Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 October 2021
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological Factors in Physical Education and Sport View all 28 articles

Differences on Prosaccade Task in Skilled and Less Skilled Female Adolescent Soccer Players

  • 1School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China
  • 2Provincial University Key Laboratory of Sport and Health Science, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China
  • 3Key Laboratory of Kinesiological Evaluation General Administration of Sport of China, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China

Although the relationship between cognitive processes and saccadic eye movements has been outlined, the relationship between specific cognitive processes underlying saccadic eye movements and skill level of soccer players remains unclear. Present study used the prosaccade task as a tool to investigate the difference in saccadic eye movements in skilled and less skilled Chinese female adolescent soccer players. Fifty-six healthy female adolescent soccer players (range: 14–18years, mean age: 16.5years) from Fujian Youth Football Training Base (Fujian Province, China) took part in the experiment. In the prosaccade task, participants were instructed to fixate at the cross at the center of the screen as long as the target appeared peripherally. They were told to saccade to the target as quickly and accurately as possible once it appeared. The results indicated that skilled soccer players exhibited shorter saccade latency (p=0.031), decreased variability of saccade latency (p=0.013), and higher spatial accuracy of saccade (p=0.032) than their less skilled counterparts. The shorter saccade latency and decreased variability of saccade latency may imply that the attentional system of skilled soccer player is superior which leads to smaller attention fluctuation and less attentional lapse. Additionally, higher spatial accuracy of saccade may imply potential structural differences in brain underlying saccadic eye movement between skilled and less skilled soccer players. More importantly, the results of the present study demonstrated that soccer players’ cognitive capacities vary as a function of their skill levels. The limitations of the present study and future directions of research were discussed.

Introduction

Eye-tracking methods are widely used in various research domains such as psychology, psychiatry, or sport (Duchowski, 2003). They are popular because eye movement measures are easy to obtain and participants can understand eye movement tasks easily (Noiret et al., 2017). More importantly, eye-tracking methods provide unique means to measure cognitive processes (Thomas et al., 2021). Therefore, eye movement experiments are carried out in different populations in various research domains (Noiret et al., 2018).

Saccade is a rapid eye movement we typically make three times every second (Rayner, 1998). Saccadic eye movements can be characterized by several eye movement measures such as saccade latency (i.e., the time elapsed from the onset of the target to the onset of the first saccade after target onset), saccade velocity (i.e., the velocity of saccade), and saccade amplitude (i.e., the spatial distance between two successive fixations). The prosaccade task is a well-known experimental paradigm that is used to examine saccadic eye movements. In a typical prosaccade task, participants are instructed to fix on a central dot. Then, they have to direct their gaze toward a target dot appearing at the periphery as quickly and as accurately as possible. Research has shown that saccadic eye movements are associated with multiple cognitive processes including processing speed, attention, and inhibitory control (Hutton, 2008; Noiret et al., 2017).

For instance, some previous studies have revealed that the saccade latency needed to trigger a prosaccade in prosaccade tasks varies as a function of the attentional process (Clark, 1999; Pratt et al., 2006; Noiret et al., 2018). Noiret et al. (2017) found that saccade latency is influenced by information processing speed. Moreover, some research on some special populations has suggested that saccade latency variability is a good indicator of attentional fluctuation in saccadic eye movement tasks (Kaufman et al., 2010, 2012). Kaufman et al. (2010) found that patients with Lewy body had greater saccade latency variability than aged controls. Mostofsky et al. (2001) examined the saccadic eye movements of boys with Tourette syndrome. They found that boys with ADHD (i.e., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) had greater saccade latency variability than boys without ADHD in the prosaccade task. Studies on Alzheimer’s disease have shown that patients with Alzheimer’s disease had longer saccade latency as well as greater saccade latency variability than healthy senior adults.

The purpose of the present study was to use the prosaccade task as a tool to investigate the difference in cognitive processing underlying saccadic eye movements in skilled and less skilled female adolescent soccer players. Soccer is a game that requires players to process a great amount of internal and external information under pressure within a limited time and react to complex and rapidly changing contexts (Vestberg et al., 2012). Soccer players have to distribute their attention over multiple objects (e.g., moving ball, teammates, and opponents) during games (Ando et al., 2001). Therefore, saccadic eye movement is a critical index for soccer players’ performance. Some research has found that eye movement measures vary as a function of skill level in many sports. Ando et al. (2001) revealed that soccer players had shorter visual reaction time than nonathletes when responding to peripheral stimulus, indicating that soccer players had faster visual processing speed. Similarly, Zwierko (2008) found that handball players had shorter response time to peripheral stimulus compared to nonathletes. A recent study showed that cricketers had faster visual reaction times than nonathletes and cricketers’ stability of gaze is associated with faster visual reaction time (Barrett et al., 2020). Moreover, some researchers examined the search behaviors of skilled and less skilled soccer players when viewing film of defensive scenarios in soccer finding that skilled players exhibited more fixations, shorter duration, and more fixation locations than less skilled players (Ward and Williams, 2003; Williams and Ericsson, 2005; Williams, 2009; Roca et al., 2011).

In summary, previous studies have shown that there are differences in eye movement tasks among populations with various skill levels. However, to our knowledge, little work has used the prosaccade task to investigate the differences in saccadic eye movements between skilled and less skilled players. Some studies using the prosaccade task only focus on examining the cognitive changes of players caused by head impact exposure in contact and collision sports (Kiefer et al., 2018; Gallagher et al., 2020).

Therefore, the goal of the present study was to examine the potential differences in cognitive processes underlying saccadic eye movements between skilled and less skilled female adolescent soccer players by using the prosaccade task. Based on previous studies, we predict that skilled players would have higher cognitive ability and would thus perform better than less skilled players in prosaccade task. More specifically, we hypothesized that skilled players will exhibit shorter saccade latency, decreased variability of saccade latency, and higher spatial accuracy of saccade than their less skilled counterparts.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Fujian Normal University. All participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. This study was performed in full compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants

The G*Power tool (Erdfelder et al., 2009) was used to calculate the sample size in the present study. A statistical power analysis was conducted based on the reported effect size of skill level on saccade latency (mean Cohen’s d=0.817, from Ducrocq et al., 2016). The result indicated with an alpha level of 0.05; at least 46 participants are required to get a power of 0.80 (n=23 in each group). Therefore, we recruited 56 healthy female soccer players from Fujian Youth Football Training Base to participate in our experiment, which met the requirements of statistical power for replicating previous results. Their ages ranged from 14 to 18years, with an average of 16.5years. Each player was paid ¥50 for their participation. The experimental design of the present study is a one-way design. Players were assigned into two groups. The skilled player group consisted of 24 skilled players who were determined as Level 1 or Level 2 players. The less skilled player group consisted of 32 less skilled players who were determined as Level 3 or Level 4 players. The levels of players are determined by Fujian Provincial Sports Association according to the latest version of Technical Classification Standards of Soccer Players (2019 edition) developed by Chinese Football Association. Table 1 reports the age, training years, and the skill level ranking for skilled and less skilled players. Before participants were enrolled in the study, each of them was informed about the study protocol and gave his written informed consent. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, with no reported color blindness. None of them have suffered from any neurological or psychiatric disorders.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of skilled and less skilled adolescent soccer players.

Apparatus

The prosaccade task was programmed in Experimental Builder (SR Research Ltd.). The materials were presented on a 17-inch DELL PC laptop (DELL VOSTRO 15; resolution: 1,920×1,080 pixels; refresh rate: 150Hz). Stimulus was displayed in black (RGB: 0, 0, 0) on a grey background (RGB: 153, 153, 153). Participants were seated at a viewing distance of approximately 58cm from the computer monitor. A chin rest was used to stabilize the participants’ heads. Participants viewed stimulus binocularly, while only their right eyes were monitored. Their eye movements were recorded using an Eyelink Portable Duo eye-tracking system with a sampling rate of 1,000Hz.

Procedure

The prosaccade task comprised 75 trials. Five of them were practice trials. Each trial began with a fixation cross (1°×1°) at the center of the screen displayed for 1,000ms. Then, the target circle (1.2°×1.2°) was displayed with an eccentricity of ±10° of visual angle in the horizontal plane for 1,500ms (30 trials for each side), followed by an intertrial interval randomly varied between 800 and 1,200ms. Participants were instructed to fixate at the cross to ensure that they were looking at the center of the screen when the target appeared peripherally. They were also instructed to look at the target circle as quickly and accurately as possible once it appeared.

Participants were tested individually in a quiet room. After reading the experimental instructions and a brief description of the apparatus, the chair was adjusted to make them feel comfortable, and the eye tracker was calibrated using a nine-point calibration and validation procedure. The maximal error of validation was below 0.5° in the visual angle. At the beginning of each trial, a black circle (0.5°×0.5°) was presented on the center of the computer screen as drift correction. Once the participant successfully fixated on the black circle, following stimuli were displayed. The prosaccade task lasted about 12min.

Statistical Analysis

The raw eye movement data were analyzed using Data Viewer (SR Research Ltd.). To ensure the including eye movements data are qualified, following criteria for inclusion were used in analyses (Jazbec et al., 2006): (1) saccades with a latency between 80 and 800ms is observed, (2) saccade durations must be larger than 25ms, and (3) saccade amplitude must be greater than 3°. This resulted in a loss of approximately 11% of the trials.

On the basis of these criteria, the following saccade indexes were derived: (1) saccade latency, which was defined as the time elapsed from the onset of the target to the onset of the first saccade after target onset. (2) Variability of saccade latency, which was defined as the standard deviation of saccade latency. (3) Saccade amplitude, which was defined as the spatial distance between the last fixation on fixation cross and the first fixation on target circle. (4) Spatial accuracy of saccade, which was defined as the distance (in degrees of visual angle) between the landing position of the first saccade toward the target circle and the center of the target circle. (5) Peak velocity, which was defined as peak value of gaze velocity (in visual degrees per second) of the saccade. (6) Average velocity, which was defined as average velocity of the saccade.

Results

Independent sample t tests were conducted to compare the difference in each index between skilled and less skilled players (Table 2). Skilled players had shorter saccade latency (167.33ms, SD=23.32) than less skilled players (184.43ms, SD=32.02), t(54)=−2.21, p=0.031. Additionally, skilled players had decreased variability of saccade latency (33.89ms, SD=10.46) than less skilled players (45.00ms, SD=24.30), t(54)=−2.57, p=0.013. Skilled players showed higher spatial accuracy of saccade (0.54°, SD=0.11) than less skilled players (0.60°, SD=0.08), t(54)=−2.20, p=0.032. The saccade amplitude, peak velocity, and average velocity of two groups of players were comparable (|t|s<0.91; see Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean (SD) of saccadic eye movement indexes in prosaccade task for two groups of players.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Differences in saccadic eye movement indexes between skill and less skilled players.

Discussion

In the current study, we conducted prosaccade task to investigate the difference in saccadic eye movements between skilled and less skilled female adolescent soccer players. As we hypothesized, the results showed that the skilled players, as compared to their less skilled counterparts, were faster to saccade to the target appeared peripherally, exhibited less variability of saccade latency, and more accurate saccade. Hence, our study supports the view that skilled players have higher cognitive ability and would thus perform better than less skilled players in prosaccade task.

The result that skilled players had shorter saccade latency than less skilled players suggests that skilled players had faster information processing speed than less skilled players. This is consistent with previous studies, demonstrating that saccade latency is influenced by information processing speed (Noiret et al., 2017, 2018) and others showing that saccade latency varies as a function of cognitive function (Coors et al., 2021; Thomas et al., 2021). More importantly, this result is also consistent with studies, demonstrating that players’ cognitive capacities vary as a function of their skill levels (Vestberg et al., 2012, 2017; Glavaš, 2020).

Another critical result is that less skilled players exhibited larger variability of saccade latency than skilled players. Kapoula et al. (2010) suggested that variability of saccade latency could be used as a good index of attentional fluctuation in saccadic eye movement tasks. This is due to more attentional lapses caused by more attentional fluctuation in one’s attentional system would contribute more longer reaction times in cognitive tasks (Schmiedek et al., 2007; McVay and Kane, 2012; Unsworth and Robison, 2016). This conclusion was supported by several studies, finding that individuals with inferior attentional system had larger variability of saccade latency than their counterparts with superior attention system (Mostofsky et al., 2001; Jazbec et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2013; Kiefer et al., 2018). Taken together, this result may imply that the attentional system of skilled soccer player is superior which leads to smaller attention fluctuation and less attentional lapse.

The result also suggests that skilled soccer players’ saccades are more accurate than less skilled soccer players. Previous studies have found that the spatial accuracy of prosaccade is mainly influenced by cerebellar (Ettinger et al., 2005; Optican, 2005; Coors et al., 2021), and the cerebellum, particularly the vermis, plays an important role in the spatial accuracy of saccade (Robinson and Fuchs, 2001; Leigh and Kennard, 2004). Hence, this result may imply that the function of cerebellum of skilled soccer player is superior to that of less skilled soccer. Moreover, this result also implies potential structural differences in brain underlying saccadic eye movement between skilled and less skilled soccer players.

No significant differences were observed in saccade amplitude, peak velocity, and average velocity. For saccade amplitude, previous study found that the saccade amplitude was mainly influenced by target eccentricity in prosaccade task (Petrella et al., 2019). On the one hand, the distance between the peripheral target and the position of the onset of saccade (fixation cross) is constant in the present experiment. On the other hand, all participants in this study had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Therefore, the absent of significant difference between two groups in saccade amplitude is understandable. For saccade velocity, previous research has demonstrated that the peak velocity was relatively stable (Coors et al., 2021). Sparks (2002) suggested that, from the perspective of neurophysiology, peak velocity is subject to the number of spikes and firing rate of saccadic burst cells. Some researcher suggested that as people conduct approximately 200,000 saccades per day, the saccadic burst cells are continuously trained (Pratt and Welsh, 2006). Considering that all players in two groups had not suffered any neurological or psychiatric disorders or traumatic brain injury, their nervous system and oculomotor system should be normal and comparable. Therefore, it is also understandable that skilled and less skilled players did not differ in saccade velocity.

The findings of the present study shed some light on understanding of the relationship between saccadic eye movement and skill level in the field of sport and expertise. Besides, these findings supported the results of previous studies, indicating that cognitive ability differed as a function of skill level (Fontani et al., 2006; Brams et al., 2019; Barrett et al., 2020). Furthermore, the present study implies some practical applications for determining or predicting soccer players’ skill level and cognitive abilities by analyzing their saccadic eye movements during some eye movement tasks. It would be more economical and convenient for coaches and training instructors to determine a player’s skill level using some specific eye movement tasks.

Nevertheless, owing to the restriction of conditions, we should note that the present study has some limitations. For example, the current sample is representative of an adolescent, female, and soccer player sample. It remains unclear whether the results are fully generalizable to adolescent male or adult soccer players’ samples. Likewise, it is also unclear whether the results are fully generalizable to players in other sports. Besides, the relatively small sample was used in the present study as was subject to some objective circumstances. The future studies should include a larger sample to get greater statistical power to detect any differences between groups in cognitive processes on eye movements. Additionally, the current study only explored the differences in saccadic eye movements between skilled and less skilled soccer players using prosaccade task. Yet, growing body of research has shown that antisaccade task is a useful tool to examine the differences in inhibitory control and executive functions among different groups of population (Noiret et al., 2018; Gallagher et al., 2020; Bey et al., 2021; Coors et al., 2021). Therefore, future research should consider combining prosaccade task with antisaccade task when examining the saccadic eye movements of soccer players.

Conclusion

In summary, the results of the current study suggest that skilled soccer players outperform less skilled soccer players on saccadic eye movement in prosaccade task indicating superior cognitive processes underlying saccadic eye movement in skilled soccer players. Overall, the present findings have implications for understanding the relationship between saccadic eye movements and skill level.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Academic and Ethics Committee of the Fujian Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

ZJ contributed to the conception, design of the study, conducted the experiment, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the Fujian Social Science Foundation (FJ2018C064), and by a grant from the Planning Project of Fujian Provincial Department of Education (JAS180075).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank all participants for their cooperation. Special thanks go to Yaojun Chen for her selfless contribution and important support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.711420/full#supplementary-material

References

Ando, S., Kida, N., and Oda, S. (2001). Central and peripheral visual reaction time of soccer players and nonathletes. Percept. Mot. Skills 92, 786–794. doi: 10.2466/pms.2001.92.3.786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, B. T., Cruickshank, A. G., Flavell, J. C., Bennett, S. J., Buckley, J. G., Harris, J. M., et al. (2020). Faster visual reaction times in elite athletes are not linked to better gaze stability. Sci. Rep. 10:13216. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69975-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bey, K., Lippold, J. V., Aslan, B., Hurlemann, R., and Ettinger, U. (2021). Effects of lorazepam on prosaccades and saccadic adaptation. J. Psychopharmacol. 35, 91–99. doi: 10.1177/0269881120972424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brams, S., Ziv, G., Levin, O., Spitz, J., Wagemans, J., Williams, A. M., et al. (2019). The relationship between gaze behavior, expertise, and performance: a systematic review. Psychol. Bull. 145, 980–1027. doi: 10.1037/bul0000207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, J. J. (1999). Spatial attention and latencies of saccadic eye movements. Vis. Res. 39, 585–602. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00190-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coors, A., Merten, N., Ward, D. D., Schmid, M., Breteler, M. M. B., and Ettinger, U. (2021). Strong age but weak sex effects in eye movement performance in the general adult population: evidence from the Rhineland study. Vis. Res. 178, 124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchowski, A. T. (2003). Eye Tracking Methodology: Theory and Practice. London: Springer.

Google Scholar

Ducrocq, E., Wilson, M., Vine, S., and Derakshan, N. (2016). Training attentional control improves cognitive and motor task performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 38, 521–533. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2016-0052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ettinger, U., Antonova, E., Crawford, T. J., Mitterschiffthaler, M. T., Goswani, S., Sharma, T., et al. (2005). Structural neural correlates of prosaccade and antisaccade eye movements in healthy humans. Neuroimage 24, 487–494. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontani, G., Lodi, L., Felici, A., Migliorini, S., and Corradeschi, F. (2006). Attention in athletes of high and low experience engaged in different open skill sports. Percept. Mot. Skills 102, 791–805. doi: 10.2466/pms.102.3.791-805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, V. T., Murthy, P., Stocks, J., Vesci, B., Colegrove, D., Mjaanes, J., et al. (2020). Differential change in oculomotor performance among female collegiate soccer players versus non-contact athletes from pre- to post-season. Neurotrauma Rep. 1, 169–180. doi: 10.1089/neur.2020.0051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glavaš, D. (2020). Basic cognitive abilities relevant to male adolescents’ soccer performance. Percept. Mot. Skills 127, 1079–1094. doi: 10.1177/0031512520930158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutton, S. B. (2008). Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements. Brain Cogn. 68, 327–340. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jazbec, S., Hardin, M. G., Schroth, E., McClure, E., Pine, D. S., and Ernst, M. (2006). Age-related influence of contingencies on a saccade task. Exp. Brain Res. 174, 754–762. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0520-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapoula, Z., Yang, Q., Vernet, M., Orssaud, C., Samson, M., Dleudonne, B., et al. (2010). Longévité et robustesse de la saccade oculaire automatique chez le sujet âgé sain: Atteinte dans des cas de démence à corps de Lewy. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 8, 295–306. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2010.0228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., and Black, S. E. (2010). Antisaccades: a probe into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review. J. Alzheimers Dis. 19, 781–793. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, L. D., Pratt, J., Levine, B., and Black, S. E. (2012). Executive deficits detected in mild Alzheimer’s disease using the antisaccade task. Brain Behav. 2, 15–21. doi: 10.1002/brb3.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, A. W., DiCesare, C., Nalepka, P., Foss, K. B., Thomas, S., and Myer, G. D. (2018). Less efficient oculomotor performance is associated with increased incidence of head impacts in high school ice hockey. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leigh, R. J., and Kennard, C. (2004). Using saccades as a research tool in the clinical neurosciences. Brain 127, 460–477. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McVay, J. C., and Kane, M. J. (2012). Drifting from slow to “D’oh!”: working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 38, 525–549. doi: 10.1037/a0025896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mostofsky, S. H., Lasker, A. G., Singer, H. S., Denckla, M. B., and Zee, D. S. (2001). Oculomotor abnormalities in boys with Tourette syndrome with and without ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1464–1472. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200112000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noiret, N., Carvalho, N., Laurent, É., Chopard, G., Binetruy, M., Nicolier, M., et al. (2018). Saccadic eye movements and attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 33, 1–13. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noiret, N., Vigneron, B., Diogo, M., Vandel, P., and Laurent, É. (2017). Saccadic eye movements: what do they tell us about aging cognition? Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 24, 575–599. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1237613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Optican, L. M. (2005). Sensorimotor transformation for visually guided saccades. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1039, 132–148. doi: 10.1196/annals.1325.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrella, A. F. M., Belfry, G., and Heath, M. (2019). Older adults elicit a single-bout post-exercise executive benefit across a continuum of aerobically supported metabolic intensities. Brain Res. 1712, 197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, J., Lajonchere, C. M., and Abrams, R. A. (2006). Attentional modulation of the gap effect. Vis. Res. 46, 2602–2607. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, J., and Welsh, T. (2006). Growing older does not always mean moving slower: examining aging and the saccadic motor system. J. Mot. Behav. 38, 373–382. doi: 10.3200/JMBR.38.5.373-382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, F. R., and Fuchs, A. F. (2001). The role of the cerebellum in voluntary eye movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 981–1004. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roca, A., Ford, P. R., McRobert, A. P., and Williams, A. M. (2011). Identifying the processes underpinning anticipation and decision-making in a dynamic time-constrained task. Cogn. Process. 12, 301–310. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0392-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmiedek, F., Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Süß, H. M., and Wittmann, W. W. (2007). Individual differences in components of reaction time distributions and their relations to working memory and intelligence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 414–429. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.3.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparks, D. L. (2002). The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 952–964. doi: 10.1038/nrn986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, E. H. X., Steffens, M., Harms, C., Rossell, S. L., Gurvich, C., and Ettinger, U. (2021). Schizotypy, neuroticism, and saccadic eye movements: new data and meta-analysis. Psychophysiology 58:e13706. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unsworth, N., and Robison, M. K. (2016). Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 601–615. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0417-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T., Gustafson, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2012). Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS One 7:e34731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T., Reinebo, G., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2017). Core executive functions are associated with success in young elite soccer players. PLoS One 12:e0170845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, P., and Williams, A. M. (2003). Perceptual and cognitive skill development in soccer: the multidimensional nature of expert performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 25, 93–111. doi: 10.1123/jsep.25.1.93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. M. (2009). Perceiving the intentions of others: how do skilled performers make anticipation judgments? Prog. Brain Res. 174, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)01307-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. M., and Ericsson, K. A. (2005). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. Hum. Mov. Sci. 24, 283–307. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Q., Wang, T., Su, N., Xiao, S., and Kapoula, Z. (2013). Specific saccade deficits in patients with Alzheimer’s disease at mild to moderate stage and in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Age 35, 1287–1298. doi: 10.1007/s11357-012-9420-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwierko, T. (2008). Differences in peripheral perception between athletes and nonathletes. J. Hum. Kinet. 19, 53–62. doi: 10.2478/v10078-008-0004-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: saccadic eye movement, prosaccade task, cognitive processes, adolescent soccer players, skill level

Citation: Zhou J (2021) Differences on Prosaccade Task in Skilled and Less Skilled Female Adolescent Soccer Players. Front. Psychol. 12:711420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.711420

Received: 18 May 2021; Accepted: 23 September 2021;
Published: 14 October 2021.

Edited by:

Marianna Alesi, University of Palermo, Italy

Reviewed by:

Manuel Gómez-López, University of Murcia, Spain
Valerio Giustino, University of Palermo, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junyi Zhou, jackychou_1225@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.