Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 December 2022
Sec. Eating Behavior

Images of bodies in mass and social media and body dissatisfaction: The role of internalization and self-discrepancy

  • Department of Communication and Media Research, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland

Introduction: The study examines the influence on body dissatisfaction of viewed images of bodies transmitted over mass media and social media, as mediated by the internalization of body ideals through media and self-discrepancy (the difference between the perceived actual self and the perceived ideal self).

Method: In this study, the images of bodies individuals view in their everyday media diet are estimated using a newly developed pictorial scale for women (thinness) and men (muscularity). For participants, the perceived body image is formed through mass media (magazines, TV) and social media (Facebook, YouTube, Instagram, and Snapchat). The self-discrepancy theory is then used to explain the effect of images of bodies in the media on the internalization of these body ideals and body dissatisfaction.

Results: Results show that Facebook and YouTube shape body ideals perceived to be prevalent in the media, negatively influencing internalization and self-discrepancy. Self-discrepancy, in turn, increases body dissatisfaction. However, for males, the perceived body ideals in the media did not affect body dissatisfaction, internalization, or self-discrepancy.

Discussion: These results emphasize the importance of combining and comparing mass and social media and differentiating between female and male concerns regarding body image.

Introduction

Social, political, and economic pressures establish and perpetuate notions and norms concerning body image, often equating thinness for women and muscularity for men with attractiveness (Bessenoff and Snow, 2006; Shen et al., 2022). Representations of bodies are present in people’s everyday lives. Still, with the rise of mass media and social media, people seem to have become obsessed with images of bodies or appearance (Groesz et al., 2002; Wykes and Gunter, 2005). Media can set and reinforce standards (e.g., the “ideal” body); provide models for social comparison; and eventually lead to psychological (e.g., body dissatisfaction), motivational (e.g., pressure to be thin or muscular), or behavioral outcomes (e.g., disordered eating, exercising, dressing; Diedrichs et al., 2011; Eyal and Te’eni-Harari, 2013). In media outlets, audiences are confronted with physically attractive models and appearance-focused content (de Vries et al., 2016). In particular, social media has developed significant power in promoting attractiveness, exercise, health, and well-being, upholding appearance ideals (Boepple et al., 2016; Griffiths et al., 2018), and increasing users’ weight concerns and body dissatisfaction (Stice and Shaw, 2002; Harper and Tiggemann, 2008; Kleemans et al., 2018).

Most of the studies analyzing the association between media and body image examine the influence of the amount of media use, for example, by asking about the daily hours spent on Instagram or watching television (e.g., Marengo et al., 2018; Vuong et al., 2021; Marques et al., 2022). Other studies manipulate the media content experimentally (McLean et al., 2016; Tiggemann et al., 2018; Anixiadis et al., 2019). Thereby, participants’ effective contact with images of bodies they encounter in their daily media consumption is not considered. While some studies compare the effects of different types of social media (Cohen et al., 2017; Alberga et al., 2018; Griffiths et al., 2018; Marengo et al., 2018) or different mass media outlets (Tiggemann, 2003), there is a lack of studies directly comparing the effects of other mass media outlets on social media outlets (Tiggemann, 2003). Thus, the primary goal of this study is to expand knowledge about the impact of the actual images of bodies transmitted by media by collecting data about those images people confront in their individual diets of mass media and different social media platforms. This adds to the existing research, as not only a general influence of media use is analyzed. More specifically, the images of bodies individuals perceive to be present in their media diet are examined.

When investigating the effect of media use or media-transmitted images of bodies on dissatisfaction, it is essential to consider the mechanism between media contact and expected outcomes. One approach that describes this connection is the self-discrepancy theory, which is often used in studies on body image (Forston and Stanton, 1992; Szymanski and Cash, 1995; Jung et al., 2001; Lantz et al., 2018; Flynn et al., 2020). The self-discrepancy theory posits that a discrepancy between the actual body image (how an individual believes their body looks) and the ideal body image (how an individual wants their body to look) can lead to adverse outcomes (Higgins, 1987). The permanent visibility of images of bodies in the media can influence the perception of an ideal body image shaped through the internalization of these promoted images of bodies as body ideals (Anixiadis et al., 2019). Thus, this study analyzes the role of internalizing body ideals and self-discrepancy through media consumption.

Even though males and females are both influenced by media, the ideal body standards differ, and so does the body image dissatisfaction between genders (Cohane and Pope, 2001). Whereas women in Western societies mainly focus on thinness (e.g., Bessenoff, 2006; Tiggemann and Slater, 2013; Lang et al., 2019), men commonly indicate a desire to develop and maintain muscularity (e.g., Scott and Donald, 2004; Frederick et al., 2007; Stratton et al., 2015; Flynn et al., 2020). Therefore, it is essential to address men and women differently when assessing the effects of media images of bodies (Flynn et al., 2020).

This study focuses on three perspectives regarding the relationship between media use and body dissatisfaction. First, we are interested in the body standards viewers perceive to be prevalent in the media that comprise their particular media diet, respecting individual differences in media consumption. Thereby, perceived body ideals will be measured in several traditional mass media (e.g., TV, magazines) and social media outlets (e.g., Facebook, YouTube, Snapchat, Instagram) to consider more than just one of the media sources shaping body standards. Second, the internalization of these body ideals and the perceived discrepancy between the actual and ideal self are introduced as mediators in the association between viewed media images of bodies and body dissatisfaction. Third, differences between women’s and men’s body ideals are considered, focusing on women’s drive for thinness and men’s drive for muscularity.

Media exposure and body image

Body image is a multifaceted phenomenon that covers an individual’s attitude, feelings, and perceptions about their body size, body shape, and esthetic feelings (Cash, 2004; Wykes and Gunter, 2005). Regular exposure to pronounced thin or muscular bodies depicted in the media can make those bodies desirable. People see those depictions as standards to aspire to Bessenoff (2006). When analyzing the influence of images of bodies in media outlets, most studies ask about participants’ general use of media—for example, how often they read particular magazines (Tiggemann, 2003; Tiggemann and Miller, 2010), watch TV in general (Harrison, 2001), watch specific shows on TV (Hefner et al., 2014), or browse Instagram’s public content (Stein et al., 2019). Those measures can indirectly indicate which kinds of body images the participants are confronted with in their individual media diet.

Other studies investigate the general media use, and specific activities participants engage with on social media. This includes editing editing selfies before posting (McLean et al., 2015; Lonergan et al., 2019; Modica, 2020), the frequency of taking selfies (Modica, 2020) or posting selfies (Ridgway and Clayton, 2016), or the feedback individuals get on their posted pictures (Butkowski et al., 2019) and their influence on body satisfaction, body surveillance, or mood. These studies account for individual media use but mostly focus on the participants’ content, not the other images of bodies they might be confronted with.

In contrast, experimental studies use curated media content depicting various body images and measuring the effect before and after exposure. Some of these studies address only internal validity (Myers and Biocca, 1992; Heinberg and Thompson, 1995; Agliata and Tantleff-Dunn, 2004; Dittmar and Howard, 2004); more recent studies also include a good external validity in these experimental settings (Brown and Tiggemann, 2016; Casale et al., 2021; Sumter et al., 2022). Accurate individual media use is of minor importance in research designs that focus on the effects of particular content. At best, media use, media awareness, or media literacy are integrated into the model as a controlling factor (Anixiadis et al., 2019). Thus, the first goal of this study is to measure the perception of body standards typically viewed by media users in their individual media diet, consisting of traditional mass media and social media platforms, to ensure a high external validity. Thus, the first research question is:

RQ1: Which media types shape the users’ perceived body image in their individual media diets?

Media content analyses have frequently shown that the body type most commonly represented in the media does not match the entire population’s range of body shapes (Eyal and Te’eni-Harari, 2013). The media presents different body image cultures to men and women. For example, the media encourages women to control their weight and get thinner, mainly proliferating a culture of thinness. The media influence males, besides being thin also, to mold their bodies through exercise, proliferating a culture of muscularity (Agliata and Tantleff-Dunn, 2004; Scott and Donald, 2004; Holland and Tiggemann, 2016; Boursier and Gioia, 2022).1 Studies have shown the adverse effects of body ideals transmitted through mass media and social media on audiences’ body dissatisfaction (Grabe et al., 2008; Ferguson, 2013; Holland and Tiggemann, 2016; Mingoia et al., 2017; Huang et al., 2021).

Media exposure to desirable body shapes negatively influences one’s body image (Holmstrom, 2004; Boursier and Gioia, 2022; Shen et al., 2022), eating behaviors (Stice and Shaw, 2002; Griffiths et al., 2018; Saunders and Eaton, 2018; Guo et al., 2022), mental health (Kim and Sundar, 2012; Marengo et al., 2018; McCrory et al., 2022), and body satisfaction (Groesz et al., 2002; Grabe et al., 2008; Ferguson, 2013; Burnette et al., 2017; Wilhelm et al., 2019; Vuong et al., 2021). This negative effect on body satisfaction was shown in mass media, like television (Heinberg and Thompson, 1995; Hefner et al., 2014; Te’eni-Harari and Eyal, 2015) or magazines (Cusumano and Thompson, 1997; Tiggemann, 2003), and over various social media platforms including Facebook (Tiggemann and Slater, 2013; Meier and Gray, 2014; Griffiths et al., 2018), Instagram (Kleemans et al., 2018; Marengo et al., 2018; Tiggemann et al., 2018), Pinterest (Lewallen and Behm-Morawitz, 2016; Simpson and Mazzeo, 2017), and Snapchat (Marengo et al., 2018; Saunders and Eaton, 2018). Thus, as a first step, we assume that idealized images of bodies in the media (thinness for women, muscularity for men) lead to greater dissatisfaction with one’s body. In this study, this association will be further explored by combining mass media and social media to analyze whether different patterns emerge. Also, the hypothesis can be tested without experimentally manipulating thin/muscular body images. High external validity enables the analysis of this well-established relationship considering everyday media use.

H1: The thinner or more muscular the viewers perceive the body ideal in the media, the stronger their body dissatisfaction.

Body ideals and internalization

Besides the direct effect of media exposure on body satisfaction, other factors can influence this relationship (Bessenoff, 2006). For example, the ideal thin-internalization describes the acceptance of social standards of thinness (Heinberg and Thompson, 1995; Cusumano and Thompson, 1997). This refers to a process described in the Tripartite Influence Model, which considers three main factors as sources of body dissatisfaction (Thompson et al., 1999; Keery et al., 2004b; Shroff and Thompson, 2006). Besides media, the other two factors are peers and parents. These three factors potentially influence body dissatisfaction and eating disturbances. However, they also indirectly lead to an internalization of societal appearance standards and comparison processes, shaping body dissatisfaction. The model describes, thus, sociocultural sources which can influence individuals and lead them to a negative evaluation of their bodies (Keery et al., 2004b; Shroff and Thompson, 2006). In this study, the focus lies on the media as one of these three possible influences on body dissatisfaction directly or indirectly through internalizing societal standards.

Media users can internalize body ideals they perceive to be prevalent in mass and social media. This can lead to greater body dissatisfaction when they cannot meet the typically unrealistic body ideals (Lavender et al., 2017; Vuong et al., 2021). Meta-studies have shown that media exposure, in general, can reinforce the internalization of body ideals for both genders and many ages (Huang et al., 2021; Paterna et al., 2021). The images of bodies perceived in the media can become personal standards of attractiveness (Thompson and Stice, 2001). Individuals try to attain internalized body ideals by monitoring their body appearance (Moradi and Huang, 2008; Moradi, 2010). Body depictions can strongly shape the internalized societal standards portrayed in the media (Holland and Tiggemann, 2016; Tatangelo and Ricciardelli, 2017; Rousseau and Eggermont, 2018). Through media internalization, individuals embrace media-promoted body ideals as personal body ideals, manifesting an ideal self and increasing body dissatisfaction (Durkin et al., 2007; Rodgers et al., 2014). Based on these studies, we test this association while considering the individuals’ perceived images of bodies in their everyday media diet and combining mass and social media outlets. As males and females process body images depicted in the media differently (Watson et al., 2019), we strive to test this widely known assumption for males and females separately.

H1a: The effect of thin or muscular body depictions perceived in the media on body dissatisfaction is mediated through internalization of body ideals.

Body ideals and self-discrepancy

Besides increasing body dissatisfaction directly, internalizing body standards can emphasize the discrepancy between actual and ideal selves. Three domains of the self (actual, ideal, and ought) and two standpoints regarding the self (own and significant others) are distinguished in self-discrepancy theory. The actual self is a reflection of the beliefs about one’s attributes; the ideal self represents characteristics that one desires (Higgins, 1987), and the ought self represents details that one believes one should have (Heron and Smyth, 2013). People are motivated to establish consistency between the current self and the relevant self-guide, which in this context are the internalized ideals about body image (Higgins, 1987). Dissatisfaction increases when there is a discrepancy between the actual self and the ideal self (Higgins et al., 1986; Higgins, 1987; Strauman and Higgins, 1987). Although people differ in the degree to which they are motivated, they strive to reduce discrepancies by making their actual self match their ideal self (Heron and Smyth, 2013).

Previous studies have reported different variables that have mediating effects on self-discrepancy, such as self-esteem preventing negative body image after exposure to media images (Posavac et al., 1998; Dittmar and Howard, 2004), negative body image leading to eating disturbances (Forston and Stanton, 1992), and media images of bodies leading to body dissatisfaction (Harrison, 2001; Bessenoff, 2006). Thus, self-discrepancy can be introduced as a mediator between media body standards and body dissatisfaction. Again, this study tests whether this assumption applies to perceived images of the body in everyday media use. Comparing social media and mass media can give further insights into the connection between body images, body dissatisfaction, and self-discrepancy.

H1b: The effect of thin or muscular body images perceived in the media on body dissatisfaction is mediated through self-discrepancy.

Research model

The media can play an essential role in shaping body ideals as viewers internalize body ideals that emphasize the discrepancy between the actual and ideal self. Based on the theoretical assumptions of the self-discrepancy theory and the body of the literature, the final research model (Figure 1) employs internalization and self-discrepancy as mediators between perceived body ideals in the media and body dissatisfaction. The directed hypotheses of the model are:

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research model of the influence of perceived body images in the media on body dissatisfaction, mediated through internalization, and self-discrepancy.

H2: The thinner or more muscular the viewers perceive the body in the media image, the stronger (a) their internalization of body ideals and (b) their self-discrepancy.

H3: The more the viewers internalize the media’s body ideals, the stronger their self-discrepancy.

H4: The stronger the viewers’ self-discrepancy, the stronger their body dissatisfaction.

Materials and methods

Research design and procedure

An online questionnaire was administered to test the research model. To survey a broad demographic, a quota sample from the UK was recruited via Respondi. Following the ethical regulations, only participants over 18 years old were eligible to participate. There was no upper age limit. The participants are members of the research panel Respondi and receive a small monetary incentive for each survey they complete. The sample was not representative but balanced between males and females and covered a broad range of ages and educational backgrounds. Nevertheless, the results cannot be generalized.

The panel members received an email invitation to the survey without any information about the topic. When clicking on the invitation link, they were informed on the first page that the study would be about their use of different media and body images. Also provided was an estimated time to complete the survey and contact information in case of questions or technical problems. Before starting the study, participants received written informed consent. They were informed about the data collection, the use and anonymization of their data, and their rights to withdraw from the study at any time.2 Only participants who accepted the informed consent could continue with the survey.

Participants then answered questions about their general media use and perception of body ideals portrayed on social media and in mass media. They had to choose a picture of a body that best corresponded to their actual and ideal selves. Afterward, they were asked about their satisfaction with their bodies. The survey ended by collecting participants’ sociodemographic information.

Participants

A total of 293 participants (Table 1) from the UK were recruited. Data collection took place between June 13 and June 25, 2019. Participants needed, on average, 5 min and 29 s to complete the survey. In the sample, 53% of the participants were male (n = 156), and a majority were over 55 years old (41%). Most participants were married (53%), and most were European/Caucasian (88%). Participants had an average body mass index of 27 (BMI; underweight < 18.5, obese ≥ 40; M = 27.35, SD = 6.51; n = 274).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample.

Only 15% of the participants did not use any social media platform daily, but they use social media in general. Facebook was the most-used platform, with 49% using it for 1 h a day, followed by YouTube with 45%, Instagram with 32%, and a mere 16% using Snapchat daily. The use of mass media was more common than in social media use in the sample. Only 4% of the participants reported using mass media for less than an hour daily. Television was used between 2 and 5 h a day (59%), whereas printed media was used for 1 h a day (46%).

Measures

Scale development for body ideals

Various techniques exist to measure the perception of body ideals, for example, linear, configurational, pictorial, or verbal (Wykes and Gunter, 2005). In this study, a configurational rating scale method was used. These measures have proven robust and valid (Thompson and Gray, 1995; Conti et al., 2013; Ralph-Nearman and Filik, 2018). The differences in body image, body ideal, and satisfaction for males and females had to be considered in building the scale. Existing scales for assessing body image either only account for one of the sexes without a corresponding equivalent for the other sex (e.g., male fit body scale; Ralph-Nearman and Filik, 2018), or the image scales only cover one of the ideals found in media (e.g., drive for thinness for males and females; Harris et al., 2008). Therefore, a new range of figures was created to capture two ideal body types often prevalent in media content. For females, the drive for thinness was chosen. For males, the drive for muscularity seems to be prevalent in different media outlets ranging from television to social media platforms, even though both drives can also be found for the other sex (e.g., Hildebrandt et al., 2004; Kelley et al., 2010). To capture females’ ideal for thinness, the scale includes 13 drawings of females that range from very thin to obese. For males, the scale consists of 13 corresponding pictures of males ranging from leptosome to overly muscular to capture males’ desire for muscularity (Figures 2, 3). To compare the scales for both sexes, the higher rankings corresponded more closely with the respective ideals (1 = less thin/less muscular, 13 = thinner/more muscular).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The newly developed body shape scale assesses women’s drive for thinness (1 = very thin, 13 = obese).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The newly developed body shape scale assesses men’s drive for muscularity (1 = leptosome, 13 = highly muscular).

Actual self and ideal self

Using pictorial body scales to assess participants’ actual and ideal selves has proven valid in diverse settings and samples (e.g., Lynch et al., 2009; Conti et al., 2013; Ralph-Nearman and Filik, 2018). To measure the body image of the actual self, participants were asked to choose the picture from the body image scale of their gender that best matched their actual appearance (“What do you look like right now?”; M = 5.99, SD = 2.53). The ideal self of participants’ body image was assessed by asking the participants to choose the picture that best matched the appearance they wish to have (“What would you like to look like?”; M = 7.70, SD = 2.32).

The discrepancy between the actual self and the ideal self

The discrepancy indicator was calculated by subtracting the value of the actual self from the value of the ideal self for each participant as proven to be valid in previous research (e.g., Lynch et al., 2009; Conti et al., 2013). Therefore, positive scores indicate higher values for the ideal self than the actual self (72%), and zero indicates no difference between the actual and ideal self (20%). To better interpret the variable, participants with negative scores, meaning that their actual body was thinner/more muscular than the ideal self, were excluded (8%) so that a positive coefficient indicates an unhealthy discrepancy between the ideal and actual self.

Perceived body depictions in the media

In addition to the actual and ideal self, the pictorial body image scale was also used to assess the body depictions participants encountered in their individual media diets. Participants were asked to indicate the body depiction they are usually confronted with in their media diet (“Which of the following pictures correspond best to the men/women you see most frequently in the media you use?”). This was measured separately for television, magazines, YouTube, Facebook, Instagram, and Snapchat.

Internalization of body ideals

The thin/muscular bodies internalization was assessed using three items of the Sociocultural Internalization of Appearance Questionnaire (SIAQ) by Keery et al. (2004b). The scale is an extension of the validated Internalization Subscale of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (Thompson et al., 2004). The scale was validated in samples from three countries and showed high internal consistency in six independent samples (Keery et al., 2004a). The items were adapted for females and males (“Looking at the body images in the media makes me want to lose weight/gain muscles”). M = 2.54, SD = 1.20.

Body dissatisfaction

For females, the Body Dissatisfaction Scale (Garner, 1991), a sub-scale of Garner’s Eating Disorder Inventory, was used (e.g., “I think my hips are too big”). This scale is a standardized clinical evaluation of symptomatology associated with eating disorders and is thus suitable for this study’s purpose. As an equivalent scale for males, the Body Dissatisfaction Scale (Yelland and Tiggemann, 2003) was used (e.g., “I think my biceps are too small”). This scale is based on the validated Body Esteem Scale (Franzoi and Shields, 1984) and has shown good validity in other studies (Yelland and Tiggemann, 2003). Five items out of the original nine from both scales were used. Only the statements that did not contradict one another were selected to still cover the concept as broadly as possible but with minimizing the number of items and effort of the participants (M = 2.97, SD = 1.24, αfemales = 0.92, αmales = 0.77; N = 292; 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 = never to 5 = always).

Statistical analysis

To test the impact of perceived media images on self-discrepancy and body dissatisfaction, we used partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM). PLS is a causal-predictive approach to SEM that emphasizes prediction in estimating models derived based on theory and logic (Hair et al., 2019; Cho et al., 2022). PLS-SEM readily incorporates formatively specified constructs where the indicators are designed to jointly form said constructs (Sarstedt et al., 2016), which is the case for perceived body ideals in our model. Two path models were estimated based on the different body image scales (thinness for women and muscularity for men).

Results

The results for men show that there was a negative effect of internalization on self-discrepancy (β = −0.39, SD = 0.19, p = 0.024). However, perceived media image had neither a direct nor an indirect (via self-discrepancy) effect on body dissatisfaction. As this study focuses on the media’s role in shaping the internalization of body ideals, self-discrepancy, and body dissatisfaction, only the model based on women is included below. The null results for men are taken up again in the discussion.

The reflective measurement model results (see Figure 4 with further details in Tables 24) show that the construct measures exhibit sufficient internal consistency, reliability, and convergent validity (see Table 2). In the formative measurement model (see Table 3), we controlled for collinearity with the VIF values (Hair et al., 2016). In the next step, we ran bootstrapping (10,000 subsamples) to assess the significance of the formative indicator weights. Weights for Facebook and YouTube were significant. As the loadings of all media perceptions (besides magazines) on perceived body image were 0.50 or larger, these indicators made a decisive contribution to the construct. They were kept in the model based on theoretical assumptions. The indicator weights answered RQ1, which asked which media outlets shape the respondents’ body images. YouTube and Facebook were the essential factors forming the perceived body ideal in the media and thus created self-discrepancy and, eventually, body dissatisfaction.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Research model for females and their drive for thinness. Indirect effect through internalization and self-discrepancy (H1a), indirect effect through self-discrepancy (H1b). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reliabilities for reflective measured latent constructs.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Outer weights of formative indicators for individuals’ body images in the media.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of body images in the media on body dissatisfaction mediated through self-discrepancy and internalization.

Overall, the model fit indices are relatively low (SRMR = 0.12, NFI = 0.70). In PLS models, the classical goodness-of-fit tests are not very informative, as they usually compare the covariance matrices of a saturated vs. an estimated model. With partial least square estimation, a formative indicator can be built, not referring to the covariance matrix. Literature on PLS also suggests not relying on these indices (Henseler and Sarstedt, 2013). Therefore, the indices are reported for transparency, but the models were not adopted data-driven to improve model fit with assumptions possibly violating the theoretical assumptions.

H1 proposed that the thinner the body depictions are perceived in the media, the higher the body dissatisfaction will be. However, the data showed no significant direct effect of perceived thinness on body dissatisfaction (β = 0.27; SD = 0.18, p = 0.061; H1 rejected). The thinner individuals perceived the body images in the media to be correlated with a higher internalization of body ideals through media (β = 0.37, SD = 0.20, p = 0.035, H2a confirmed). However, internalization showed no effect on self-discrepancy (β = 0.05, SD = 0.02, p = 0.419; H3 rejected). A more pronounced self-discrepancy was associated with a higher body dissatisfaction (β = 0.39, SD = 0.15, p = 0.004; H4 accepted). The thinner the body images in the media were perceived to be, corresponded with a higher discrepancy between the actual self and the ideal self (β = 0.43, SD = 0.24, p < 0.037; H2b confirmed). The thinner women perceived the body depictions on YouTube and Facebook, the more pronounced they judged the difference between their actual body image and the body shape they wished to have. While the direct effect of the perceived body ideals in the media on body dissatisfaction was not significant, the indirect effect through self-discrepancy was significant (β = 0.17, SD = 0.10, p = 0.038; H1b confirmed), but not through internalization and self-discrepancy (H1a rejected; see Table 4 for more details).

Exploratory analyses

As in the PLS models, only participants with valid answers for all media outlets could be considered; a supplementary analysis was run to explore the hypotheses only for mass media. This model introduced TV and magazines as formative indicators of body image. The results show that, in particular, magazines (β = 0.71; SD = 0.34, t = 2.01, p = 0.018) and in tendency TV (β = 0.49; SD = 0.35, p = 0.083) form individuals’ media-perceived body ideal. Interestingly, this perceived body ideal formed from traditional mass media directly explains body dissatisfaction (β = 0.30; SD = 0.10, t = 2.872, p = 0.002), but the indirect effect via a pronounced self-discrepancy disappears (β = 0.05, SD = 0.07; p = 0.239). The effect of perceived body image on self-discrepancy is no longer significant (β = 0.09, SD = 0.13; t = 0.723, p = 0.235). This means that traditional media can still contribute to body dissatisfaction but not by triggering self-discrepancy beliefs.

Discussion

The current study aimed to contribute to the research on the influence of media-transmitted body ideals in traditional mass and social media on body dissatisfaction. It sought to explain this effect through internalizing body ideals and perceived self-discrepancy between how one looks and wishes to look. Three relevant contributions of the study should be highlighted: First, the study used all participants’ perceived media-transmitted body ideals within the background of their actual media diet, including traditional mass media and several social media outlets. Second, this estimation took advantage of a specifically adapted image scale to assess the fragile construct of body image as intuitively as possible. Third, the measures were collected for males (muscularity) and females (thinness) within a comparatively broad sample of demographic structures.

For this study, a pictorial-figure rating scale was developed to measure females’ drive for thinness and males’ drive for muscularity. The use of a visual scale for participants to indicate their media perception maximizes the intuitiveness of the measurement. Both scales were applied in this study to measure the participants’ actual body shape, their ideal body shape, and the body shapes they are confronted with on social media and mass media. The 13 images of body shapes are a major contribution of this study to the existing literature, as pictorial rating scales have proven to be a valid and reliable way to measure body images (Thompson and Gray, 1995; Ralph-Nearman and Filik, 2018). The scale is based on an often validated and applied pictorial scale that only consists of seven figures (Stunkard et al., 1983). The newly developed scale adds intermediate figures to this scale, allowing a more nuanced measurement of body shapes. Nevertheless, the newly developed scale must be validated to fully ensure a valid and reliable measurement in future studies.

According to the self-discrepancy theory, the discrepancy between the actual and ideal body image can lead to adverse outcomes such as negative feelings or even depression (Higgins et al., 1986; Higgins, 1987). Thus, the first hypothesis was that the thinner or more muscular the viewers perceive the body ideal to be in the media, the stronger their body dissatisfaction should be. The data of this study showed that the thinner the body images in social media are perceived to be for women, the more pronounced the discrepancy between the actual self and the ideal self. This confirms that the thinner or more muscular the viewers perceive the body ideal in the media, the stronger their body dissatisfaction (H1) and the results of previous studies on this association (Harrison, 2001; Halliwell and Dittmar, 2006; Hefner et al., 2014) and underlines the importance of media in this context of body image concerns.

Through media internalization, individuals endorse media-promoted body ideals as standards of attractiveness to be attained, resulting in the manifestation of an ideal self (Bessenoff, 2006). The confrontation with body ideals in the media can lead to an unrealistic ideal self, usually manifesting as social media outlets being prone to increase the internalization of body ideals (Perloff, 2014; Mingoia et al., 2017). This assumption was tested: The effect of thin or muscular body depictions perceived in the media on body dissatisfaction is mediated through the internalization of body ideals. The results showed that the thinner body depictions in the media are perceived to be, the more robust the internalization of these ideals. However, this did not lead to a more pronounced discrepancy between the actual and ideal selves. More internalization of the media’s body ideals did not lead to a stronger self-discrepancy among viewers. This could be due to several reasons. Media internalization was considered a mediator, but it could also be argued that it is instead a trait and should be introduced as a moderator (Bessenoff, 2006; Krawczyk and Thompson, 2015; McLean et al., 2016; Vuong et al., 2021). Additionally, the association between internalization and body dissatisfaction can be explained through self-discrepancy and other processes such as body surveillance (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., 2012), which should be considered in future studies.

The indirect effect of perceived body ideals in the media on body dissatisfaction via self-discrepancy indicates that the association between media perception and body dissatisfaction can partially be explained by triggering a pronounced gap between perceived body ideals and desired appearance. The effect of thin or muscular body images perceived in the media on body dissatisfaction is mediated through self-discrepancy. Besides media exposure, other drivers for the actual/ideal/ought self-congruency may be present. Moreover, the effect might be more pronounced if moderators introduced the association between media perceptions and self-discrepancy. These could be, for example, personality traits (Roberts and Good, 2010), motives for media use (Tiggemann, 2005), media literacy (Yamamiya et al., 2005; Levine and Murnen, 2009; Anixiadis et al., 2019), and parasocial relationships with media characters or character liking (Eyal and Te’eni-Harari, 2013; Te’eni-Harari and Eyal, 2015). It can also be argued that self-discrepancy and internalization of body ideals may drive or reinforce a particular media use or perception of media content (Rousseau and Eggermont, 2018; Marques et al., 2022). However, causality cannot be proven using our non-experimental cross-sectional design.

Although numerous studies investigate the association between body image and mass media or social media exposure, there is still a dearth of literature directly comparing the effects of traditional mass media to social media (Perloff, 2014). The strength of this study is that the perceived mediated body ideal was considered a formative construct, referring to an index of a weighted sum of variables of perceived body ideals in different media outlets. The data suggest that, in particular, YouTube and Facebook contribute to the media-transmitted body ideal for females, triggering self-discrepancy and eventual body dissatisfaction. Surprisingly, Instagram was not an important factor in this process, contradicting the results of other studies in which Instagram was a driver for self-discrepancy, body dissatisfaction, or other body image concerns (Tiggemann et al., 2018; Anixiadis et al., 2019; Stein et al., 2019; McCrory et al., 2022). Traditional mass media do not play a crucial role in influencing body ideals. Social media, with its pronounced active, selective, and highly individual choice of content, triggers social comparison and eventually leads to pronounced self-discrepancy (Marengo et al., 2018; McCrory et al., 2022).

The results for men and the media’s influence on their body dissatisfaction through body ideals with a drive for muscularity are discussed separately. For men, all media indicators had non-significant weights to form the latent variable perceived images of bodies in media. Thus, the different images of bodies perceived to be prevalent on these other media platforms do not seem to result in a general construct measuring body images in the media. Accordingly, no effects of these general body images can be found for self-discrepancy or body dissatisfaction. Studies investigating men’s body perceptions of their peer groups found that men are more likely to compare themselves to their peers than media depictions and that peers have more significant effects on body satisfaction (Yamamiya et al., 2005; Ferguson, 2013; Piatkowski et al., 2022).

There are several possible explanations for our results for men. First, this study only examined men’s drive for muscularity, which limits the findings. For men, there is a dual drive to be thin and to be muscular, and they may be split between both drives (Hildebrandt et al., 2004). Men were shown to have the desire to look lean with low body fat (e.g., Ricciardelli, 2012; Field et al., 2014), and the internalization of these thin ideals through media was shown to increase male body dissatisfaction (Dye, 2016b). It could be that rather than the drive for muscularity, a combination of both these drives for ideal body image or only the drive for thinness on the different media platforms influences men’s body dissatisfaction. The inclusion of only one of these body image desires does not cover men’s body ideals holistically and limits this study’s findings. Second, it could be that media probably influence men less, and they react differently to body ideals presented in media than females (Watson et al., 2019), as shown in other studies (Yamamiya et al., 2005; Ferguson, 2013; Piatkowski et al., 2022). Other factors, e.g., social comparison, could be more relevant regarding men’s body dissatisfaction. Also, the media content and other factors, for example, users’ comments, could influence men’s reactions to body images (Kim, 2021). Additionally, our sample consisted of men from different age groups, while many studies are conducted with relatively young samples. This raises the question if these findings are also applicable to the general population and would need to be tested with a representative sample.

The present study has some further limitations. First, the validity of measuring media perception could be criticized. As we measured the perceived body ideals in mass media and on social media, we knew neither the exact content participants were consuming nor the amount of the overall media consumption. Therefore, the actual body depictions presented in the media could not be verified, and the participants experienced the perceived body image subjectively. Another drawback is asking for only one body ideal as an indicator of all perceived body ideals in the media. It is a broad indicator (individual yet aggregated) and just functions as a point estimator for a wide variety of body shapes to which users are exposed in their media diet. Future studies might use the scale to ask people about a range of body images rather than forcing them to choose one picture. Furthermore, both the drive for thinness and the drive for muscularity should be examined for males and females (Kelley et al., 2010), as media could influence body ideals in more than just one way, and all individuals, regardless of their gender, can have a drive for thinness and/or muscularity (e.g., Dye, 2016a; Huang et al., 2021). In media, different body ideals can be represented for both gender (Huang et al., 2021), influencing their body dissatisfaction. Future studies should include these dual drives for thinness and muscularity for all individuals.

Second, social comparison theory was not explicitly considered. The theory establishes individuals’ need to compare themselves with others to determine their levels of abilities and success (Festinger, 1954). Many studies dealing with body image and media seek to explain the influence of media exposure on body dissatisfaction with upward social comparison processes between audiences and the people represented in the media (Field et al., 1999; Martin et al., 2004; Tiggemann and McGill, 2004; Bessenoff, 2006). In the research model of the present study, we included only the perceived body ideal in the media so no specific person would be available for social comparisons in the context of this ideal. However, social comparison theory could provide valuable insight into media influences on body dissatisfaction in future studies.

Third, the sample of the study limits the findings of this study. It was a non-representative sample, not allowing generalization of the results. However, a wide spectrum of age, BMI, and gender-balanced sample was achieved. The final sample in the analysis was relatively small, as only participants indicating body images for all kinds of media were considered. This was necessary for comparing the media outlets and the meaningful construction of the latent variable perceived body ideal. Although pairwise deletion would have resulted in different calculations, the analysis would have been based on different sample sizes, which can bias the results.

Further, the small sample size did not allow the integration of more variables into the model to, for example, control for age or BMI, as the model would have been too complex to estimate. This is a limitation to the findings, as BMI was shown to be a detrimental factor regarding body dissatisfaction (Conti et al., 2013; Marques et al., 2022). In the exploratory analysis, we further tested a model with only mass media, television, and magazines to enhance a larger sample. Thereby, mass media was shown to influence body dissatisfaction directly. This would mean that although social media is prevalent in forming body ideals, mass media still have power in this regard.

Conclusion

The current study contributes to the discussion on the association between media exposure and body dissatisfaction as mediated by self-discrepancy and internalization. The study contributes to the existing literature by providing a newly developed pictorial scale with 13 images to measure females’ drive for thinness and males’ drive for muscularity. The scale was employed to measure the actual self, the ideal self, and the perceived body shapes in media. The body images individuals perceive to be present in the different media outlets could be considered. This ensures higher external validity than just examining the amount of time spent with certain media outlets. Data showed that the direct effects of perceived media-transmitted body ideals on body dissatisfaction for women are mediated through self-discrepancy but not internalization. Mainly, social media shape the body images perceived to be present for women. For men, the perceived body ideal for muscularity did not trigger internalization of body ideals, self-discrepancy, or body dissatisfaction. Future studies should continue to analyze the role of mass and social media and body image concerns while distinguishing between women’s and men’s transmitted body ideals.

Data availability statement

The data presented in this study can be found in the online repository OSF: https://osf.io/gzafh/.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AF and FM developed the research idea. FM performed the data collection. MM wrote the theoretical part and discussion, described the method, and conducted the data preparation. AF ran the statistical analysis and write-up, supervised the project, and performed the proofreading of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Different body ideals exist for males. Besides the desire for increased muscularity, there is also the desire for weight change and reduced body fat (e.g., Drewnowski et al., 1995; Yelland and Tiggemann, 2003; Hildebrandt et al., 2004). However, in this study, only males’ drive for muscularity is analyzed.
  2. ^ The study was not approved by an ethics committee, as to the time of data collection the Institutional Review Board did not exist. The informed consent data collection procedure were in line with ethical guidelines and conform to the IRB directives of today.

References

Agliata, D., and Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2004). The impact of media exposure on males’ body image. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 7–22. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.1.7.26988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberga, A. S., Withnell, S. J., and von Ranson, K. M. (2018). Fitspiration and thinspiration: a comparison across three social networking sites. J. Eat. Disord. 6:39. doi: 10.1186/s40337-018-0227-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anixiadis, F., Wertheim, E. H., Rodgers, R., and Caruana, B. (2019). Effects of thin-ideal instagram images: the roles of appearance comparisons, internalization of the thin ideal and critical media processing. Body Image 31, 181–190. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bessenoff, G. R. (2006). Can the media affect us? social comparison, self-discrepancy, and the thin ideal. Psychol. Women Quarterly 30, 239–251. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00292.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bessenoff, G. R., and Snow, D. (2006). Absorbing society’s influence: body image self-discrepancy and internalized shame. Sex Roles 54, 727–731. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9038-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boepple, L., Ata, R. N., Rum, R., and Thompson, J. K. (2016). Strong is the new skinny: a content analysis of fitspiration websites. Body Image 17, 132–135. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boursier, V., and Gioia, F. (2022). Which are the effects of body-objectification and instagram-related practices on male body esteem? a cross-sectional study. Clin. Neuropsychiatr. 19, 8–19. doi: 10.36131/cnfioritieditore20220103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, Z., and Tiggemann, M. (2016). Attractive celebrity and peer images on Instagram: effect on women’s mood and body image. Body Image 19, 37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnette, C. B., Kwitowski, M. A., and Mazzeo, S. E. (2017). I don’t need people to tell me I’m pretty on social media:” a qualitative study of social media and body image in early adolescent girls. Body Image 23, 114–125. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butkowski, C. P., Dixon, T. L., and Weeks, K. (2019). Body surveillance on instagram: examining the role of selfie feedback investment in young adult women’s body image concerns. Sex Roles 81, 385–397. doi: 10.1007/s11199-018-0993-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casale, S., Gemelli, G., Calosi, C., Giangrasso, B., and Fioravanti, G. (2021). Multiple exposure to appearance-focused real accounts on instagram: effects on body image among both genders. Curr. Psychol. J. Diverse Perspect. Diverse Psychol. Issues 40, 2877–2886. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00229-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, T. F. (2004). Body image: past, present, and future. Body Image 1, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00011-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, G., Sarstedt, M., and Hwang, H. (2022). A comparative evaluation of factor- and component-based structural equation modelling approaches under (in)correct construct representations. Br. J. Mathematical Statistical Psychol. 75, 220–251. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohane, G. H., and Pope, H. G. (2001). Body image in boys: a review of the literature. Int. J. Eat Disord. 29, 373–379. doi: 10.1002/eat.1033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., and Slater, A. (2017). The relationship between Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activities and body image concerns in young women. Body Image 23, 183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conti, M. A., Ferreira, M. E. C., de Carvalho, P. H. B., Kotait, M. S., Paulino, E. S., Costa, L. S., et al. (2013). Stunkard figure rating scale for Brazilian men. Eat Weight Disord. 18, 317–322. doi: 10.1007/s40519-013-0037-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusumano, D. L., and Thompson, J. K. (1997). Body image and body shape ideals in magazines: exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles: J. Res. 37, 701–721.

Google Scholar

de Vries, D. A., Peter, J., de Graaf, H., and Nikken, P. (2016). Adolescents’ social network site use, peer appearance-related feedback, and body dissatisfaction: testing a mediation model. J. Youth Adolescence 45, 211–224. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0266-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichs, P. C., Lee, C., and Kelly, M. (2011). Seeing the beauty in everyday people: a qualitative study of young Australians’ opinions on body image, the mass media and models. Body Image 8, 259–266. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dittmar, H., and Howard, S. (2004). Professional hazards? the impact of models’ body size on advertising effectiveness and women’s body-focused anxiety in professionals that do and do not emphasize the cultural ideal of thinness. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 477–497. doi: 10.1348/0144666042565407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drewnowski, A., Kurth, C. L., and Krahn, D. D. (1995). Effects of body image on dieting, exercise, and anabolic steroid use in adolescent males. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 17, 381–386.

Google Scholar

Durkin, S. J., Paxton, S. J., and Sorbello, M. (2007). An integrative model of the impact of exposure to idealized female images on adolescent girls’ body satisfaction. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1092–1117. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00201.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dye, H. (2016a). Are there differences in gender, race, and age regarding body dissatisfaction? J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 26, 499–508. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2015.1091240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dye, H. (2016b). Does internalizing society and media messages cause body dissatisfaction, in turn causing disordered eating? J. Evidence-Inform. Soc. Work 13, 217–227. doi: 10.1080/23761407.2015.1024572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyal, K., and Te’eni-Harari, T. (2013). Explaining the relationship between media exposure and early adolescents’ body image perceptions: the role of favorite characters. J. Media Psychol. Theories Methods Appl. 25, 129–141.

Google Scholar

Ferguson, C. J. (2013). In the eye of the beholder: thin-ideal media affects some, but not most, viewers in a meta-analytic review of body dissatisfaction in women and men. Psychol. Popular Media Culture 2, 20–37. doi: 10.1037/a0030766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relations 7, 117–140. doi: 10.1177/001872675400700202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. E., Camargo, C. A., Taylor, C. B., Berkey, C. S., and Colditz, G. A. (1999). Relation of peer and media influences to the development of purging behaviors among preadolescent and adolescent girls. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 153, 1184–1189. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.11.1184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. E., Sonneville, K. R., Crosby, R. D., Swanson, S. A., Eddy, K. T., Camargo, C. A., et al. (2014). Prospective associations of concerns about physique and the development of obesity, binge drinking, and drug use among adolescent boys and young adult men. JAMA Pediatrics 168, 34–39. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.2915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzsimmons-Craft, E. E., Harney, M. B., Brownstone, L. M., Higgins, M. K., and Bardone-Cone, A. M. (2012). Examining social physique anxiety and disordered eating in college women. the roles of social comparison and body surveillance. Appetite 59, 796–805. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flynn, M. A., Cotchett, E., and Lin, L. (2020). The relationship between men’s peer and social media muscularity ideal discrepancies and body satisfaction. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 37, 1534–1553. doi: 10.1177/0265407520903387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forston, M. T., and Stanton, A. L. (1992). Self-discrepancy theory as a framework for understanding bulimic symptomatology and associated distress. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 11, 103–118. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1992.11.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franzoi, S. L., and Shields, S. A. (1984). The body esteem scale: multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college population. J. Pers. Assess 48, 173–178. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4802_12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Buchanan, G. M., Sadehgi-Azar, L., Peplau, L. A., Haselton, M. G., Berezovskaya, A., et al. (2007). Desiring the muscular ideal: Men’s body satisfaction in the United States, Ukraine, and Ghana. Psychol. Men Masculinity 8, 103–117. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.8.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating Disorder Inventory: Professional Manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., and Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychol. Bull. 134, 460–476. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, S., Murray, S. B., Krug, I., and McLean, S. A. (2018). The contribution of social media to body dissatisfaction, eating disorder symptoms, and anabolic steroid use among sexual minority men. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 21, 149–156. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., and Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of experimental presentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Eat Disord. 31, 1–16. doi: 10.1002/eat.10005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, L., Gu, L., Peng, Y., Gao, Y., Mei, L., Kang, Q., et al. (2022). Online media exposure and weight and fitness management app use correlate with disordered eating symptoms: evidence from the mainland of China. J. Eat. Disord. 10:58. doi: 10.1186/s40337-022-00577-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C., and Sarstedt, M. (2016). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), 2nd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Risher, J. J., Sarstedt, M., and Ringle, C. M. (2019). When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. Eur. Bus. Rev. 31, 2–24. doi: 10.1108/EBR-11-2018-0203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliwell, E., and Dittmar, H. (2006). Associations between appearance-related self-discrepancies and young women’s and men’s fffect, body satisfaction, and emotional eating: a comparison of fixed-item and participant-generated self-discrepancies. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 447–458. doi: 10.1177/0146167205284005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, B., and Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal media images on women’s self-objectification, mood, and body image. Sex Roles 58, 649–657. doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9379-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M., Anseel, F., and Lievens, F. (2008). Keeping up with the joneses: a field study of the relationships among upward, lateral, and downward comparisons and pay level satisfaction. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 665–673. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, K. (2001). Ourselves, our bodies: thin-ideal media, self-discrepancies, and eating disorder symptomatology in adolescents. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 20, 289–323. doi: 10.1521/jscp.20.3.289.22303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hefner, V., Woodward, K., Figge, L., Bevan, J. L., Santora, N., and Baloch, S. (2014). The influence of television and film viewing on midlife women’s body image, disordered eating, and food choice. Media Psychol. 17, 185–207. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2013.838903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinberg, L. J., and Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness and attractiveness: a controlled laboratory investigation. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 14, 325–338. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1995.14.4.325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J., and Sarstedt, M. (2013). Goodness-of-fit indices for partial least squares path modeling. Comput. Stat. 28, 565–580. doi: 10.1007/s00180-012-0317-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heron, K. E., and Smyth, J. M. (2013). Body image discrepancy and negative affect in women’s everyday lives: an ecological momentary assessment evaluation of self-discrepancy theory. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 32, 276–295. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2013.32.3.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychol. Rev. 94, 319–340. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., and Strauman, T. J. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional vulnerability: how magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influence affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 5–15. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hildebrandt, T., Langenbucher, J., and Schlundt, D. G. (2004). Muscularity concerns among men: development of attitudinal and perceptual measures. Body Image 1, 169–181. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holland, G., and Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image 17, 100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmstrom, A. J. (2004). The effects of the media on body image: a meta-analysis. J. Broadcasting Electron. Media 48, 196–217. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4802_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Q., Peng, W., and Ahn, S. (2021). When media become the mirror: a meta-analysis on media and body image. Media Psychol. 24, 437–489. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2020.1737545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, J., Lennon, S. J., and Rudd, N. A. (2001). Self-Schema or self-discrepancy? which best explains body image? Clothing Textiles Res. J. 19, 171–184. doi: 10.1177/0887302X0101900403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keery, H., Shroff, H., Thompson, J. K., Wertheim, E., and Smolak, L. (2004a). The Sociocultural Internalization of Appearance Questionnaire — Adolescents (SIAQ-A): psychometric analysis and normative data for three countries. Eat Weight Disord 9, 56–61. doi: 10.1007/BF03325046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keery, H., van den Berg, P., and Thompson, J. K. (2004b). An evaluation of the tripartite influence model of body dissatisfaction and eating disturbance with adolescent girls. Body Image 1, 237–251. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, C. C. G., Neufeld, J. M., and Musher-Eizenman, D. R. (2010). Drive for thinness and drive for muscularity: opposite ends of the continuum or separate constructs? Body Image 7, 74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. M. (2021). What do others’ reactions to body posting on Instagram tell us? the effects of social media comments on viewers’ body image perception. New Media Soc. 23, 3448–3465. doi: 10.1177/1461444820956368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y., and Sundar, S. S. (2012). Visualizing ideal self vs. actual self through avatars: impact on preventive health outcomes. Comp. Hum. Behav. 28, 1356–1364. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleemans, M., Daalmans, S., Carbaat, I., and Anschütz, D. (2018). Picture perfect: the direct effect of manipulated Instagram photos on body image in adolescent girls. Media Psychol. 21, 93–110. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2016.1257392

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krawczyk, R., and Thompson, J. K. (2015). The effects of advertisements that sexually objectify women on state body dissatisfaction and judgments of women: the moderating roles of gender and internalization. Body Image 15, 109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, B., Ahlich, E., Verzijl, C. L., Thompson, J. K., and Rancourt, D. (2019). The role of drive for thinness in the association between weight status misperception and disordered eating. Eat. Behav. 35:101319. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.101319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lantz, E. L., Gaspar, M. E., DiTore, R., Piers, A. D., and Schaumberg, K. (2018). Conceptualizing body dissatisfaction in eating disorders within a self-discrepancy framework: a review of evidence. Eat Weight Disord 23, 275–291. doi: 10.1007/s40519-018-0483-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavender, J. M., Brown, T. A., and Murray, S. B. (2017). Men, muscles, and eating disorders: an overview of traditional and muscularity-oriented disordered eating. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 19:32. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0787-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, M. P., and Murnen, S. K. (2009). “Everybody knows that mass media are/are not [pick one] a cause of eating disorders”: a critical review of evidence for a causal link between media, negative body image, and disordered eating in females. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 28, 9–42. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2009.28.1.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewallen, J., and Behm-Morawitz, E. (2016). Pinterest or Thinterest?: social comparison and body image on social media. Soc. Media Soc. 2:2056305116640559. doi: 10.1177/2056305116640559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lonergan, A. R., Bussey, K., Mond, J., Brown, O., Griffiths, S., Murray, S. B., et al. (2019). Me, my selfie, and I: the relationship between editing and posting selfies and body dissatisfaction in men and women. Body Image 28, 39–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, E., Liu, K., Wei, G. S., Spring, B., Kiefe, C., and Greenland, P. (2009). The relation between body size perception and change in body mass index over 13 Years: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 169, 857–866. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marengo, D., Longobardi, C., Fabris, M. A., and Settanni, M. (2018). Highly-visual social media and internalizing symptoms in adolescence: the mediating role of body image concerns. Comp. Hum. Behav. 82, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, M. D., Paxton, S. J., McLean, S. A., Jarman, H. K., and Sibley, C. G. (2022). A prospective examination of relationships between social media use and body dissatisfaction in a representative sample of adults. Body Image 40, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, B. A. S., Lang, B., and Wong, S. (2004). Conclusion explicitness in advertising: the moderating role of need for cognition (NFC) and argument quality (AQ) on persuasion. J. Advert. 32, 57–65.

Google Scholar

McCrory, A., Best, P., and Maddock, A. (2022). “It’s just one big vicious circle”: young people’s experiences of highly visual social media and their mental health. Health Educ. Res. 37, 167–184. doi: 10.1093/her/cyac010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, S. A., Paxton, S. J., and Wertheim, E. H. (2016). Does media literacy mitigate risk for reduced body satisfaction following exposure to thin-ideal media? J. Youth Adolescence 45, 1678–1695. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0440-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, S. A., Paxton, S. J., Wertheim, E. H., and Masters, J. (2015). Photoshopping the selfie: self photo editing and photo investment are associated with body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 1132–1140. doi: 10.1002/eat.22449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, E. P., and Gray, J. (2014). Facebook photo activity associated with body image disturbance in adolescent girls. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 199–206. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mingoia, J., Hutchinson, A. D., Wilson, C., and Gleaves, D. H. (2017). The relationship between social networking site use and the internalization of a thin ideal in females: a meta-analytic review. Front. Psychol. 8:1351. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modica, C. A. (2020). The associations between instagram use, selfie activities, appearance comparison, and body dissatisfaction in adult men. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 23, 90–99. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moradi, B. (2010). Addressing gender and cultural diversity in body image: objectification theory as a framework for integrating theories and grounding research. Sex Roles 63, 138–148. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9824-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moradi, B., and Huang, Y.-P. (2008). Objectification theory and psychology of women: a decade of advances and future directions. Psychol. Women Quarterly 32, 377–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00452.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, P. N., and Biocca, F. A. (1992). The elastic body image: the effect of television advertising and programming on body image distortions in young women. J. Commun. 42, 108–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1992.tb00802.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterna, A., Alcaraz-Ibáñez, M., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., and Sicilia, Á (2021). Internalization of body shape ideals and body dissatisfaction: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 54, 1575–1600. doi: 10.1002/eat.23568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perloff, R. M. (2014). Social media effects on young women’s body image concerns: theoretical perspectives and an agenda for research. Sex Roles 71, 363–377. doi: 10.1007/s11199-014-0384-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piatkowski, T. M., Obst, P. L., White, K. M., and Hides, L. (2022). The relationship between psychosocial variables and drive for muscularity among male bodybuilding supplement users. Aust. Psychol. 57, 148–159. doi: 10.1080/00050067.2021.2020074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posavac, H. D., Posavac, S. S., and Posavac, E. J. (1998). Exposure to media images of female attractiveness and concern with body weight among young women. Sex Roles 38, 187–201. doi: 10.1023/A:1018729015490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ralph-Nearman, C., and Filik, R. (2018). New body scales reveal body dissatisfaction, thin-ideal, and muscularity-ideal in males. Am. J. Mens. Health 12, 740–750. doi: 10.1177/1557988318763516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricciardelli, L. (2012). “Body image development - adolescent boys,” in Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance, ed. T. F. Cash (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier Academic Press), doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-384925-0.00028-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridgway, J. L., and Clayton, R. B. (2016). Instagram unfiltered: exploring associations of body image satisfaction, instagram #selfie posting, and negative romantic relationship outcomes. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19, 2–7. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A., and Good, E. (2010). Media images and female body dissatisfaction: the moderating effects of the five-factor traits. Eat. Behav. 11, 211–216. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R. F., Paxton, S. J., and McLean, S. A. (2014). A biopsychosocial model of body image concerns and disordered eating in early adolescent girls. J. Youth Adolescence 43, 814–823. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0013-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, A., and Eggermont, S. (2018). Media ideals and early adolescents’ body image: selective avoidance or selective exposure? Body Image 26, 50–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarstedt, M., Hair, J. F., Ringle, C. M., Thiele, K. O., and Gudergan, S. P. (2016). Estimation issues with PLS and CBSEM: where the bias lies! J. Bus. Res. 69, 3998–4010. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, J. F., and Eaton, A. A. (2018). Snaps, selfies, and shares: how three popular social media platforms contribute to the sociocultural model of disordered eating among young women. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 21, 343–354. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, J. D., and Donald, R. M. (2004). Body image, eating disorders, and the drive for muscularity in gay and heterosexual men. J. Homosexuality 47, 45–58. doi: 10.1300/J082v47n03_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, J., Chen, J., Tang, X., and Bao, S. (2022). The effects of media and peers on negative body image among Chinese college students: a chained indirect influence model of appearance comparison and internalization of the thin ideal. J. Eat. Disord. 10:49. doi: 10.1186/s40337-022-00575-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shroff, H., and Thompson, J. K. (2006). The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance: a replication with adolescent girls. Body Image 3, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, C. C., and Mazzeo, S. E. (2017). Skinny is not enough. a content analysis of fitspiration on Pinterest. Health Commun. 32, 560–567. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1140273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J.-P., Krause, E., and Ohler, P. (2019). Every (Insta)gram counts? applying cultivation theory to explore the effects of Instagram on young users’ body image. Psychol. Popular Media Culture 10, 87–97. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., and Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. J. Psychosom. Res. 53, 985–993. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00488-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stratton, R., Donovan, C., Bramwell, S., and Loxton, N. J. (2015). Don’t stop till you get enough: factors driving men towards muscularity. Body Image 15, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauman, T. J., and Higgins, E. T. (1987). Automatic activation of self-discrepancies and emotional syndromes: when cognitive structures influence affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 1004–1014. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.6.1004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stunkard, A. J., Sørensen, T., and Schulsinger, F. (1983). Use of the danish adoption register for the study of obesity and thinness. Res. Publ. Assoc. Res. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 60, 115–120.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sumter, S. R., Cingel, D., and Hollander, L. (2022). Navigating a muscular and sexualized Instagram feed: an experimental study examining how Instagram affects both heterosexual and nonheterosexual men’s body image. Psychol. Popular Media 11, 125–138. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymanski, M. L., and Cash, T. F. (1995). Body-image disturbances and self-discrepancy theory: expansion of the body-image ideals questionnaire. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 14, 134–146. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1995.14.2.134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatangelo, G. L., and Ricciardelli, L. A. (2017). Children’s body image and social comparisons with peers and the media. J. Health Psychol. 22, 776–787. doi: 10.1177/1359105315615409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Te’eni-Harari, T., and Eyal, K. (2015). Liking them thin: adolescents’ favorite television characters and body image. J. Health Commun. 20, 607–615. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2015.1012241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., Coovert, M. D., and Stormer, S. M. (1999). Body image, social comparison, and eating disturbance: a covariance structure modeling investigation. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 26, 43–51. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199907)26:1<43::aid-eat6>3.0.co;2-r

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., and Stice, E. (2001). Thin-Ideal internalization: mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 181–183. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., and Heinberg, L. J. (2004). The sociocultural attitudes towards appearance scale-3 (SATAQ-3): development and validation. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 293–304. doi: 10.1002/eat.10257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, M. A., and Gray, J. J. (1995). Development and validation of a new body-image assessment scale. J. Personal. Assess. 64, 258–269. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6402_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M. (2003). Media exposure, body dissatisfaction and disordered eating: television and magazines are not the same! Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 11, 418–430. doi: 10.1002/erv.502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M. (2005). Television and adolescent body image: the role of program content and viewing motivation. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24, 361–381. doi: 10.1521/jscp.24.3.361.65623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., Hayden, S., Brown, Z., and Veldhuis, J. (2018). The effect of Instagram “likes” on women’s social comparison and body dissatisfaction. Body Image 26, 90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., and McGill, B. (2004). The role of social comparison in the effect of magazine advertisements on women’s mood and body dissatisfaction. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 23–44. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.1.23.26991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., and Miller, J. (2010). The internet and adolescent girls’ weight satisfaction and drive for thinness. Sex Roles 63, 79–90. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9789-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., and Slater, A. (2013). NetGirls: the internet, facebook, and body image concern in adolescent girls. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 630–633. doi: 10.1002/eat.22141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuong, A. T., Jarman, H. K., Doley, J. R., and McLean, S. A. (2021). Social media use and body dissatisfaction in adolescents: the moderating role of thin- and muscular-ideal internalisation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:13222. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182413222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, A., Murnen, S. K., and College, K. (2019). Gender differences in responses to thin, athletic, and hyper-muscular idealized bodies. Body Image 30, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, L., Hartmann, A. S., Becker, J. C., Kisi, M., Waldorf, M., and Vocks, S. (2019). Thin media images decrease women’s body satisfaction: comparisons between veiled muslim women, christian women and atheist women regarding trait and state body image. Front. Psychol. 10:1074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wykes, M., and Gunter, B. (2005). The Media and Body Image: If Looks could Kill. London: SAGE Publications Ltd, doi: 10.4135/9781446215395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamiya, Y., Cash, T. F., Melnyk, S. E., Posavac, H. D., and Posavac, S. S. (2005). Women’s exposure to thin-and-beautiful media images: body image effects of media-ideal internalization and impact-reduction interventions. Body Image 2, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yelland, C., and Tiggemann, M. (2003). Muscularity and the gay ideal: body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in homosexual men. Eat. Behav. 4, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00014-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: body images in media, body dissatisfaction, body shape scale, self-discrepancy theory, internalization of body ideals, survey design

Citation: Möri M, Mongillo F and Fahr A (2022) Images of bodies in mass and social media and body dissatisfaction: The role of internalization and self-discrepancy. Front. Psychol. 13:1009792. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1009792

Received: 02 August 2022; Accepted: 25 November 2022;
Published: 20 December 2022.

Edited by:

Antonio Cepeda-Benito, University of Vermont, United States

Reviewed by:

Ada H. Zohar, Ruppin Academic Center, Israel
Mary Pritchard, Boise State University, United States
Lianguo Fu, Bengbu Medical College, China

Copyright © 2022 Möri, Mongillo and Fahr. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michelle Möri, michelle.moeri@unifr.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.