Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 21 April 2015
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Learning to see (better): improving visual deficits with perceptual learning View all 10 articles

Editorial: Improving visual deficits with perceptual learning

  • 1Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Human Inspired Technologies Research Centre – HIT, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 3Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université de Toulouse-UPS, Toulouse, France
  • 4Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Toulouse, France

The capability of improving performance on visual tasks with practice has been a matter of intense investigation during the last 40 years (Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980; Sagi, 2011). This phenomenon, called perceptual learning, has been proven to occur with virtually any visual skill or stimulus characteristic (Fahle and Poggio, 2002), and to be long-lasting, thus involving neural plasticity at the level of perceptual or even sensory areas (Sagi and Tanne, 1994). Despite this, only recently has perceptual learning started to be considered a useful tool for improving visual functions in clinical populations. This delayed exploitation has possibly been caused by the common finding that learning was highly specific for the trained stimulus attributes (Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980; Ball and Sekuler, 1981; Ahissar and Hochstein, 1996; Schoups et al., 2001; Campana and Casco, 2003; Fahle, 2005), or even for the trained eye or retinal location (Karni and Sagi, 1991), thus resulting impractical for therapeutic purposes. More recently it has become clear that, under specific training conditions, perceptual learning could generalize to other stimuli, tasks and circumstances (McGovern et al., 2012), yielding potential benefits for various types of visual impairments. So far, perceptual learning has been shown to be effective in improving, among other dysfunctions, visual abilities in amblyopia (Levi and Li, 2009; Polat, 2009; Hussain et al., 2012), mild refractive defects (myopia: Tan and Fong, 2008; Camilleri et al., 2014a; presbyopia: Polat et al., 2012), central or peripheral vision loss and cortical blindness (Kasten et al., 1998; Sabel et al., 2005; Huxlin et al., 2009; Chung, 2011; Das et al., 2014), dyslexia (Gori and Facoetti, 2015), and has even been shown to improve the efficacy of other sensory modalities so that they can somehow replace vision (so called sensory substitution) in blind people (Bach-y-Rita and Kercel, 2003; Ortiz et al., 2011).

The goal of this Research Topic is to demonstrate the development of innovative methods, based on perceptual learning, for treating—or at least overcoming some of the deleterious effects of—various visual dysfunctions, from mild deficits such as myopia to complete blindness. New frontier methods should aim at finding the most effective procedures both in terms of perceptual learning and transfer to useful visual functions. This is made possible by combining different techniques aimed at boosting learning or its generalization, such as training with different stimulus features (Xiao et al., 2008; Harris et al., 2012), exploiting multisensory facilitation (Shams and Seitz, 2008) and reinforcement procedures (Seitz and Watanabe, 2009), or combining perceptual learning with non-invasive brain stimulation procedures (Fertonani et al., 2011). Also, in order to achieve the best possible compliance with the patients, shorter and/or more enjoyable trainings (possibly self-administered at home) should be preferred.

For example, while training on either off-the shelf video games (Li et al., 2011; Franceschini et al., 2013), or specifically designed video games involving detection of low contrast stimuli (Deveau et al., 2014a,b) has been shown to improve a range of visual functions (visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, reading skills and even sport performances) both in normally sighted people and people with developmental dyslexia or amblyopia, in the present Research Topic we see that the latter type of video games can also improve visual acuity in participants with refractive defects such presbyopia (Deveau and Seitz, 2014) or reduce crowding (the deleterious effect of nearby elements on target's perception; Levi, 2008) in participants with cortical deficits such as amblyopia (Hussain et al., 2014). While negligible in normal foveal vision, crowding is an important issue also in children with visual impairment accompanied by nystagmus. Reduction of crowding in these children (besides an improvement of near visual acuity, see Huurneman et al., 2013) can be obtained with training on crowded letters, thus producing faster reaction times and an increase of fixation durations (Huurneman and Boonstra, 2014).

Visual functions in participants with mild refractive defects or amblyopia have also been shown to considerably improve with contrast detection trainings (with or without lateral masking) (Tan and Fong, 2008; Levi and Li, 2009; Polat, 2009; Polat et al., 2012; Camilleri et al., 2014a). Here we see how, both in mild myopia and amblyopia, combining a contrast detection training with non-invasive brain stimulation (specifically, transcranial random noise stimulation—tRNS) seems to yield to faster/more effective perceptual learning and transfer to visual acuity and contrast sensitivity (Camilleri et al., 2014b; Campana et al., 2014).

Perceptual learning can also be successfully applied to patients with loss of central vision. Indeed, past research has shown, in sighted participants, how perceptual learning on a contrast detection task with lateral masking was able to reduce crowding at eccentric retinal locations (Maniglia et al., 2011). Here we see how, in patients with macular degeneration, eccentric perceptual learning with a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) produces an improvement in reading speed mainly with supra-threshold word durations (above 200 ms) (Coates and Chung, 2014), while a texture discrimination training enhances temporal processing of eccentric stimuli (reflected in shorter stimulus onset asynchrony needed for discrimination), especially when fixation was stable (Plank et al., 2014).

In fact improved temporal processing in areas of residual vision (besides an extension of such areas) in patients with vision loss (hemianopia or quadrantanopia) can be also obtained with the so-called vision restoration therapy, an individualized program providing stimulation at the border of the dysfunctional visual field (Poggel et al., 2014).

Finally, perceptual learning could be useful even for blind people. Blindness often produces an impaired spatial representation in other sensory domains (e.g., Gori et al., 2014a). Here it is shown that blindfolded sighted participants can learn an auditory spatial bisection task, but improvements only occur when a tactile feedback is delivered, indicating that the tactile system can be used to recalibrate the spatial representation in the auditory domain (Gori et al., 2014b). This finding suggests that, also in blind people, auditory spatial representation can be improved via tactile feedback.

To sum up the findings of the present Research Topic, the studies collected here provide the frontline of behavioral and brain stimulation-coupled treatments of a heterogeneous ensemble of visual dysfunctions. Future studies are needed to define the best combination of approaches in order to improve vision with the shortest and most efficacious training, increasing patients' compliance and tailoring the training specifically for each patients' needs.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ahissar, M., and Hochstein, S. (1996). Learning pop-out detection: specificities to stimulus characteristics. Vision Res. 36, 3487–3500.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach-y-Rita, P., and Kercel, S. W. (2003). Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 541–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.013

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, K., and Sekuler, R. (1981). Adaptive processing of visual motion. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 7, 780–794.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, R., Pavan, A., Ghin, F., Battaglini, L., and Campana, G. (2014b). Improvement of uncorrected visual acuity and contrast sensitivity with perceptual learning and transcranial random noise stimulation in individuals with mild myopia. Front. Psychol. 5:1234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01234

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, R., Pavan, A., Ghin, F., and Campana, G. (2014a). Improving myopia via perceptual learning: is training with lateral masking the only (or the most) efficacious technique? Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76, 2485–2494. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0738-8

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campana, G., Camilleri, R., Pavan, A., Veronese, A., and Lo Giudice, G. (2014). Improving visual functions in adult amblyopia with combined perceptual training and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS): a pilot study. Front. Psychol. 5:1402. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01402

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campana, G., and Casco, C. (2003). Learning in combined-feature search: specificity to orientation. Percept. Psychophys. 65, 1197–1207. doi: 10.3758/BF03194845

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, S. T. (2011). Improving reading speed for people with central vision loss through perceptual learning. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 52, 1164–1170. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6034

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, D. R., and Chung, S. T. (2014). Changes across the psychometric function following perceptual learning of an RSVP reading task. Front. Psychol. 5:1434. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01434

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, A., Tadin, D., and Huxlin, K. R. (2014). Beyond blindsight: properties of visual relearning in cortically blind fields. J. Neurosci. 34, 11652–11664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1076-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deveau, J., Lovcik, G., and Seitz, A. R. (2014a). Broad-based visual benefits from training with an integrated perceptual-learning video game. Vision Res. 99, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deveau, J., Ozer, D. J., and Seitz, A. R. (2014b). Improved vision and on-field performance in baseball through perceptual learning. Curr. Biol. 24, R146–R147. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deveau, J., and Seitz, A. R. (2014). Applying perceptual learning to achieve practical changes in vision. Front. Psychol. 5:1166. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01166

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahle, M. (2005). Perceptual learning: Specificity versus generalization. Curr. Op. Neurobiol. 15, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahle, M., and Poggio, T. (2002). Perceptual Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Fertonani, A., Pirulli, C., and Miniussi, C. (2011). Random noise stimulation improves neuroplasticity in perceptual learning. J. Neurosci. 31, 15416–15423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2002-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiorentini, A., and Berardi, N. (1980). Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency. Nature 287, 43–44.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. 23, 462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gori, M., Sandini, G., Martinoli, C., and Burr, D. C. (2014a). Impairment of auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human subjects. Brain 137(Pt 1), 288–293. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt311

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gori, M., Vercillo, T., Sandini, G., and Burr, D. (2014b). Tactile feedback improves auditory spatial localization. Front. Psychol. 5:1121. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01121

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gori, S., and Facoetti, A. (2015). How the visual aspects can be crucial in reading acquisition? The intriguing case of crowding and developmental dyslexia. J. Vis. 15, 1–20. doi: 10.1167/15.1.8

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, H., Gliksberg, M., and Sagi, D. (2012). Generalized perceptual learning in the absence of sensory adaptation. Curr. Biol. 22, 1813–1817. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.059

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussain, Z., Astle, A. T., Webb, B. S., and McGraw, P. V. (2014). The challenges of developing a contrast-based video game for treatment of amblyopia. Front. Psychol. 5:1210. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01210

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussain, Z., Webb, B. S., Astle, A. T., and McGraw, P. V. (2012). Perceptual learning reduces crowding in amblyopia and in the normal periphery. J. Neurosci. 32, 474–480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3845-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huurneman, B., and Boonstra, F. N. (2014). Training shortens search times in children with visual impairment accompanied by nystagmus. Front. Psychol. 5:988. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00988

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huurneman, B., Boonstra, F. N., Cox, R. F., Van Rens, G., and Cillessen, A. H. (2013). Perceptual learning in children with visual impairment improves near visual acuity. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 54, 6208–6216. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-12220

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huxlin, K. R., Martin, T., Kelly, K., Riley, M., Friedman, D. I., Burgin, W. S., et al. (2009). Perceptual relearning of complex visual motion after V1 damage in humans. J. Neurosci. 29, 3981–3991. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4882-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karni, A., and Sagi, D. (1991). Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 4966–4970.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasten, E., Wüst, S., Behrens-Baumann, W., and Sabel, B. A. (1998). Computer-based training for the treatment of partial blindness. Nat. Med. 4, 1083–1087.

Google Scholar

Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding–An essential bottleneck for object recognition: a mini-review. Vision Res. 48, 635–654. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, D. M., and Li, R. W. (2009). Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res. 49, 2535–2549. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, R. W., Ngo, C., Nguyen, J., and Levi, D. M. (2011). Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biol. 9:e1001135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001135

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maniglia, M., Pavan, A., Cuturi, L. F., Campana, G., Sato, G., and Casco, C. (2011). Reducing crowding by weakening inhibitory lateral interactions in the periphery with perceptual learning. PLoS ONE 6:e25568. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025568

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGovern, D. P., Webb, B. S., and Peirce, J. W. (2012). Transfer of perceptual learning between different visual tasks. J. Vis. 12:4. doi: 10.1167/12.11.4

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz, T., Poch, J., Santos, J., Requena, C., Martinez, A., Ortiz-Teran, L., et al. (2011). Recruitment of occipital cortex during sensory substitution training linked to subjective experience of seeing in people with blindness. PLoS ONE 6:e23264. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plank, T., Rosengarth, K., Schmalhofer, C., Goldhacker, M., Brandl-Rühle, S., and Greenlee, M. W. (2014). Perceptual learning in patients with macular degeneration. Front. Psychol. 5:1189. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01189

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poggel, D. A., Treutwein, B., Sabel, B. A., and Strasburger, H. (2014). A matter of time: improvement of visual temporal processing during training-induced restoration of light detection performance. Front. Psychol. 6:22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00022

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polat, U. (2009). Making perceptual learning practical to improve visual functions. Vis. Res. 49, 2566–2573. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polat, U., Schor, C., Tong, J. L., Zomet, A., Lev, M., Yehezkel, O., et al. (2012). Training the brain to overcome the effect of aging on the human eye. Sci. Rep. 2:278. doi: 10.1038/srep00278

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabel, B. A., Kenkel, S., and Kasten, E. (2005). Vision restoration therapy. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 89, 522–524. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2005.068163

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagi, D. (2011). Perceptual learning in Vision Research. Vision Res. 51, 1552–1566. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.019

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagi, D., and Tanne, D. (1994). Perceptual learning: learning to see. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 195–199.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoups, A., Vogels, R., Qian, N., and Orban, G. (2001). Practicing orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons. Nature 412, 549–553. doi: 10.1038/35087601

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitz, A. R., and Watanabe, T. (2009). The phenomenon of task-irrelevant perceptual learning. Vision Res. 49, 2604–2610. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shams, L., and Seitz, A. R. (2008). Benefits of multisensory learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 411–417. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, D. T., and Fong, A. (2008). Efficacy of neural vision therapy to enhance contrast sensitivity function and visual acuity in low myopia. J. Cataract Refract. Surg. 34, 570–577. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrs.2007.11.052

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, L. Q., Zhang, J. Y., Wang, R., Klein, S. A., Levi, D. M., and Yu, C. (2008). Complete transfer of perceptual learning across retinal locations enabled by double training. Curr. Biol. 18, 1922–1926. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.030

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: myopia, presbyopia, amblyopia, crowding, nystagmus, tRNS, macular degeneration, blindness

Citation: Campana G and Maniglia M (2015) Editorial: Improving visual deficits with perceptual learning. Front. Psychol. 6:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00491

Received: 18 March 2015; Accepted: 06 April 2015;
Published: 21 April 2015.

Edited and reviewed by: Philippe G. Schyns, University of Glasgow, UK

Copyright © 2015 Campana and Maniglia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gianluca Campana, gianluca.campana@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.