complementary mechanisms for upright balance during walking. PLoS ONE 12:e0172215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roos, P. E., and Dingwell, J. B. (2013). Influence of neuromuscular noise and walking speed on fall risk and dynamic stability in a 3D dynamic walking model. J. Biomech. 46, 1722–1728. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schniepp, R., Schlick, C., Schenkel, F., Pradhan, C., Jahn, K., Brandt, T., et al. (2017). Clinical and neurophysiological risk factors for falls in patients with bilateral vestibulopathy. J. Neurol. 264, 277–283. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8342-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schniepp, R., Wuehr, M., Neuhaeusser, M., Kamenova, M., Dimitriadis, K., Klopstock, T., et al. (2012). Locomotion speed determines gait variability in cerebellar ataxia and vestibular failure. Mov. Disord. 27, 125–131. doi: 10.1002/mds.23978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A., Hicks, J. L., Uchida, T. K., Habib, A., Dembia, C. L., Dunne, J. J., et al. (2018). OpenSim: simulating musculoskeletal dynamics and neuromuscular control to study human and animal movement. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14:e1006223. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., and Geyer, H. (2015). A neural circuitry that emphasizes spinal feedback generates diverse behaviours of human locomotion. J. Physiol. 593, 3493–3511. doi: 10.1113/JP270228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., and Geyer, H. (2018). Predictive neuromechanical simulations indicate why walking performance declines with ageing. J. Physiol. 596, 1199–1210. doi: 10.1113/JP275166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stimpson, K. H., Heitkamp, L. N., Horne, J. S., and Dean, J. C. (2018). Effects of walking speed on the step-by-step control of step width. J. Biomech. 68, 78–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., and Abiru, M. (2010). Rhythmic auditory stimulation in rehabilitation of movement disorders: a review of current research. Music Percept. Interdiscipl. J. 27, 263–269. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, M. A. (1985). Biped gait stabilization via foot placement. J. Biomech. 18, 21–38. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(85)90042-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlutters, M., van Asseldonk, E. H., and van der Kooij, H. (2016). Center of mass velocity-based predictions in balance recovery following pelvis perturbations during human walking. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 1514–1523. doi: 10.1242/jeb.129338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Srinivasan, M. (2014). Stepping in the direction of the fall: the next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biol. Lett. 10, 1–5. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., and Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 213–216. doi: 10.1038/84054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, D. A. (1995). Human blance and posture control during standing and walking. Gait Posture 3, 193–214. doi: 10.1016/0966-6362(96)82849-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittwer, J. E., Webster, K. E., and Hill, K. (2013). Rhythmic auditory cueing to improve walking in patients with neurological conditions other than Parkinson's disease-what is the evidence. Disabil. Rehabil. 35, 164–176. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2012.690495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wuehr, M., Nusser, E., Decker, J., Krafczyk, S., Straube, A., Brandt, T., et al. (2016). Noisy vestibular stimulation improves dynamic walking stability in bilateral vestibulopathy. Neurology 86, 2196–2202. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiou, E., Caderby, T., Delafontaine, A., Fourcade, P., and Honeine, J.-L. (2017). Balance control during gait initiation: state-of-the-art and research perspectives. World J. Orthoped. 8, 815–828. doi: 10.5312/wjo.v8.i11.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mechanisms, walking, locomotion, cadence, balance, foot placement, push-off

Citation: Fettrow T, Reimann H, Grenet D, Crenshaw J, Higginson J and Jeka J (2019) Walking Cadence Affects the Recruitment of the Medial-Lateral Balance Mechanisms. Front. Sports Act. Living 1:40. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2019.00040

Received: 11 July 2019; Accepted: 16 September 2019;
Published: 01 October 2019.

Edited by:

Fabio Augusto Barbieri, State University Paulista Júlio de Mesquita Filho, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Hanatsu Nagano, Victoria University, Australia
Christopher McCrum, Maastricht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2019 Fettrow, Reimann, Grenet, Crenshaw, Higginson and Jeka. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tyler Fettrow, tfettrow@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.