Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 31 October 2023
Sec. Elite Sports and Performance Enhancement
This article is part of the Research Topic Hockey: Testing and Performance View all 7 articles

Developmental activities of elite junior hockey players: an analysis of early sport specialization

\r\nW. J. GarlandW. J. GarlandK. L. Smith
K. L. Smith*J. C. Dixon&#x;J. C. DixonS. Horton&#x;\r\nS. Horton
  • Department of Kinesiology, University of Windsor, Windsor, ON, Canada

Early sport specialization is a popular and contentious topic in the scientific literature and popular media. The lure of extrinsic rewards has led to increasing rates of specialization among young athletes, while expert recommendations promote multisport participation. The purpose of this study was to describe and analyze developmental activities of a group of elite junior hockey players in Canada. Within this context, elements of specialization were investigated in accordance with existing theoretical frameworks and long-term athlete development models to enhance the literature. Fifteen participants from the Ontario Hockey League completed quantitative retrospective interviews, detailing past sport and recreational activities. Thirty-one developmental milestones were assessed. Accumulated hours of activity were categorized in accordance with Côté's (1999) Developmental Model of Sports Participation, along with the number and types of sports in which they participated during childhood. Jayanthi et al.'s (2015) continuum was utilized to determine the age at which the athletes became moderately and highly specialized. Accrued hours of deliberate practice reported by participants increased from ages 6 to 16 years, as did competition in organized hockey games. Reported hours of deliberate play peaked at 9 years of age and decreased thereafter. Participants played a combined 16 sports other than hockey, ranging from an average of 2.0 at age 6, to a maximum average of 5.6 at 12 years old, and decreasing each year to 2.3 by age 15. The greatest number of hours in other sports was accumulated at 12 years of age. Using a three-point scale, participants considered themselves “highly specialized” at 14 years old; however, other quantitative indicators suggested this may have occurred at 12 years of age. Relative to previous research on early sport specialization, participants in this study spent more time practicing hockey, while ceasing hockey-specific play and other sports at younger ages. Despite a diverse sport history, hockey competition was initiated earlier than recommended, showing high levels of sport commitment as young as 9 years old. The early specialization path remains a popular trajectory among coaches, parents, and athletes in Canadian ice hockey.

1. Introduction

Sport specialization generally refers to concentrated training in one sport for a significant portion of the year at the expense of participation in other sport and activities (14); although operational definitions vary in the published literature [see (5) for further discussion]. This developmental trajectory in youth sport has become both a popular and contentious topic in the literature and popular media, as experts debate the benefits and consequences of intensified participation for young athletes (69). The belief that intense participation is the best route to an athletic scholarship (10), or a professional contract (11) has led to a dramatic rise in privatized programs and year-round participation, and a decrease in the number of multi-sport athletes (1215). Yet, athletes, parents, coaches, and administrators alike, may be overlooking the documented physical and psychological consequences of this path as they pursue the external rewards associated with athletic success (4, 16).

Early sport specialization is not a new approach with respect to talent development, dating back to the 1950s when Eastern European countries introduced intensive training programs in the quest for political power through Olympic success (17, 18). Coaching philosophies gradually migrated West with promotion of year-round participation in a single sport at a young age, privatized training (19), and commercialization of sport programs (12)1. Modern day athletes have many motives to excel in their given domains, and the belief that intense, year-round participation is the best route to excellence has flourished (15, 28). For instance, the National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) has studied the trend of early specialization and reported that more than 50% of male soccer and hockey players have specialized by 12 years of age (29), which is notably short of the completion of adolescence wherein young athletes undergo a significant period of physical maturation and might be at increased risk of injury (19, 30). Despite the many proponents of early sport diversification [e.g., (13, 31)], trends show a decrease in the number of multi-sport athletes, presumably due to private entities promoting specialization as a path to elite status [(32); see (2) for further discussion].

The popularity of sport specialization is also tied to the work of Ericsson et al. (33), whose seminal paper outlined a monotonic relationship between practice and performance, such that the more one practices, the better one will perform. This paper further supported the 10-year model proposed by Simon and Chase (34) following an examination of chess players, suggesting that 10 years of meaningful, purposeful practice is needed to demonstrate expertise in a given domain. This form of training is often referred to as deliberate practice and consists of highly structured, effortful activities aimed at improving specific aspects of performance (33). Support for this concept has been garnered across several domains including sport [e.g., (3537)], although a notable distinction has accompanied sport-specific findings. Helsen and colleagues (36) reported that practice is often seen as enjoyable and highly relevant to athletes, contradicting Ericsson et al.'s (33) observations of the talented musicians who reported practice as relatively less enjoyable in comparison to other activities. Deliberate practice has reliably differentiated skill level across sport-specific studies [see (38, 39)], although not all individuals benefit equally from the effects of training (40) and early specialization is not necessarily a requirement to reach elite status (31).

In contrast to the premises put forth by the deliberate practice framework, Côté (41) explained an athlete's development through the Developmental Model of Sport Participation (DMSP). The first stage, referred to as “sampling” (i.e., ages 6–13), occurs when athletes experience a variety of different sports with an emphasis on fun and enjoyment. During the “specialization” stage (i.e., ages 13–15), athletes commit to one or two sports and achievement becomes more important. Finally, during the “investment” stage, athletes (over 15 years of age) commit to one sport with the hope of attaining an elite level of performance. There is a possibility of a fourth stage if a high degree of accomplishment has been attained; this is referred to as the “performance” or “perfection” stage. During this time, athletes maintain their elite status and work towards perfecting the skills of their domain.

As an alternative to the DMSP model, Balyi and colleagues (42) proposed a seven-stage model based on developmental stages, aimed at optimizing long-term athlete development (LTAD) and promoting physical activity in the general population. The first stages of “active start”, “fundamentals”, and “learn to train” promote the skills required for physical literacy and are recommended to be completed before the onset of adolescence. High performance athletes may then progress through the stages of “train to train”, “train to compete”, and “train to win” by showing key psychosocial and physical indicators. As expected with high performance, tiered systems and increasingly difficult challenges cause athletes to be filtered out at each stage, allowing fewer to move on to the subsequent stage or tier. Ideally, as these athletes are filtered out of the high-performance system, they enter the “active for life” stage with the goal of having them stay active and healthy. This seven-stage model has been adapted and implemented at the developmental level by various sport governing bodies. For instance, Hockey Canada (43) made the stages specific to the contextual and logistical constraints of minor hockey in Canada. The program is designed to increase physical literacy and life-long engagement in hockey activities by encouraging young players to participate in other activities until the age of 12, when it is recommended to increase the specificity of training towards hockey.

The cultural shift toward early sport specialization has also prompted several organizations to release position statements advising against this potentially harmful trend. For instance, the International Olympic Committee has outlined recommendations for managing the sport-life balance of young athletes in their consensus statement (44), and the American Orthopaedic Society for Sports Medicine called for improved messaging concerning specialization, due to associations with adverse outcomes for athletes (45). While a positive correlation does exist between practice and performance, the recommended amount and type of training differs at various stages of development (23, 35, 36, 46, 47). Moreover, accumulating more practice by age 15 and selection to elite junior teams does not necessarily predict success at higher levels [see (31)]. Thus, early specialization could be associated with an athlete reaching their highest level of play prematurely (48, 49).

Further, intense training without adequate rest has been identified as an independent risk factor for injury (14, 50). This risk may be attributed to repetitive movements, strength and flexibility imbalances, inadequate neuromuscular skills to prevent injury, and specialized athletes pushing themselves with more intensity (10, 14, 51). For example, a common overuse injury sustained by ice hockey players is femoroacetabular impingement (FAI),2 which occurs due to the repetitive biomechanics of ice skating (53). Risk factors for FAI include on-ice exposure and younger start ages, and the prevalence increases with age showing that hockey-specific movements may lead to the development of FAI (53). This is noteworthy, as hockey reports a high incidence of early specialization (54). Psychosocially, early specialization may alter relationships, create socially maladaptive behaviour, increase the risk of anxiety, depression, and burnout, and decrease intrinsic motivation (2, 4). These negative outcomes may be associated with the pressure created by overly structured, adult-driven training schedules (2, 10, 26, 55).

Following recommendations for sport diversification and specialization within the Canadian developmental system for ice hockey has had its challenges. The Canadian Hockey League (CHL) serves as the primary developmental league for the National Hockey League (56, 57), and consists of the Western Hockey League (WHL), Ontario Hockey League (OHL), and Quebec Major Junior Hockey League (QMJHL). Players may enter the CHL as early as 15 years old (58) and thus, may feel pressure to accumulate practice hours in ice hockey at the expense of other sports to demonstrate superior ability prior to that age (2). The adapted Balyi et al. (42) developmental model recommended by Hockey Canada (43) suggests hockey-specific training should be initiated at the age of 12, leaving a limited window of opportunity for players to elevate their skills above the competition. The existing developmental literature involving CHL players [e.g., (25, 59)] was published after the 1999 Molson Open Ice Summit on player development. As a consequence of this summit, Hockey Canada changed its grassroots programs, and recommended the implementation of “skills academies” throughout the country (60). These changes led to as many as 5,000 elementary and secondary schools offering hockey as part of their curricula, allowing minor hockey players as young as 12 years old to focus on activities specific to hockey (60, 61).

While empirical research regarding specialization and LTAD is found for several sports, including soccer [e.g., (23)], volleyball (24), and athletics (50)—relatively little is available for hockey. Thus, the purpose of this study was to describe and analyze the developmental activities of a group of elite junior (OHL) hockey players to investigate elements of specialization and enhance the LTAD literature for one of Canada's national sports. The developmental activities of CHL players potentially affected by changes occurring after the Open Ice Summit were considered. Accumulated hours of activity were categorized in accordance with the DMSP, along with the number and types of sports in which participants engaged during childhood. Jayanthi et al.'s (62) continuum was utilized to determine the age at which the athletes became moderately and highly specialized. Given current trends and the popularity of ice hockey in Canada, it was hypothesized that “specialization” and “investment” would occur earlier than suggested by the DMSP, and that the athletes would be considered “highly specialized” prior to recommended ages, according to Jayanthi et al.'s (62) continuum.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Potential participants were current or former OHL players who were born no earlier than January 1st, 1994. These inclusion criteria created a sample of elite junior hockey players who had initiated their developmental activities after the Open Ice Summit on player development (60). All participants were male and had spent a portion of their developmental years in the Kent or Essex counties of Ontario, Canada. Participants were from a relatively small geographical area in Ontario giving similar proximity to training resources. Patton (63) refers to this type of criterion-based sampling as purposeful sampling, which is intended to create an information-rich and homogeneous sample for an in-depth analysis of cases that have been determined to be important.

2.2. Questionnaire/instruments

The retrospective semi-structured interview script3 used in this study was an adapted version of the guide developed by Côté et al. (64) to obtain quantitative information regarding the developmental activities of participants in various stages throughout their childhood up to their time in the OHL. Versions of this interview have been used in several previous studies for other sports, including triathlon [e.g., (65)], volleyball [e.g., (66)], soccer [e.g., (67)], rugby [e.g., (68)] and swimming [e.g., (69)]. The information collected from the interviews was based on questions from three sections: (a) “early activities”, (b) “maturation and performance in main sport”, and (c) “relevant practice activities in main sport”. This information helped to generate longitudinal, methodical, and comprehensive histories of the participants' developmental activities. The interview script also involved closed-ended questions to add context by rating each activity on several subjective measures, such as effort, mental concentration, and enjoyment.

The “early activities” section created a list of activities that participants engaged in throughout their development. Participants were asked to recall the number of hours per week, and the number of months per year, for every year that they took part in each activity. One adaptation from Côté et al.'s (64) interview guide was a designation of peak and off-peak months. This was included to account for the changes in training hours between seasons. The “maturation and performance in main sport” section asked each participant for specific ages of developmental milestones, information regarding injuries, subjective feelings towards training intensity and resources, and personal and team accomplishments. Finally, the “relevant practice activities in main sport” section regrouped the information previously collected into periods of similar training quality and quantity. These periods helped to quantify the time and effort required for the activities identified in previous sections of the interview. All sections of the interview focused on the participants' relevant activities from the age of 6 years and throughout their development to include those pursued at the time of the interview.

To identify the age at which participants became moderately and highly specialized, the scale suggested by Jayanthi et al. (62) was utilized. Participants were asked when they chose hockey as their main sport, when they engaged in hockey for more than eight months per year, and when they first dropped out of other activities to focus their attention on hockey4. Participants were also asked if they had ever been told that they were not permitted to play other sports. Those who answered in the affirmative were also asked who made the request, and what rationale was provided, to gain an understanding of why participants specialized in hockey. Participants were then asked if they had ever attended a hockey academy during their development and if they had ever relocated to play hockey. This was to determine the rationale for the relocation, and if there were any trends among the participants.

2.3. Procedure

Current and former OHL players were approached by the first author to explain the study and its objectives. Interviews were conducted on an individual basis at a private location convenient to the participant, and each interview took roughly two hours to complete. Confidentiality was maintained by using pseudonyms for each participant. The first author assisted the participants by helping them during the interview which aided them in maintaining focus and ensured the validity and reliability of their data. These procedures received clearance from the Research Ethics Board at the authors' home institution and all participants provided their informed consent.

2.4. Reliability

Côté et al. (64) first proposed their interview guide to reduce the limitations of athletes’ recall accuracy. The interview procedure describes how to collect verifiable information, trace longitudinal changes in developmental activities, and assess the validity and reliability of the information given. Questions focus on the objective recall of specific episodic experiences (e.g., training hours), which are considered reliable, in part, because of the large impact that they have on athletes' lives. Côté et al. (64) found the test re-test reliability to be between .70 and .84 for these questions, and the convergent validity between athletes' recall and that of others with knowledge of their training quantity was found to be statistically reliable (e.g., parents, coaches, training partners). During subsequent studies, the test-retest reliability was determined to be between .83 and .98 (66, 70, 71). Convergent validity involving similar methods in other studies was between .76 and 1.00 (70, 72, 73). In this study, the first author had general knowledge of the developmental histories of each participant, which facilitated accurate recall of specific events, in comparison to a researcher unfamiliar with the athletes' past activities (64).

2.5. Data analyses

The interview systematically explored all activities that each participant had undertaken during development. This included a record of the number of hours per week and the number of months per year for each activity, allowing for the calculation of the total number of hours per year. As per the interview guide, these hours were then separated into “sport”, “artistic”, and “other” (e.g., watching TV) categories. Consistent with the methodology used in Soberlak and Côté (25), sporting activities were divided into “deliberate practice activities”, “deliberate play activities”, “organized games”, and “other sports”. The hours accumulated in each category were divided in accordance with the age groups suggested in the DMSP: sampling (i.e., ages 6–13), and specializing (i.e., ages 13–15). Descriptive statistics (i.e., mean, median, standard deviation, maximum and minimum hours) were calculated and graphed to identify trends, and analyzed to determine if transitions were consistent with those proposed in the DMSP. “Deliberate practice activities” were defined as any activities that had the purpose of improving the participants' performances in hockey [e.g., self-initiated practice, team practice, individual coaching; (33)]. “Deliberate play activities” were hockey-related activities that had the goal of providing enjoyment to the participants (41). “Organized games” were any organized hockey competitions, and “other sports” were sports other than hockey that were identified by participants. The findings highlight descriptive statistics derived from the interviews.

Due to the importance and relevance of sporting activities, each participant's involvement was categorized using Thorpe et al.'s (74) games classification model, similar to the methodology used in Berry et al. (72). The Thorpe et al. (74) model classifies all games into one of five categories based on the basic strategies and principles: invasion (e.g., hockey, soccer), net/wall (e.g., tennis, volleyball), field/run scoring (e.g., baseball), target (e.g., golf), and individual (e.g., athletics). Hockey qualifies as an invasion sport because the purpose is to invade the opponent's territory and score on the opponent's goal. Descriptive statistics of the accumulated hours of structured and unstructured involvement in each of these sport classifications were calculated, as well as the frequency of the various activities identified by participants.

According to the scale developed by Jayanthi et al. (62), all the participants in this study were considered highly specialized at the time of the interview. Therefore, the age at which each athlete first qualified as highly specialized was determined and reported with descriptive statistics. The remaining questions about playing other sports, relocation, and hockey academy attendance were included in the analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Fifteen participants were interviewed, all of whom were current or former OHL players. These athletes had a mean age of 19.6 years with the oldest athlete being 22 years of age, and the youngest being 16. The number of OHL games played by participants ranged from 51 to 294 (mean of 145, median of 139). Since all athletes had commenced playing in the OHL by 17 years of age, results were calculated up to and including 16 years of age for each athlete.

3.2. Accumulated hours of activity

The number of estimated activity hours that each athlete accumulated per year in “sport5”, “artistic6”, and “other” categories is reported in Table 1. For each age, the participants reported accumulating a greater number of total hours in the “sport” category, and attained greater mean, median, maximum and minimum values than the “artistic” and “other” categories combined. The maximum number of activity hours per year reported by participants occurred at the age of 13 for “sport” (16,338), 9 years old for “artistic” (774), and 13 years of age for “other” (3,508).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of accumulated activity hours by age.

The “sport” hours accumulated were further classified as hockey-specific “deliberate practice”, hockey-specific “deliberate play”, “organized [hockey] games”, and sports other than hockey. Descriptive statistics for each of these categories are presented in Table 2, and the total accumulated hours of all participants at each age are graphed in Figure 1. The combined total hours of all participants were lowest at 6 years old (2,688) and steadily increased until participants were 16 years of age, when deliberate practice achieved its highest combined maximum (9,892), and median (568) number of hours. The highest combined total of deliberate play hours was achieved at 9 years of age (3,160), which decreased sharply thereafter. Despite the decline in combined accumulated hours of deliberate play, participants had the highest median (156) hours at 11 years of age. The number of hours that participants engaged in organized games gradually increased at each age, with the highest combined total (3,668), and median (256) at age 16. Finally, when considering sports other than hockey, participants had the highest combined total of hours (5,342), median hours (282), and individual maximum hours at 12 years of age (1,300), at which point the commitment to non-hockey related sport activities decreased steadily.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for accumulated sport hour categories.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The combined accumulated hour totals for hockey-specific deliberate practice, hockey-specific deliberate play, organized hockey games and sports other than hockey at each age.

To illustrate the proportion of time participants spent in each of the four sport categories, Figure 2 shows the percentage of time that participants were engaged in each activity for each year during the developmental period. The proportion of time that participants allocated to deliberate practice increased steadily from a low of 30.75% at 9 years of age to a high of 61.31% at 16 years. In contrast, the proportion of time that participants allocated to deliberate play decreased each year from the high of 34.06% at 6 years old to 4.71% at age 16. The proportion of time that participants allocated to organized hockey games showed small increases from 6 (10.48%) to 12 (13.93%) years of age, and then steadily increased from ages 13 (14.85%) to 16 (22.73%). At 12 years old, participants allocated the greatest proportion of time to other sports (34.08%), but the time committed to these activities decreased sharply to its lowest value (11.24%) at 16 years old.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of total sport hours divided by hockey-specific deliberate practice activities, hockey-specific deliberate play, organized hockey games and sports other than hockey at each age.

3.3. Developmental milestones

In the second section of Côté et al.'s (64) interview script, athletes are asked questions related to 31 developmental milestones throughout their hockey careers. Participants in the current study indicated that they were, on average, approximately 3 years old when they first started playing hockey, and 4 years old when they became regularly involved in the sport (see Table 3). At an average of approximately 5 years of age, participants became involved in competition at the recreational level, and the majority were recognized among the best five players at that level. By 9 years old, most participants were playing competitive hockey, and most were recognized as being among the best five players by 10 years of age. Seven of 15 participants had competed at the national level, with five taking part in international competition. The idea of becoming an elite athlete emerged between 3 and 14 years of age, while the decision to become an elite athlete was made between 6 and 15 years of age. Most participants first engaged in regular team training by age 8 and started non-sport specific training by age 12. By the age of 13, most participants had completed their first off-season training camp, and by 14 years old, most acknowledged that they spent all their available leisure time training for hockey. When looking toward their athletic futures, participants thought they would reach their maximum athletic potential, on average, at approximately 23 years of age, and would retire from competitive hockey at an average age of 33 years.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for first emergence of developmental milestones (in years).

Additional quantitative and qualitative information was obtained from the interviews regarding each participant's developmental history. This information was compiled and presented for each year of the survey. Of note, participants engaged in various levels of hockey at younger ages, but all athletes were playing at the highest level of youth hockey by the age of 12 (refer to Table 4). While five participants in the sample played against older players at the ages of 6 and 7, only two were doing so at 8 and 9 years of age, and only one remained in an older age group thereafter (see Table 5). Only one injury was reported before the age of 12, but a total of 22 injuries were reported between the ages of 12 and 16. Three of these injuries were considered chronic, or overuse, while the other 19 were acute in nature (Table 6). Fourteen years was the earliest age identified at which a participant first reported an injury affecting his current performance, despite being able to participate with the condition. This portion of the interview also involved the collection of subjective information regarding the perceived intensity of training, and the available resources at each age (see Table 7). Both steadily increased throughout participants' development from the lowest values at the age of 7, until attaining maximum values at 16 years old. Finally, this section of the interview asked participants to calculate the number of hours spent on hockey activities at different ages. At each age, players understated their involvement in hockey activities, compared to the following section when they analyzed each age more thoroughly.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Number of participants at each level of hockey for each age.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Participants’ ages relative to that of other players in their respective leagues.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Information regarding injuries for hockey related activities for each age.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Descriptive statistics for intensity, resources, hockey activity hours reported, and hockey activities calculated.

3.4. Effort, concentration, and fun

In addition to calculating the number of hours in each of the hockey activity categories, participants were asked to subjectively rate the “physical effort”, “mental concentration”, and “fun” for each hockey activity that they identified throughout their development. Table 8 outlines the descriptive statistics of the responses for organized games. The median value of the participants' responses regarding physical effort increased steadily from 6 years of age (82.5%) to 16 years (100%), with the exception of when the participants were 10 years old, which decreased slightly (81.67%) from when they were 9 years of age (85%). The reported “mental concentration” of participants followed a similar trend, with increasing median values from ages 6 (85%) through 16 (100%). “Organized games” were seen as enjoyable by the participants from 6 to 15 years of age, with a median “fun” rating of 100% during this timeframe. However, the median (80%) decreased considerably once participants turned 16 years of age. Similarly, participants rated “practices” (see Table 9) with increasingly higher levels of “physical effort” and “mental concentration” from ages 6 until 16, with both having maximum (100%) median ratings at 16 years of age. Much like organized games, the median of reported ratings regarding “fun” for practices was 100% from 6 years old until the median decreased substantially to 75% at 16 years of age. Finally, participants rated self-initiated activities (summary of results available by request) as relatively constant throughout their development, with the “physical effort” median ratings reported as being from 80% to 90%. The “mental concentration” reported by participants had a median rating of 70% until the age of 10, when it increased steadily to a median of 95% at 16 years old. Lastly, self-initiated “fun” ratings were highest early in participants' developmental histories, with a median of 100% at 6 years old. The reported “fun” ratings then decreased to a median of 85% at 15 years old and remained the same at 16.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Subjective ratings of effort, concentration, and fun for organized games (subjective ratings on a scale of 0–100).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Subjective ratings of effort, concentration, and fun for practices (subjective ratings on a scale of 0–100).

3.5. Other sports

Information regarding the other sports that the participants engaged in while growing up was compiled and categorized by sport type (available in Table 10). At 10 years old, all participants involved in this study played at least one invasion sport other than hockey, and at 12 years old, 14 of the participants amassed the greatest combined total hours (1,942), mean hours (129.47), and median hours (108) in invasion sports. At 15 years old, participation in invasion sports dropped substantially, with only six athletes accumulating any hours whatsoever. Eleven participants engaged in run scoring/field sports at the ages of 10 and 11, but the greatest combined total of hours from all participants came at 12 years old (800). By 14 years of age, only a minority of participants were involved in run scoring/field sports, and there were only two athletes participating in these sports by the age of 16. Net/wall sports had the highest combined total hours (1,110), maximum hours for a participant (528), and number of participants (12) at age 13, but by age 15 less than half of the participants reported any involvement. Participation in target sports followed a different trend as the highest combined total hours (984), median (32), and maximum number of participants (11) occurred at the age of 16. The highest number of participants in individual sports (i.e., 10) occurred between the ages of 10 and 13, and only six participants reported any involvement by the age of 14. Additionally, at 12 years old, participants accumulated the highest combined number of hours in individual sports (528). Invasion sports accrued the most hours at each age until 15 years, when target sports became the most prominent, according to the total number of hours accumulated. The gain in the relative popularity of net/wall sports is second only to invasion sports at age 13 and were the second most popular type of sport at the ages of 15 and 16.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Descriptive statistics for hours of sports by type and age.

Table 11 outlines specific sport involvement. Soccer was played by all the athletes that were interviewed at some point during their development. From the ages of 6 through 12, at least ten athletes participated in soccer; however, only one participant was involved in soccer at the age of 15. Baseball, basketball, golf, and volleyball shared the second most popular spot, with 12 participants taking part at some point during their development. Golf was popular among five participants at 6 years of age and reached its maximum of 12 participants at 15 years old. At 12 years, 5 sports (i.e., soccer, baseball, basketball, golf, and volleyball) had ten or more participants, while at 14 years of age, only golf had more than seven participants. By the age of 15, no sport other than golf had more than four participants.

TABLE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Specific sport participation by age.

3.6. Supplemental questions related to specialization

To supplement the Côté et al. (64) interview guide, several questions were asked to identify other aspects of sport specialization. Three of these questions were proposed by Jayanthi et al. (62), which helped assign a degree of specialization to participants based on their responses. Participants in this study stated that they chose “hockey as their main sport” by 11.67 years of age, on average, with a median of 13 years old. The mean for “quitting other activities to play hockey” was 12.47, with a median of 12 years old. Finally, participants responded that they “trained for hockey for more than eight months a year” at 12.40 years of age, on average, with a median of 13 years old.

Due to participants reaching each criterion at different ages, the responses from individual participants were analyzed to determine the combined degree of specialization. The reported answers indicate that the participants in this study were “moderately specialized” in hockey at a mean of 12.33, and a median of 13 years of age, and “highly specialized” at a mean of 13.40, and a median of 14 years. However, upon calculating the duration of annual training via answers to the “hockey activity” portion of the interview, it appears that participants underestimated their annual commitment to hockey. Based on these calculations, the mean age at which training occurred for more than eight months in a given year was 7.8 years, with a median of 7 years. These calculations indicate that the mean age at which the participants became highly specialized in hockey was slightly younger at 12.87 years of age (vs. reported mean age of 13.40 years), although the median age remains at 14 years (see Table 12).

TABLE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Table 12. Answers to questions regarding the degree of specialization (age in years).

Finally, four questions regarding issues of specialization were asked of each participant. Eight participants were instructed to not participate in other sports during the hockey season at some point during their development. In five of these cases, coaches made the requests independently, while both coaches and parents made the requests in the remaining cases. In each instance, participants were prevented from participating in other sports to prevent potential injuries. Only two participants were instructed not to play other sports during the off-season, and both the parents and coaches made these requests, again, to prevent injuries from occurring. Only one participant had ever attended a hockey academy, and he attended this academy for a total of one year. Of the 15 athletes interviewed for the study, 11 had played for more than one minor hockey association, with nine participants having relocated to play against higher levels of competition; one to avoid conflict with a coach and one was cut from his previous team.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to describe and analyze the developmental histories of a group of current and former OHL players. Thus, the results concentrate on the years prior to participants’ involvement in this league, which may commence as young as 16 years old, with the rare instance of “exceptional status” being granted to players who are 15 years old (75). The median of 139 OHL games played shows that the majority of participants had been involved in more than two complete seasons in the League at the time of their interviews. These participants created a homogeneous sample of skilled hockey players by having maintained OHL status, thereby showing a high level of proficiency, which is a common determinant of expert status (76). As hypothesized, participants reported involvement in hockey competition prior to the age suggested by Hockey Canada and showed high levels of commitment to hockey as young as 9 years old. However, the participants' backgrounds also featured a diverse sampling stage early in their athletic careers.

4.1. Hockey-specific deliberate play

Collectively, participants of the current study spent approximately 22% of their total sports hours in the sampling stage (age 6–13) engaged in deliberate play activities and only 8% during the specialization stage (age 13–15). A large decrease in the combined total of deliberate play hours was observed between the ages of 9 and 10, as well as between 14 and 15. While hockey-specific comparisons are limited in the literature, Soberlak (77)7 reported 55% and 30% of hours spent in deliberate play activities during sampling and specialization stages, respectively for CHL players (n = 4). This may suggest that deliberate play hours were on the lower end in this particular sample of OHL players. Using the same criteria as Soberlak and Côté (25), participants of the current study may have been considered to be specialized as early as 10 years old and invested by 15 years of age. These ages are younger than those suggested by both Hockey Canada (43) and the DMSP (41).

The reduction of sport-specific play activities noted in this study may negatively affect some elements of development that assist with improving performance. Roca and colleagues (78) found that 21.8% of the variance found on a soccer decision-making task was accounted for by participants' accumulated childhood soccer-specific play hours. Moreover, Ford et al. (79) found that soccer-specific play was the only developmental activity that differentiated professionals from less successful players. In studies of sport-specific creativity, hours spent in unstructured play differentiated the “most creative” athletes from the “least creative” within team ball sports (80). An 18-week deliberate-play, basketball intervention suggested similar benefits with respect to tactical creativity and tactical intelligence (81). Free play has also been tied to the development of intrinsic motivation for the activity (82, 83); although this view is not universally accepted [e.g., (84)].

4.2. Hockey-specific deliberate practice

While the difference between practice and play may not always be clear (85), deliberate practice in the current study included any activity (e.g., self-initiated practice, team practice, individual coaching) that was done to improve performance (64). The increase in accumulated hours of deliberate practice reported from 9 to 10 years of age may be explained by the increase in the number of players competing at the highest level of youth hockey (i.e., AAA). The difference in accumulated hours of deliberate practice between 12 and 13 years old may be explained by the advancement of participants from the “peewee” to “bantam” age categories8. Similarly, between the ages of 15 and 16, participants were transitioning to the OHL, where time commitments to the sport are much greater. Corresponding with this increased investment was the reduction of fun reported by participants, such that by the age of 16 hockey was not as enjoyable as in previous years. For comparison purposes, only 10% of the hours collected by the junior hockey players in the Soberlak and Côté (25) study were dedicated to deliberate practice in the sampling stage (age 6–13), whereas 35% of all sports hours were spent on deliberate practice in the current sample. Deliberate practice during the specializing stage (age 13–15) accounted for 18% of the Soberlak and Côté (25) participants' sport hours, and 50% of the hours reported by the participants of this study.

Participants appeared to underestimate their commitment to hockey when asked to summarize the time spent in hockey-related activities. When the total number of hours spent in activities specific to hockey were calculated manually by the research team, there were more than double the number of hours reported. While the interview script used in this study allowed for the distinction between deliberate play and deliberate practice, Côté et al. (85) suggest that a continuum exists between the two forms of developmental activities. Thus, the difference between the two activities might not be easily recognized. The general trend of participants' increasing amounts of deliberate practice at the expense of play activities found in this study is consistent with previous research (86, 87). However, while some studies have found that higher amounts of accumulated sport-specific practice results in better performance [e.g., (72, 76)], others find that expert status is not explained by accumulated practice hours alone [e.g., (88, 89)]. One explanation for the early accumulation of deliberate practice hours in youth sports is that some coaches pressure young athletes into training at higher levels than needed due to an overemphasis on winning (90). Valuing winning at the expense of player development creates an adult-centric environment that rationalizes specialization (91).

4.3. Organized hockey games

The hours that participants in the current study spent competing in organized hockey games increased throughout their developmental years in similar proportions to that found by Soberlak and Côté (25). This may suggest that the proportion of time spent competing in organized games has not changed substantially for athletes of this level since Hockey Canada implemented changes to its grassroots programs after the Open Ice Summit. However, the majority of participants in the current study started playing hockey by the time they were 3 years old, began regular involvement by 4 years old, and were competing in an organized league when they were 5 years of age. This is considerably younger than the hockey players in the Wall and Côté (92) study where, on average, participants started playing after they were 5 years old, and initiated competition when they were 6 years of age. The younger ages found in the current study suggest that Canadian hockey players are starting to engage in organized hockey at younger ages than has been documented in previous research, although further investigations are warranted to draw conclusions.

4.4. Other sports

The total number of accumulated hours that participants played sports other than hockey increased until the age of 12 and then decreased every year thereafter. The number of sports played other than hockey increased from an average of 2.0 at the age of 6, to a maximum average of 5.6 at 12 years old, and then decreased each year to 2.3 at 15 years old. Comparatively, Soberlak (77) found that the junior hockey players in his study played a similar number of sports (on average, 3 sports between ages 6–8 and 6 sports between 9 and 12) but did not decrease the amount of time spent playing other sports until the age of 14 (25). Further, a lower proportion of the sampling stage was spent participating in sports other than hockey relative to the specialization stage (77). This suggests participants in the current study may have spent more time playing sports other than hockey early in their development, but ceased playing these other sports sooner than previous research on junior ice hockey players has indicated. However, the participants of the current study began to eliminate sports other than hockey at an age that is consistent with Hockey Canada's (43) LTAD program, and the DMSP's (41) recommendations regarding participating in other sports.

Consistent with findings in this study, previous work has shown that expert athletes in other sports tend to participate in several sports until the age of 12, at which point they narrow their focus [i.e., (66, 91, 93)]. Likewise, athletes who played a variety of sports at the age of 12 have better results in several measures of athletic performance (94). Conversely, Ford et al. (79) found no difference in the number of hours spent playing other sports between elite and non-elite soccer players. While the authors did not find an advantage in playing different sports early in one's development, they also found that doing so did not hinder the chances of attaining elite status.

In addition to estimating the number of hours played in sport other than hockey, the type and classification of sport was also collected. The 15 participants in the current study indicated that they competed in a combined 16 sports, other than hockey, from all five sport categories, displaying the diverse developmental pathways noted by other authors [e.g., (95)]. While the extent to which skills may transfer across sports is not universally accepted, there is evidence to suggest that similar sports have elements of positive skill or tactical transfer [e.g., (91, 9698)]. Consistent with previous research, the number of hours that participants of the current study engaged in invasion sports is greater than the hours accumulated in other categories (72). The similarity of tactics employed in invasion sports may increase the likelihood for transfer because athletes can “chunk” information, allowing them to broaden their focus and process more stimuli (96, 98, 99). Therefore, participants may have enhanced hockey performance due to their participation in these sports by a mechanism known as lateral, or near, transfer (100), where the training outcomes in one sport can be used in other sports (101).

4.5. Supplemental questions related to specialization

In the current study, most participants considered hockey their main sport by the age of 13, quit other activities for hockey by the age of 12, and reported training more than eight months per year for hockey at 13 years old. Due to athletes reaching each criterion at different ages, the degree of specialization was calculated individually, and then analyzed. Accordingly, most of the participants in the current study were considered “moderately specialized” at 13 years old, and “highly specialized” at 14 years of age (based on median values). Using the detailed information derived from the Côté et al. (64) interview script, the age at which participants in the current study trained for eight or more months per year could be empirically calculated. Based on these calculations, most participants in the current study had trained for eight or more months per year by 7 years of age, as opposed to their general estimation of 13 years old. Moreover, the calculated age at which participants trained for more than eight months per year indicates that they were moderately specialized at 12 years of age, which is one year earlier than the 13-year-old average the participants reported in their responses to the supplemental questions. In reference to the age divisions for minor hockey in Canada, this one-year disparity is the difference between a player competing at the “bantam” vs. “peewee” level (102). The calculated median age of participants being categorized as highly specialized was 14 years, which is consistent with the average reported in their answers to the supplemental questions. However, most of the participants answered that they had the idea to become an elite athlete by 8 years old and decided to become one by 12 years of age. These qualitative indications suggest that participants may have been highly specialized prior to 14 years of age.

Research has shown that being classified as highly specialized is an independent risk factor for reporting an injury, regardless of training volume and age (50, 62, 103). Jayanthi and colleagues (62) also stated that athletes who exceed a 2:1 ratio of organized training (i.e., deliberate practice and organized games) to free play (i.e., deliberate play) were more likely to develop an injury. Accordingly, this ratio was achieved by most of the participants of the current study by 12 years of age, which corresponds to when the first participant reported playing through an injury. Empirically calculating the age at which these participants became highly specialized may provide insight into the injuries that they reported starting in that year.

It has been reported that some youth coaches prohibit athletes from participating in sports other than their primary sport (12). In all eight instances of participants in this study being instructed to not play other sports, the rationale was to prevent injuries that may inhibit their performance in hockey. However, the recommendation may be ill-advised as specialization has been closely associated with overuse injuries, and athletes may suffer acute injuries at any time in their main sport as well (62, 93, 104). Youth sport organizations also place pressure on parents and coaches to promote early specialization by placing value in the result of competition, and devaluing the developmental process (91). Pressure is placed on coaches to win by these organizations by advertising national rankings as young as 4 years old (105, 106). Likewise, parents feel pressured to push specialization as third-party entities attempt to forecast their children's professional outlook as early as 7 years old (105).

Only one of the participants in the current study attended a hockey academy, and only for a single year. This participant attended an academy at the age of 16, prior to attaining his position on an OHL team. Despite the proliferation of hockey academies throughout Canada, and specifically Ontario (60), they did not appear to have a major influence on the development of the OHL players in the current study. While hockey academies are becoming increasingly popular among minor hockey players, the four academies located within the region selected for the inclusion criteria did not begin operating until approximately 2014 (60, 107, 108). However, all participants in the current study reported using private instructors, such as skating and goalie coaches, as an element of their deliberate practice. Participants chose instructors for these sessions based on the specific skill(s) they were trying to enhance. Most participants in the current study began utilizing the services of individual coaches by 10 years old, while four participants started as early as 6 years of age. This means that the participants’ parents or guardians were responsible for paying for up to 10 years of individual coaching in an attempt to excel above their peers.

4.6. Strengths/limitations and future directions

The retrospective recall methodology used in this study provided invaluable insight into the developmental histories of these participants. However, most participants began hockey activities by 3 years of age and started competition at 5 years of age. The interview script prescribed that information be collected beginning at the age of 6 years; thus, future research examining the developmental histories of elite Canadian junior hockey players should account for the hours collected prior to 6 years of age to give a more accurate portrayal of athletes' sport involvement. Additionally, the retrospective answers that participants gave to the questions in Jayanthi et al.'s (62) three-point scale did not always substantiate the answers to similar questions in the Côté et al. (64) script. While the scale has demonstrated to be a valid tool in the study of specialization, it may not provide reliable information when used retrospectively. Future examinations of participant histories should aim to expand on these limitations, along with inclusion of larger samples to athletes (i.e., a notable limitation of previous work and the current study). Additional detailed analyses using alternative developmental models would also be beneficial. Rees et al. (109) concluded that both early specialization and early diversification (i.e., sampling and play) can lead to athletic success. Thus, the DMSP and LTAD models may have inherent limitations that require further exploration. Finally, while the Côté et al.'s (64) interview methodology is believed to provide valid and reliable information, participant memory recall is a potential limitation of retrospective studies of this nature.

Notably, participants consistently reported having enjoyed all their hockey pursuits despite an increase in structured activities. The fun that they experienced at a young age presumably allowed them to commit to the high levels of physical effort and concentration needed to develop into elite junior hockey players. Parents of children who aspire to play hockey at a high level should be aware that these participants consistently played sports other than hockey, and enjoyed their time spent in hockey activities. Therefore, finding ways to promote fun at a young age, regardless of the type of activity, appears to be instrumental in the development of elite junior hockey players.

4.7. Conclusion

Despite recommendations for delayed sport specialization among children, the early specialization path remains a popular trajectory among coaches, parents, and athletes alike in their pursuit of extrinsic rewards. This study provides detailed developmental histories of a group of elite junior hockey players from the OHL. The findings suggest the developmental pathways of elite junior hockey players may have changed since Hockey Canada altered its grassroots programs. However, despite diverse sampling activities early in their athletic careers, athletes reported evidence of specialization in hockey at young ages.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not available on ethical grounds as they contain personal information.

Ethics statement

The study involving humans was cleared by the University of Windsor Research Ethics Board. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

WG designed the analysis, collected the data, and drafted the initial manuscript under the supervision of JD and SH. KS updated the manuscript and prepared the data/document for publication. All authors edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

The contents of this manuscript first appeared in the first author's thesis (110). The authors would like to thank the athletes who volunteered their time to participate in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1From the early stages, a variety of sports have been studied to ascertain the relevance of various developmental activities, further reinforcing the interest and cultural importance placed on athlete achievement. Examples include tennis (20), field hockey (21), golf (22), soccer (23), volleyball (24), ice hockey (25), cricket (26), and gymnastics (27).

2Femoroacetabular impingement is characterized by ‘cam’ and ‘pincer’ bony abnormalities to the femoral neck or acetabular rim respectively (52).

3A copy of the interview script may be obtained from the corresponding author.

4‘Moderately specialized’ at a given age is indicated by two positive responses to any of the three questions. ‘Highly specialized’ is assigned for affirmative responses to all three questions at a particular age. Refer to Jayanthi et al. (62) for further discussion.

5‘Sport’ refers to all sports, including hockey.

6Examples of ‘artistic’ activities include music, dance, and drawing.

7Soberlak and Côté (25) refers to the published work that was derived from the more detailed Soberlak (77) Master's thesis. Both are referred to in the current manuscript, depending on where the information was provided.

8Hockey Canada modified the names used to identify developmental age groups for the 2022-2023 season. ‘Peewee’ and ‘bantam’ are now referred to as ‘U13’ and ‘U15’, respectively. This change occurred after the current participants progressed through the stages of development; thus, the former names are used throughout the manuscript.

References

1. Bell DR, Snedden TR, Biese KM, Nelson E, Watson AM, Brooks A, et al. Consensus definition of sport specialization in youth athletes using a delphi approach. J Athl Train. (2021) 56(11):1239–51. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-0725.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Brenner JS. Sports specialization and intensive training in young athletes. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:3. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ferguson B, Stern PJ. A case of early sports specialization in an adolescent athlete. J Can Chiropr Assoc. (2014) 58(4):377–83.25550662

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

4. Jayanthi NA, Post EG, Laury TC, Fabricant PD. Health consequences of youth sport specialization. J Athl Train. (2019) 54(10):1040–9. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-380-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mosher A, Fraser-Thomas J, Baker J. What defines early specialization: a systematic review of literature. Front Sports Act Living. (2020) 2:596229. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2020.596229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hornig M, Aust F, Güllich A. Practice and play in the development of German top-level professional football players. Eur J Sport Sci. (2016) 16(1):96–105. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2014.982204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Mosher A, Till K, Fraser-Thomas J, Baker J. Revisiting early sport specialization: what’s the problem? Sports Health. (2022) 14(1):13–9. doi: 10.1177/19417381211049773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. The Associated Press. The age of single-sport athletes endures despite detractors’ suspicions. New York, NY: The New York Times (2016). Available at: http://www.nytimes.com/2016/05/01/sports/the-age-of-single-sport-athletesendures-despite-detractors-suspicions.html?_r=2

9. Waldron S, DeFreese JD, Register-Mihalik J, Pietrosimone B, Barczak N. The costs and benefits of early sport specialization: a critical review of literature. Quest. (2020) 72(1):1–18. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2019.1580205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Myer GD, Jayanthi N, DiFiori JP, Faigenbaum AD, Kiefer AW, Logerstedt D, et al. Sports specialization, part II: alternative solutions to early sport specialization in youth athletes. Sports Health. (2015a) 8(1):65–73. doi: 10.1177/1941738115614811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Feeley BT, Agel J, LaPrade RF. When is it too early for single sport specialization? Am J Sports Med. (2016) 44(1):234–41. doi: 10.1177/0363546515576899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Coakley J. The “logic” of specialization: using children for adult purposes. J Phys Educ Recreat Dance. (2010) 81(8):16–25. doi: 10.1080/07303084.2010.10598520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Côté J, Hancock DJ. Evidence-based policies for youth sport programmes. Int J Sport Policy Politics. (2016) 8(1):51–65. doi: 10.1080/19406940.2014.919338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Jayanthi N, Pinkham C, Dugas L, Patrick B, LaBella C. Sports specialization in young athletes: evidence-based recommendations. Sports Health. (2012) 5(3):251–7. doi: 10.1177/1941738112464626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lloyd RS, Faigenbaum D, Howard R, De Ste Croix MB, Williams CA. Long-term athletic development, part 2: barriers to success and potential solutions. J Strength Cond Res. (2012) 29(5):1451–64. doi: 10.1519/01.JSC.0000465424.75389.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Buckley PS, Bishop M, Kane P, Ciccotti MC, Selverian S, Exume D, et al. Early single-sport specialization: a survey of 3,090 high school, collegiate, and professional athletes. Orthop J Sports Med. (2017) 5(7):2325967117703944. doi: 10.1177/2325967117703944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gonçalves CE, Rama LM, Figueiredo AB. Talent identification and specialization in sport: an overview of some unanswered questions. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. (2012) 7:390–3. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.7.4.390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Washburn JN. Sport as a soviet tool. Foreign Aff. (1956) 34(3):490–9. doi: 10.2307/20031180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Malina RM. Early sport specialization: roots, effectiveness, risks. Curr Sports Med Rep. (2010) 9(6):364–71. doi: 10.1249/JSR.0b013e3181fe3166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Carlson R. The socialization of elite tennis players in Sweden: an analysis of the players’ backgrounds and development. Sociol Sport J. (1988) 5:241–56. doi: 10.1123/ssj.5.3.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Güllich A, Emrich E. Considering long-term sustainability in the development of world class success. Eur J Sport Sci. (2014) 14(S1):S383–97. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2012.706320

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hayman R, Polman R, Taylor J, Hemmings B, Borkoles E. Development of adolescent golfers. Talent Dev Excell. (2011) 3(2):249–61.

Google Scholar

23. Livingston J, Schmidt C, Lehman S. Competitive club soccer: parents’ assessments of children’s early and later sport specialization. J Sport Behav. (2016) 39(3):301–16.

Google Scholar

24. Coutinho P, Mesquita I, Fonseca AM, Côté J. Expertise development in volleyball: the role of early activities and players’ age and height. Kinesiology. (2015) 47(2):215–25. https://hrcak.srce.hr/150549

Google Scholar

25. Soberlak P, Côté J. The developmental activities of elite ice hockey players. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2003) 15:41–9. doi: 10.1080/10413200390180053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Weissensteiner J, Abernethy B, Farrow D. Towards the development of a conceptual model of expertise in cricket batting: a grounded theory approach. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2009) 21:276–92. doi: 10.1080/10413200903018675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Law M, Côté J, Ericsson KA. Characteristics of expert development in rhythmic gymnastics: a retrospective study. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2007) 5:82–103. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2008.9671814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kliethermes SA, Marshall SW, LaBella CR, Watson AM, Brenner JS, Nagle KB, et al. Consensus statement. Defining a research agenda for youth sport specialization in the USA: the AMSSM youth early sport specialization summer. Br J Sports Med. (2021) 55(3):135–43. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2020-102699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Schwarb AW. After school specialized: Studies discourage specialization for young athletes. Indianapolis, IN: NCAA Champion Magazine (2016). Available at: http://www.ncaa.org/champion/after-school-specialized

30. Malina RM, Eisenmann JC, Cumming SP, Ribeiro B, Aroso J. Maturity-associated variation in the growth and functional capacities of youth football (soccer) players 13–15 years. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2004) 91:555–62. doi: 10.1007/s00421-003-0995-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Güllich A, Macnamara BN, Hambrick DZ. What makes a champion? Early multidisciplinary practice, not early specialization, predicts world-class performance. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2022) 17(1):6–29. doi: 10.1177/1745691620974772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Aspen Institute (2016). Project Play. State of play 2016: Trends and developments. Available at: http://www.aspenprojectplay.org/sites/default/files/StateofPlay_2016_FINAL.pdf

33. Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev. (1993) 100(3):363–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Simon HA, Chase WG. Skill in chess: experiments with chess-playing tasks and computer simulation of skilled performance throw light on some human perceptual and memory processes. Am Sci. (1973) 61(4):394–403. https://www.jstor.org/stable/27843878

Google Scholar

35. Baker J, Côté J, Abernethy B. Sport-specific practice and the development of expert decision-making in team ball sports. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2003) 15:12–25. doi: 10.1080/10413200390180035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Helsen WF, Starkes JL, Hodges NJ. Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (1998) 20:12–34. doi: 10.1123/jsep.20.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Williams MA, Ford PR. Expertise and expert performance in sport. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 1(1):4–18. doi: 10.1080/17509840701836867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Baker J, Young B. 20 Years later: deliberate practice and the development of expertise in sport. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. (2014) 7(1):135–57. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2014.896024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Baker J, Young BW, Tedesqui RA, McCardle L. New perspectives on deliberate practice and the development of sport expertise. In: Tenenbaum G, Eklund B, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. 4th ed. London: Wiley (2020). p. 556–77.

40. Hambrick DZ, Oswald FL, Altmann EM, Meinz EJ, Gobet F, Campitelli G. Deliberate practice: is that all it takes to become an expert? Intelligence. (2014) 45:34–45. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Côté J. The influence of the family in the development of talent in sport. Sport Psychol. (1999) 13:395–417. doi: 10.1123/tsp.13.4.395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Balyi I, Way R, Higgs C. Long-term athlete development. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics (2013).

43. Hockey Canada. (2013). Hockey Canada long term player development plan: hockey for life, hockey for excellence. Available at: https://az184419.vo.msecnd.net/hockey-canada/Hockey-Programs/Coaching/LTPD/Downloads/LTPD_manual_may_2013_e.pdf

44. Bergeron MF, Mountjoy M, Armstrong N, Chia M, Côté J, Emery CA, et al. International Olympic committee consensus statement on youth athletic development. Br J Sports Med. (2015) 49:843–51. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-094962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Laprade RF, Agel J, Baker J, Brenner JS, Cordasco FA, Côté J, et al. AOSSM Early sport specialization consensus statement. Orthop J Sports Med. (2016) 4(4):1–8. doi: 10.1177/2325967116644241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Barker D, Barker-Ruchti N, Rynne S, Lee J. “Just do a little more”: examining expertise in high performance sport from a sociocultural learning perspective. Reflective Pract. (2014) 15(1):92–105. doi: 10.1080/14623943.2013.868797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Williams AM, Ward P, Bell-Walker J, Ford PR. Perceptual-cognitive expertise, practice history profiles and recall performance in soccer. Br J Psychol. (2011) 103:292–411. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02081.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Barreiros A, Côté J, Fonseca AM. From early to adult sport success: analysing athletes’ progression in national squads. Eur J Sport Sci. (2014) 14(S1):S178–82. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2012.671368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Moesch K, Elbe AM, Hauge ML, Wikman JM. Late specialization: the key to success in centimeters, grams, or seconds (cgs) sports. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2011) 21:e282–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01280.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bell DR, Post EG, Trigsted SM, Hetzel S, McGuine TA, Brooks MA. Prevalence of sport specialization in high school athletics: a 1-year observational study. Am J Sports Med. (2016) 44(6):1469–74. doi: 10.1177/0363546516629943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Myer GD, Jayanthi N, DiFiori JP, Faigenbaum AD, Kiefer AW, Logerstedt D, et al. Sport specialization, part I: does early sports specialization increase negative outcomes and reduce the opportunity for success in young athletes? Sports Health. (2015b) 7(5):437–42. doi: 10.1177/194173815598747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Philippon MJ, Ho CP, Briggs KK, Stull J, LaPrade RF. Prevalence of increased alpha angles as a measure of cam-type femoroacetabular impingement in youth ice hockey players. Am J Sports Med. (2013) 41(6):1357–62. doi: 10.1177/0363546513483448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Stull JD, Philippon MJ, LaPrade RF. “At-risk” positioning and hip biomechanics of the peewee ice hockey sprint start. Am J Sports Med. (2011) 39(1):29S–35S. doi: 10.1177/0363546511414012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. McFadden T, Bean C, Fortier M, Post C. Investigating the influence of youth hockey specialization on psychological needs (dis) satisfaction, mental health, and mental illness. Cogent Psychol. (2016) 3:1–16. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2016.1157975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Russell WD. The relationship between youth sport specialization, reasons for participation, and youth sport participation motivations: a retrospective study. J Sport Behav. (2014) 37(3):286–305.

Google Scholar

56. About the CHL. (n.d.). Available at: http://chl.ca/aboutthechl

57. National Hockey League. (2015). NHL draft historical draft summary 1969–2014. Available at: http://www.nhl.com/ice/page.htm?id=31887

58. Western Hockey League. (n.d.). WHL bantam draft. Available at: http://whl.ca/whlbantam-draft

59. Robertson-Wilson J. E. (2002). The role of parental influences and activity involvement in elite and novice hockey players [Master’s thesis]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. (MQ73081).

60. Hockey Canada. (2005). Molson open ice summit on player development. Five years in the making: a report on the open ice summit initiatives. Available at: http://www.hockeycanada.ca/e/events/openice/index.html

61. Traikos M. Buying into the dream: hockey academies a growing trend, with costs surpassing $50,000 a year. Toronto, ON: The National Post (2015). Available at: http://www.nationalpost.com/m/wp/sports/nhl/blog.html?b=news.nationalpost.com/sports/nhl/buying-into-the-dream-hockey-academies-a-growing-trend-withcosts-surpassing-50000

62. Jayanthi NA, LaBella CR, Fischer D, Pasculka J, Dugas LR. Sports-specialized intensive training and the risk of injury in young athletes: a clinical case-control study. Am J Sports Med. (2015) 43(4):794–801. doi: 10.1177/0363546514567298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Patton MQ. Two decades of developments in qualitative inquiry: a personal, experiential perspective. Qual Soc Work. (2002) 1(3):261–83. doi: 10.1177/1473325002001003636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Côté J, Ericsson KA, Law MP. Tracing the development of athletes using retrospective interview methods: a proposed interview and validation procedure for reported information. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2005) 17:1–19. doi: 10.1080/10413200590907531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Baker J, Côté J, Deakin J. Expertise in ultra-endurance triathletes early sport involvement training structure, and the theory of deliberate practice. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2005) 17:64–78. doi: 10.1080/10413200590907577

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Barreiros A, Côté J, Fonseca AM. Training and psychosocial patterns during the early development of Portuguese national team athletes. High Abil Stud. (2013) 24(1):49–61. doi: 10.1080/13598139.2013.780965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Ford PR, Carling C, Garces M, Marques M, Miguel C, Farant A, et al. The developmental activities of elite soccer players aged under-16 from Brazil, England, France, Ghana, Mexico, Portugal, and Sweden. J Sports Sci. (2012) 30(15):1653–63. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.701762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Rothwell M, Stone JA, Davids K, Wright C. Development of expertise in elite and sub-elite British rugby league players: a comparison of practice experiences. Eur J Sport Sci. (2017) 17(10):1252–60. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2017.1380708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Johnson MB, Tenenbaum G, Edmonds WA, Castillo Y. A comparison of the developmental experiences of elite and sub-elite swimmers: similar developmental histories can lead to differences in performance level. Sport Educ Soc. (2008) 13(4):453–75. doi: 10.1080/13573320802445108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ford PR, Low J, McRobert AP, Williams AM. Developmental activities that contribute to high or low performance by elite cricket batters when recognizing type of delivery from bowlers’ advanced postural cues. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2010) 32:638–54. doi: 10.1123/jsep.32.5.638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ford PR, Williams AM. The effect of participation in Gaelic football on the development of Irish professional soccer players. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 30:709–22. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.6.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Berry J, Abernethy B, Côté J. The contribution of structured activity and deliberate play to the development of expert perceptual and decision-making skill. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 30:685–708. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.6.685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Fraser-Thomas J, Côté J, Deakin J. Examining adolescent sport dropout and prolonged engagement from a development perspective. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2008) 20:318–33. doi: 10.1080/10413200802163549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Thorpe R, Bunker D, Almond L. Rethinking games teaching. Leicestershire, UK: Loughborough University Press (1986).

75. RSEN. (2013). Canadian junior hockey (CHL) 101. Available at: http://rsenreport.com/canadian-junior-hockey-chl-101/

76. Coutinho P, Mesquita I, Fonseca AM. Talent development in sport: a critical review of pathways to expert performance. Int J Sports Sci Coach. (2016) 11(2):279–93. doi: 10.1177/1747954116637499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Soberlak P. A retrospective analysis of the developmental and motivation of professional ice hockey players [Master’s thesis]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing (2001). (Thesis No. MQ55932).

78. Roca A, Williams AM, Ford PR. Developmental activities and the acquisition of superior anticipation and decision making in soccer. J Sports Sci. (2012) 30(15):1643–52. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.701761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Ford PR, Ward P, Hodges NJ, Williams AM. The role of deliberate practice and play in career progression in sport: the early engagement hypothesis. High Abil Stud. (2009) 20(1):65–75. doi: 10.1080/13598130902860721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Memmert D, Baker J, Bertsch C. Play and practice in the development of sport-specific creatively in team ball sports. High Abil Stud. (2010) 21(1):3–18. doi: 10.1080/13598139.2010.488083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Greco P, Memmert D, Morales JCP. The effect of deliberate play on tactical performance in basketball. Percept Mot Skills. (2010) 110(3):849–56. doi: 10.2466/PMS.110.3.849-856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Baker J, Cobley S. Outliers, talent codes, and myths. In: Farrow D, Baker J, MacMahon C, editors. Developing sport expertise: researchers and coaches put theory into practice. London: Routledge (2013). p. 13–29.

83. Güllich A, Kovar P, Zart S, Reimann A. Sport activities differentiating match-play improvement in elite youth footballers—a 2-year longitudinal study. J Sports Sci. (2017) 35(3):207–15. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1161206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hendry DT, Crocker PR, Hodges NJ. Practice and plays as determinants of self-determined motivation in youth soccer players. J Sports Sci. (2014) 32(11):1091–9. doi: 10.1080/02640414.1014.880792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Côté J, Erickson K, Abernethy B. Play and practice during childhood. In: Côté J, Lidor R, editors. Conditions of children’s talent development in sport. Morgantown: Fitness Information Technology (2013). p. 9–20.

86. Côté J, Baker J, Abernethy B. Practice and play in the development of sport expertise. In: Eklund R, Tenenbaum G, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. Hoboken: Wiley (2007). p. 184–202.

87. Huttermann S, Memmert D, Baker J. Understanding the microstructure of practice: training differences between various age classes, expertise levels and sports. Talent Dev Excell. (2014) 6(1):17–29.

Google Scholar

88. Moesch K, Hauge ML, Wikman JM, Elbe AM. Making it to the top in team sports: start later, intensify, and be determined! Talent Dev Excell. (2013) 5(2):85–100.

Google Scholar

89. Phillips E, Davids K, Renshaw I, Portis M. Expert performance in sport and the dynamics of talent development. Sports Med. (2010) 40(4):271–83. doi: 10.2165/11319430-000000000-00000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Kutz M, Secrest M. Contributing factors to overtraining in the adolescent multi-season/sport athlete. Strength Cond J. (2009) 31(3):37–42. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0b013e3181a1008c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Vealey RS, Chase MA. Best practice for youth sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics (2016).

92. Wall M, Côté J. Developmental activities that lead to dropout and investment in sport. Phys Educ Sport Pedagogy. (2007) 12(1):77–87. doi: 10.1080/17408980601060358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Post EG, Thein-Nissenbaum JM, Stiffler MR, Brooks MA, Bell DR. High school sport specialization patterns of current division I athletes. Sports Health. (2017) 9(2):148–53. doi: 10.1177/1941738116675455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Fransen J, Pion J, Vandendriessche J, Vandorpe B, Vaeyens R, Lenoir M, et al. Differences in physical fitness and gross motor coordination in boys aged 6-12 years specializing in one versus sampling more than one sport. J Sports Sci. (2012) 30(4):379–86. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2011.642808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Huxley DJ, O’Connor D, Larkin P. The pathway to the top: key factors and influences in the development of Australian Olympic and world championship track and field athletes. Int J Sports Sci Coach. (2017) 12(2):264–75. doi: 10.1177/1747954117694738

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Chow JY, Davids K, Button C, Renshaw I. Nonlinear pedagogy in skill acquisition: an introduction. New York, NY: Routledge (2016).

97. Gorman AD. Methods for measuring recall and recognition in sport experts. In: Baker J, Farrow D, editors. Routledge handbook of sport expertise. London: Routledge (2015). p. 198–208.

98. Santos S, Mateus N, Sampaio J, Leite N. Do previous sports experiences influence the effect of an enrichment programme in basketball skills. J Sports Sci. (2017) 35(17):1759–67. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1236206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Tsal Y, Lavie N. Location dominance in attending to color and shape. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. (1993) 19(1):131–9. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Rosalie S, Müller S. A model for the transfer of perceptual-motor skill learning in human behaviors. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2012) 83(3):413–21. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2012.10599876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Issurin VB. Training transfer: scientific background and insights for practical application. Sports Med. (2013) 43:675–94. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0049-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. OMHA. (2017). Age reference chart 2017–18. Available at: http://www.omha.net/page/show/940864-age-reference-chart

103. Jayanthi NA, Gugas LR. The risks of sport specialization in the adolescent female athlete. Strength Cond J. (2017) 39(2):20–6. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Axelrod B. (2016). The failed college football recruitment of LeBron James. Bleacher report. Available at: http://bleacherreport.com/articles/2609589-the-failed-college-football-recruitment-of-lebron-james

105. Gregory S. (2017). How kids’ sports became a $15 billion industry. New York, NY: Time. Available at: http://time.com/4913687/how-kids-sports-became-15-billionindustry/?utm_campaign=time&utm_source=twitter.com&utm_medium=social&xid=time_socialflow_twitter

106. University of Florida Sport Policy & Research Collaborative (UFSPRC). Research brief: what does the science say about athletic development in children? Gainesville, FL: The Aspen Institute’s Project Play (2013). Available at: https://assets.aspeninstitute.org/content/uploads/2016/06/Project-play-september-2013-roundtable-research-brief.pdf

107. Hockey Canada Skills Academies. (n.d.). Hockey Canada skills academies. Available at: https://az184419.vo.msecnd.net/hockey-canada/Hockey-Programs/Schools/Skills-Academy/Downloads/Contacts/hcsa_contacts_ont.pdf

108. Thorne L. F. J. Brennan cèntre of excellence launches hockey Canada skills academy. Windsor, ON: Windsorite Dot Ca News (2014). Available at: http://windsorite.ca/2014/09/f-j-brennan-centre-of-excellence-launches-hockeycanada-skills-academy/

109. Rees T, Hardy L, Güllich A, Abernethy B, Côté J, Woodman T, et al. The great British medalists project: a review of current knowledge on the development of the world’s best sporting talent. Sports Medicine. (2016) 46:1041–58. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0476-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Garland WJ. Developmental activities of Ontario hockey league players [Thesis]. Windsor, ON, Canada: University of Windsor (2017).

Keywords: early specialization, athlete development, deliberate practice, deliberate play, long-term athlete development, sport commitment, junior hockey

Citation: Garland WJ, Smith KL, Dixon JC and Horton S (2023) Developmental activities of elite junior hockey players: an analysis of early sport specialization. Front. Sports Act. Living 5:1253007. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2023.1253007

Received: 4 July 2023; Accepted: 9 October 2023;
Published: 31 October 2023.

Edited by:

Jeppe Foged Vigh-Larsen, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Carlos Eduardo Gonçalves, University of Coimbra, Portugal
Julie Agek, University of Washington, United States

© 2023 Garland, Smith, Dixon and Horton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: K. L. Smith smith43@uwindsor.ca

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior/last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.