ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 June 2025

Sec. Educational Psychology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1471486

Investigating the demotivation of Moroccan CFL learners from an alienation theory perspective

  • College of Humanities and Foreign Languages, China Jiliang University, Hangzhou, China

This study reports on the results of a multi-method investigation on second language (L2) demotivation among Moroccan Learners of Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL) learners from the perspective of Alienation Theory. A total of 97 Moroccan students with varied majors from a university in East China took part in a questionnaire survey. Then Semi-structured interviews were applied to collect the data. Through Pearson’s correlations, a multiple regression and a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) analysis approach, this study found that L2 demotivation prevailed among the participants, influenced by an array of social isolation or estrangement-mediated and normlessness-mediated factors among classic four alienators. Students at different L2 demotivation levels reacted to alienators differently. Students from the high demotivation group suffered most across all four demotivators, but such gaps appeared most substantially in social isolation or estrangement, followed by meaninglessness, and then powerlessness. No significant difference was found between average demotivators and low demotivators in powerlessness and meaninglessness. Qualitative findings contextualized these results, identifying institutional pressures and sociocultural barriers as key mechanisms driving motivational attrition. The study advances theoretical understanding of L2 demotivation in transnational educational contexts and provides actionable strategies for fostering inclusive CFL pedagogies.

Introduction

Learning Motivation plays a critical role in successful second language acquisition (Oxford and Shearin, 1994). Not only has positive motivation received a lot of attention, such as enjoyment, resilience and flow (e.g., Jin and Zhang, 2018; Liu and Song, 2021; Liu and Han, 2022), but negative motivation has also been heatedly discussed in recent years (Dewaele et al., 2019), especially after Dörnyei (2001) proposed definition of second language (L2) learning demotivation. After that, more and more researchers have paid close attention to L2 learning demotivation (e.g., Kikuchi, 2009; Karaca and Inan, 2020). To date, most previous studies have focused on describing demotivation and identifying its demotivators in English as a foreign language (EFL) class (Hu and Cai, 2010; Li, 2013; Moiinvaziri and Razmjoo, 2014; Kim et al., 2018). However, few studies have investigated the demotivation of Chinese as a foreign language (CFL) class, which thus necessitates a more comprehensive approach to understand the formulation of L2 demotivation of CFL learners.

Studying abroad constitutes a transitional phase marked by multifaceted physiological, social, and emotional adjustments. Failure to navigate these transitions effectively may lead to maladjustment and psychological challenges, including academic alienation—a phenomenon negatively associated with educational quality and linked to broader institutional issues such as reduced academic achievement and behavioral problems (Newmann, 1981). Universities worldwide face growing complexities in supporting culturally diverse student populations, particularly as international students frequently encounter systemic microaggressions across academic and social environments (Ancis et al., 2000; Solórzano et al., 2000). Compounding these challenges are persistent sociocultural barriers, including social alienation, racial stereotyping, cultural intolerance, and exclusionary practices, which exacerbate psychological distress and hinder integration (Hanassab, 2006; Klomegah, 2006; Lee and Rice, 2007; Sherry et al., 2010). Academic alienation, defined as a “a separation or distance among two or more entities and involves a sense of anguish or loss, resulting in a student viewing life and school as fragmentary and incomplete” (Dean, 1961; Galbo, 1980), correlates strongly with attrition risks, underscoring its significance as a predictor of dropout intentions (Schram and Lauver, 1988; Nottingham et al., 1992).

China’s growing economic influence and expanding global trade partnerships have positioned it as a key destination for international students. In recent years, initiatives such as the Zhejiang-Africa educational cooperation program have further incentivized international enrollment, particularly among Moroccan high school graduates pursuing tertiary education at universities in Hangzhou, Zhejiang Province. Central to these efforts is Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL) education, which aims to equip students with advanced linguistic proficiency and intercultural communication skills. To achieve this, universities provide daily language courses, including Comprehensive Chinese, Chinese Characters and Writing, and Chinese Listening and Speaking. Proficiency in Chinese is critical for academic success, as it enables students to meet institutional requirements such as passing the HSK (Hanyu Shuiping Kaoshi)—a standardized proficiency test—and obtaining graduation credentials (Liu et al., 2020).

However, emerging observations from educators, including the authors, highlight demotivation as a critical issue among international students in Chinese universities, particularly Moroccan learners. Compared to peers from other countries, Moroccan students exhibited notably higher disengagement in Chinese language courses, manifested through recurrent tardiness, early departures, absenteeism, and midterm attrition. Classroom participation was markedly low: students often avoided discussions, provided minimal responses to questions, and cited non-specific health concerns (e.g., “feeling unwell”) to justify disengagement. Furthermore, social isolation was evident, with many students sitting alone in corners during class and reporting limited interaction with Chinese peers. These behaviors collectively signal a progressive decline in motivation toward Chinese language acquisition. Such patterns underscore the urgency for CFL educators and administrators to critically examine international students’ learning experiences and implement strategies to sustain L2 motivation. This study therefore investigates the contextual and psychosocial factors driving Moroccan students’ demotivation within Chinese higher education settings.

Literature review

Research on L2 demotivation

Research on L2 demotivation originated in European contexts (Chambers, 1993; Oxford and Shearin, 1994) and subsequently expanded to Asian EFL settings, including Japan (Kikuchi, 2019; Weda, 2018), South Korea (Kim and Kim, 2015; Song and Kim, 2017), and China (Xie et al., 2018; Zhou, 2012). Early conceptualizations defined demotivation as “external forces that reduce or diminish the motivational basis of a behavioral intention or ongoing action” (Dörnyei, 2001), framing it primarily as an externally driven phenomenon, particularly teacher-related influences. For instance, Chambers (1993) seminal study of 191 British secondary students exclusively ascribed L2 motivational decline to teacher-related variables. This teacher-centric perspective persisted in Oxford and Shearin’s (1994) longitudinal analysis of 250 U. S. foreign language learners, identifying four educator-linked demotivation catalysts.

While these studies laid critical groundwork for understanding L2 demotivation, their heavy focus on external factors (e.g., pedagogical practices, classroom dynamics) resulted in a partial and learner-passive conceptualization, neglecting internal psychological and socio-cognitive dimensions (Falout and Maruyama, 2004). Zsuzsanna’s (1996) exploratory work with 15 Hungarian adolescents first identified learner-internal and contextual co-determinants, shifting focus beyond teacher-centric explanations. Building on this, Dörnyei (1998) analyzed 50 Hungarian secondary students learning English or German and proposed nine demotivation factors: (1) teacher factors, (2) undesirable teaching environment, (3) decreasing confidence, (4) negative attitude towards the target language, (5) the target language as a compulsory subject, (6) interference of anther foreign language under learning, (7) negative attitudes towards the native country associated with the target language, (8) attitudes towards peers around, and (9) textbooks and teaching materials. While Dörnyei (2001) reaffirmed teachers as a primary contributor, this multidimensional framework catalyzed later explorations of internal factors. Subsequent studies highlighted learner-internal drivers, such as negative prior language experiences (Zhou, 2012), low perceived competence (Ghadirzadeh et al., 2012), and diminished self-esteem or interest (Falout and Maruyama, 2004; Trang and Baldauf, 2007; Moiinvaziri and Razmjoo, 2014; Akay, 2017), particularly in Asian EFL contexts.

Recent studies on L2 motivation have expanded beyond traditional teacher-learner dynamics to examine systemic educational and technological factors influencing learners’ motivational trajectories. Key contextual drivers include classroom environments, assessment systems, instructional methodologies, and technology integration. For instance, Iftanti et al. (2023) observed significant motivational decline among Indonesian EFL learners during pandemic-era online learning, correlating with reduced linguistic achievement. Similarly, Zhang (2024) longitudinal analysis highlighted sustained motivation loss in hybrid learning contexts. Assessment practices have emerged as critical demotivators: Alamer et al. (2023) identified repeated low test scores as catalysts for frustration and eroded self-efficacy, while Escudero et al. (2024) documented chronic demotivation arising from persistent gaps between learner abilities and institutional expectations. Pedagogical approaches also play a role, with grammar-heavy instruction cited as a primary demotivator across diverse cultural settings (Bribesh, 2024). Additionally, overreliance on machine translation tools has been linked to learner complacency and reduced engagement, as evidenced by Klimova’s (2025) mixed-methods study, urging cautious implementation of such technologies in language education.

Existing research on L2 demotivation predominantly adopts a multifactorial framework, identifying diverse external (e.g., pedagogical practices, institutional policies) and internal (e.g., self-efficacy, affective states) factors that contribute to motivational attrition. While these studies have advanced our understanding of discrete demotivators, they largely overlook the dynamic interplay between these factors, particularly in Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL) context. Current approaches rarely delineate how external constraints (e.g., curricular demands, sociocultural barriers) and internal psychological processes (e.g., identity conflicts, perceived incompetence) interact to shape learners’ motivational trajectories. To address this gap, this study employs Alienation Theory (Hadjar and Lupatsch, 2010; Morinaj et al., 2017), which holistically integrates environmental pressures and subjective experiences to explain disengagement mechanisms. This theoretical lens enables a systematic examination of how institutional, interpersonal, and intrapersonal factors coalesce to generate demotivation in CFL settings.

Demotivation from an alienation theory perspective

Alienation theory

The concept of alienation—derived from the Latin verb alienare (“to remove” or “to separate”)—originated in theological and philosophical discourses, later expanding into sociological, historical, and educational domains (Sarfraz, 1997). Historically, it has been variably conceptualized, ranging from theological interpretations (e.g., humanity’s estrangement from divine order) to sociological critiques of modern industrial society (e.g., loss of individual agency). Seeman (1991) proposed six major variants of alienation (powerlessness, normlessness, meaninglessness, self-estrangement, social isolation, and cultural estrangement) in order to integrate various meanings of the concept.

To address conceptual ambiguities, Seeman (1991) systematized alienation into six dimensions: (1) powerlessness (perceived inability to influence one’s environment), (2) meaninglessness (absence of behavioral or belief guidance), (3) normlessness (adoption of illegitimate means to achieve goals), (4) social isolation (emotional detachment from sociocultural norms), (5) cultural estrangement (disconnection from dominant cultural values), and (6) self-estrangement (disengagement from intrinsically rewarding activities). Subsequent scholarship refined these dimensions for empirical application, particularly in educational contexts. For instance, studies on student alienation emphasize powerlessness (e.g., perceived lack of academic autonomy), social isolation (e.g., peer disconnection), and self-estrangement (e.g., disidentification with institutional goals) as key measurable constructs (Hadjar et al., 2015; Hascher and Hadjar, 2017; Hascher and Hagenauer, 2010). This multidimensional framework enables nuanced analysis of alienation as both a psychological state and a systemic phenomenon.

Empirical research has validated the multidimensional nature of alienation across diverse educational and sociocultural contexts. For instance, Khan et al. (2020) demonstrated that ESL teachers’ assessment practices—particularly the predominance of face-threatening over face-saving feedback—heightened students’ alienation during language tasks, with speaking activities showing the highest alienation levels, followed by writing and comprehension exercises. The COVID-19 pandemic further illuminated environmental drivers of alienation: Dakhi’s (2020) and Lalwani et al.’s (2023) studies linked prolonged remote learning to intensified feelings of detachment from peers and instructors, exacerbating academic disengagement. Cross-cultural investigations by Pabodha and Abeywickrama (2021) revealed that 90% of Sri Lankan students abroad experienced linguistic alienation post-arrival, manifesting as struggles in social integration, lecture comprehension, and classroom participation. Barua (2022) redefined language anxiety as a form of alienation, arguing that learners’ fear of cultural marginalization amplifies anxiety and impedes L2 acquisition. Collectively, these studies underscore alienation as a dynamic construct shaped by pedagogical, environmental, and intercultural factors.

While student alienation remains an emerging construct in educational research, its exploration has predominantly focused on Western contexts (Biasco et al., 2001). In contrast, empirical investigations of international student alienation in Chinese higher education remain scarce, despite its rising relevance amid growing transnational enrollment. Longitudinal studies reveal that initial student engagement often diminishes due to unmet academic or social expectations, leading to progressive estrangement, negative institutional attitudes, and eventual attrition (Archambault et al., 2009; Eccles and Alfeld, 2007). For instance, Moroccan students in Chinese universities may experience alienation through linguistic barriers, cultural disjuncture, social isolation, or perceived academic stagnation—factors that collectively foster a sense of marginalization within the host educational system. These observations underscore the urgency of contextualized research on alienation mechanisms in non-Western academic settings.

Conceptual framework

Building on the preceding analysis, demotivation is conceptualized as a socially mediated phenomenon characterized by a perceived disconnect between learners and key educational actors or structures (e.g., peers, instructors, institutional norms), coupled with psychological distress that diminishes motivation (Dean, 1961; Galbo, 1980). Drawing from this conceptualization, Figure 1 proposes an analytical framework for L2 demotivation, grounded in its multidimensional and socially embedded nature (Rashidi et al., 2014). Rooted in alienation theory (Brown et al., 2003), the framework operationalizes demotivation through four interrelated dimensions: powerlessness (i.e., teachers), normlessness (i.e., unapproved behaviors), meaninglessness (i.e., engaged in certain or any school activities), and social isolation/estrangement (i.e., interpersonal relationship including cognitive and emotional components) as school alienation.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual framework.

Demotivation in Chinese language learning emerges as a socially mediated process shaped by both systemic factors (e.g., institutional policies, pedagogical practices) and learner-related dynamics (e.g., non-conforming behaviors, perceived social isolation). The interplay of these factors may either facilitate or impede the development of learning motivation, contingent on their alignment or conflict. Grounded in alienation theory, the proposed analytical framework elucidates how demotivation arises through four interconnected mechanisms, offering a holistic lens to examine motivational attrition in Chinese language acquisition contexts.

Given the rising enrollment of international students, understanding the dual mechanisms of motivational enhancement and demotivational triggers—particularly in the context of social alienation—is critical to improving educational outcomes for both educators and learners. While existing research has explored motivational strategies, the role of alienation in shaping demotivation remains underexamined. To address this gap, the study proposes the following research questions:

1. Do Moroccan students in CFL class experience demotivation in their Chinese learning in Chinese University?

2. What alienation factors might account for the demotivation in learning Chinese among the participants?

3. Do students at different L2 demotivation levels react to alienators differently?

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were 35 male and 62 female Moroccan students aged from 19 to 22 years old currently studying at a local university in Zhejiang province, PRC between March 2024 to April 2024. The enquiry took place after they had been learning Chinese for around 4 months. The questionnaire was administered to the participants during a 30-min class break. Participation was completely voluntary and anonymous.

Six participants were purposefully selected through maximum variation sampling to ensure diversity across three dimensions: academic performance (high, moderate, low), gender balance (3 males, 3 females), and disciplinary backgrounds (6 distinct majors). This strategy enabled cross-case analysis while maintaining demographic alignment with the broader study cohort (N = 97). The sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the interviewer sample.

Instrument

In this study, questionnaires and semi-structured interviews protocol were used to collect data.

We incorporated the scale of demotivation and alienation into the questionnaire design and ensured that the questionnaire was consistent with the study’s content and objectives, from item setting to question wording, and worked out “Questionnaire for Investigating Chinese Learning Demotivation among Moroccan students in Chinese University.” The questionnaire consisted of two main parts: the first part contains of the demographic information, including participant profile, concerning their gender, age and grade. The second part was the main survey, which comprises 20 items to provide an all-embracing picture of Moroccan students’ Chinese learning from the dimensions of “perceived language learning demotivation,” “learner alienation.” Questions 1 to 5 test for demotivation, and were represent with Dn. Questions 6 to 20 test for alienation, and were represent with An. The items were rated using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

The measurement of perceived learning demotivation consulted Albalawi and Al-Hoorie’s (2021) demotivation model, which is a self-reported questionnaire to assess the L2 learning demotivation. The Cronbach’s alpha, of this measure, is 0.877. The measurement of learner Alienation was developed from Jessor and Jessor’s (1977) General Alienation Scale (GAS), which has been extensively used in related studies to measure learner alienation from multiple dimensions. The scale consists of 15 items, primarily measuring feelings of interpersonal alienation, uncertainty about one’s involvement in activities, and a sense of separation from others. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient ranged from 0.80 to 0.83. All indicated good validity and reliability.

To triangulate survey findings, semi-structured interviews were conducted to collect qualitative data aligned with Brown et al.’s (2003) four-dimensional alienation framework. Each 20-min interview followed a protocol systematically addressing four alienation components: meaninglessness, powerlessness, normlessness and social isolation. Interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim, and iteratively refined through member checking to ensure accuracy. The protocol underwent linguistic validation to enhance cross-cultural clarity. For instance, the question “Does the student feel it is all right to break the law as long as he or she does not get caught?” was revised to “Some people believe it’s acceptable to break rules if there are no consequences. What is your perspective?” This adaptation preserved theoretical fidelity while improving respondent comprehension. Probing questions were dynamically adjusted based on participant responses to deepen exploration of alienation mechanisms.

Data analysis

Data from the questionnaires were first filled into an Excel form, and then were input into SPSS 26.0 for analysis. Firstly, the data were analyzed using descriptive statistics to show the profile of the questionnaire items. Table 2 displays the descriptive statistics of participants’ questionnaire response. Both the skewness and kurtosis were between –2 and +2, enumerating that all the data is normally distributed, and they could be used for further parametric inferential analysis. It can be seen that most means of the items are between 3.00 and 4.00, except for item A2, A4, A5, A13 (concerning meaningless) barely under 3.00. More than half of the participants choose 4 (agree) or 5 (strongly agree) for item A6 and A7, which means this item perceived as more alienating by participants.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for participants’ questionnaire responses (N = 97).

Then, a Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient analysis was performed to explore the inter reliability of the questionnaire items. The reliability coefficient value is 0.895, greater than 0.8, which indicates that the reliability quality of the research data is high. After we deleted a certain number of the analyzed items, there was no significant increase in the coefficient value, indicating that most of the questions in the questionnaire should be retained. Validity was evaluated through factor analysis, with the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure confirming excellent sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.880). Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 = 689.228, df = 105, p < 0.001), rejecting the null hypothesis of identity correlation matrices and confirming sufficient shared variance among variables to proceed with factor extraction. These results collectively support the scale’s robust psychometric properties in measuring the intended construct (see Table 3). To answer the research questions we asked above, we performed the following steps.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Reliability and validity test results of the scale.

First, Pearson’s correlations were computed to determine whether significant correlations exist between the four alienators and the demotivation scores, followed by a multiple regression to examine which alienator(s) in the scale is (are) predictive of CFL performance as assessed by demotivation.

Second, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed to examine alienating factor attribution differences among high-demotivating (HD), average-demotivating (AD), and low-demotivating (LD) students. The demotivation scores were reported on a scale from 5 to 25 (M = 16.73, SD = 4.88), according to Albalawi and Al-Hoorie’s (2021) demotivation model, which led us to divide participants into three groups based on these demotivation score thresholds (see Table 4).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Academic performance level parameters.

Third, to determine how alienator(s) discourage(s) students from learning Chinese, inductive thematic analysis was used to analyze the data manually, following the procedures suggested by Braun and Clarke (2006): (1) Two researchers, FP and QYL, familiarized themselves with the data gathered through the interviews. (2) The researchers noted the initial codes that could describe the content of the collected data. (3) Codes formed were categorized into opinions based on their significance. After that, the key topics were extracted and grouped together as emergent themes. (4) The whole research team examined all the categorized extracts to determine whether they supported the themes and whether there were conflicts or the themes overlapped. (5) The final phase of report writing involves presenting evidence for each theme, ensuring the validity of the findings by illustrating extracts from participant transcriptions.

A total of 9 codes were identified and divided into four themes (Table 5). To ensure coding reliability, we implemented rigorous interrater checks throughout the analytic process. Any differences in opinions about the themes and codes were discussed together to reach a consensus. The occurrences of each participant on each of these codes were counted (present one/absent zero), and the transcripts associated with these codes were used to interpret the data. The detailed process and results are included in the Results and Discussion section.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Themes, codes and number of occurrences.

Results

Table 6 presents descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations between the CFL university students’ alienators. As shown in the table, among the four demotivators, meaninglessness showed the highest mean, followed by normlessness.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Descriptive statistics and correlations between alienators and demotivation scores (N = 97).

A multiple regression was conducted to address the first research question (RQ2): Which demotivator(s) is (are) predictive of students’ Chinese performance? Results of the multiple regression are presented in Table 7. While social isolation/estrangement was the only statistically significant predictor, β = 0.622, p < 0.001.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Results of regression analysis of alienators for demotivation scores.

A MANOVA was conducted to respond to RQ3: Do students at different L2 demotivation levels react to alienators differently? Pillai’s trace, an inferential test statistic for MANOVAs, revealed that students of different performance levels are influenced differently by the four alienating triggers in the CFL classroom, V = 0.63, F(5, 13) = 5.895, p < 0.001, Using the Bonferroni correction to control for familywise error rate (α = 0.05/4 = 0.0125), separate univariate ANOVAs on the outcome variables revealed a significant effect of achievement meaninglessness, F = 26.72, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.22, social isolation/estrangement, F = 23.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33, and normlessness, F = 32.24, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.26, but a non-significant effect on teacher behavior, powerlessness, F = 15.80, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.25. Figure 2 summarizes the post hoc tests (Bonferroni) results. While the high demotivation (HD) group scored the highest across all four alienators, the attribution gaps appear to be more substantial in social isolation/estrangement, followed by normlessness.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean alienators scores by motivation group. Note. LD = Low Demotivation; AD = Average Demotivation; HD = High Demotivation. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.001. Reference group = LD for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni).

Based on the classical theoretical framework of alienation, the four alienators were divided into two types, namely, external factors and internal factors. In our study, learners’ average ratings for these alienators endorse such a division: internal factors (i.e., meaninglessness, social isolation/estrangement) received higher ratings than external ones (i.e., powerlessness, normlessness; see Table 6). The four demotivators significantly correlate with each other at a moderate to high degree, except for the small-to-medium sized correlation between powerlessness and meaninglessness, r = 0.66, p = 0.25. Many CFL teachers strive to stimulate and maintain learners’ interest in the subject area, and our finding indicates that, to achieve this goal, only teachers’ help may not be effective as expected.

(RQ2) Which alienator(s) is (are) predictive of performance? While all four predictors in this study were positively correlated with the demotivation score, only social isolation/estrangement was found to be significant in the multiple regression model. This indicates that the semi-partial correlation between the demotivation score and meaninglessness, normlessness or powerlessness becomes trivial, after controlling for the effect of social isolation/estrangement. This finding corroborates Maslow’s (1970) theory hierarchy of needs. According to Maslow, social belongingness and love are fundamental human needs, placing them just above physiological needs like food and shelter. Social isolation threatens these needs directly, leading to feelings of alienation because humans are inherently social beings. When someone is socially isolated, they lack the essential connections, support, and sense of belonging that are crucial for psychological well-being which in turn produce motivation deficits that translate into weak performance.

(RQ3) Do students at different L2 demotivation levels react to alienators differently? Eccles et al. (2008) suggested that high-demotivators are particularly at risk of becoming alienated. This has guided us to make the high demotivation (HD) group the reference group in the post hoc tests following the significant MANOVA. As shown in Figure 2, the HD group suffers most across all four demotivators, but such gaps appear most substantial in social isolation/estrangement, followed by meaninglessness, and then powerlessness. No significant difference was found between average demotivators (AD) and low demotivators (LD) in powerlessness and meaninglessness. In the HD group’s ratings across four alienators, we even found that social isolation/estrangement was regarded as the most detrimental element. This finding confirmed antecedent studies conducted (Brown et al., 2003; Morinaj et al., 2017), indicating that more demotivated students tend to attribute their alienation internally (e.g., loneliness). Remarkably, average and low achievers do not differ in their demotivation attributions of meaninglessness and powerlessness, suggesting a diminished role of teachers and learning mindset in accounting for CFL performance gaps. This conclusion is contrary to most East Asia findings (Falout et al., 2009; Sakai and Kikuchi, 2009; Li and Zhou, 2013; Xie et al., 2018), which emphasize teachers’ unchallengeable authority in the classroom.

Contrary to East Asian L2 studies emphasizing pedagogical authoritarianism as a demotivation catalyst, our findings demonstrate participants’ positive affect toward instructors, suggesting that attenuated Chinese learning motivation operates independently of authoritative dynamics. This divergence may be attributed to two contextual particularities: (1) Chinese teachers’ provision of targeted emotional scaffolding for international learners, and (2) the implementation of task-based pedagogies fostering linguistic optimism. The construct of learning mindset further mediates this relationship, encompassing learners’ perceptions of education’s instrumental value across micro–macro contexts: their daily lives, familial contexts, community dynamics, or broader global issues. This belief system functions as a motivational scaffold, driving academic investment through perceived sociopersonal efficacy.

Following the conclusion drawn from our initial study, six students were interviewed to delve deeper into the findings. To present the data, the most representative transcriptions linked to each theme and code are displayed below.

In terms of “Friends,” the fact of ‘No Chinese friends’ related to social isolation were expressed by 6 participants/6. For example, we have heard “I do not make Chinese friends” (LL), “I had a terrible experience with Chinese friends” (CH), “It’s something I’d never thought before. I did not think I could do it. But I have Chinese friends on my WeChat…but I never see them in person” (CB), “I know a Chinese girl. She helped me make a call to my medical insurance claim. After that, we lost touch” (SE) or even “We do not have Chinese in our class, so I have no Chinese friends” (NL). Almost as many participants (5/6) expressed their longing for ‘close friends’. So, for example, we noted “I also want to make some close friends.” (SE), “Making new friends is very difficult. I find it tiring to maintain a new relationship.” (CH), “I do not have close friends at this school. But I have several close friends next to our school” (FS) or even “My roommates are all from other majors, so I have to go to class alone” (CB).

In terms of “HSK,” all participants expressed a feeling of difficult and high demanding. For example, we have heard “We need to pass HSK 4 to graduate, but 4 is too difficult” (SE), “I study computer science. All of our professional courses are taught in English. It’s quite strange that we are required to pass HSK Level 4” (NL), “I think this requirement should be cancelled. I was initially very interested in Chinese, but the exams have made me anxious and bored” (LL) or even “I do not want to take those exams.” (FS) Of these participants, five expressed the high exam fees and costly textbooks, for example: “If you take the HSK 4, you also need to register for the KK (Chinese Proficiency Oral Test). KK is even more expensive” (LL), “You need to finish taking exams from Level 1 to Level 3 first before you can take the Level 4. The Level 4 exam costs 450 yuan. KK…400 yuan or 300 yuan…I cannot remember clearly. They are really expensive.” (CH) Although HSK exams costs a lot, 2 participants complained about the bad exam environment and malfunctioning equipment, for example: “During the exam, everyone will speak at the same time. It gets extremely noisy then. I will be affected by it” (CB), “During one exam, my computer did not want to work. I had to change three computers before I could log in to the exam system. I was really in a bad mood at that time” (LL).

In terms of “Teachers,” 6 participants mentioned their teacher ‘caring and kind’. For example: “My Chinese teacher is very gentle, and I really like her teaching way. We often carry out a lot of interesting activities” (SE), “During Ramadan, I did not feel well and missed several classes. My Chinese teacher did not blame me, and I’m really grateful for that” (NL). Of these, 2 participants reported an image of strict (for example, “Once I wasn’t feeling well and went to the hospital, but I forgot to submit the leave application. Even though I explained the situation to the teacher, I was still marked as absent” (CH), “The teacher of the Cultural Communication course is the strictest. Even if the leave has been approved, it will still be recorded as an absence in the attendance record” (FS).

Regarding the “school work,” three participants spoke about their ‘heavy course load’ during the first 2 years and ‘software operation challenges’ when submit homework. For example: “We already have a lot of professional courses, but there are even more Chinese courses” (FS). “Many courses require the use of Xuexitong to submit homework. But this app does not allow me to submit, always” (CH), “Xuexitong does not have a deadline reminder. Many times, I finish my work but cannot submit it. I have to explain the situation to the TA and ask if she can submit it to the teacher for me” (NL).

Discussion

Alienation factors leading to demotivation among the participants

Powerlessness-mediated factors

Powerlessness, defined as learners’ perceived inability to influence their educational environment, has been widely linked to motivational attrition in L2 acquisition (Dörnyei, 2005), particularly in teacher-centric EFL contexts where pedagogical authority dominates curriculum and assessment (Li and Zhou, 2013; Xie et al., 2018). However, in Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL) settings, while teacher-centered instruction persists, powerlessness manifests distinctively through institutional rather than interpersonal dynamics.

Analysis revealed that powerlessness exerted minimal direct influence on participants’ demotivation. A minority reported instances of overly rigid teaching methods or administrative inflexibility, which fostered perceptions of futility in seeking institutional support. For example, some students described dismissed concerns when attempting to address academic issues, exacerbating feelings of helplessness. Nevertheless, most participants perceived educators and administrators as supportive and approachable. Interviewee SE noted: “…my Chinese teacher is so great. She really helps me a lot.” Similarly, Interviewee CH highlighted compassionate institutional practices: “Our Chinese class is really interesting, we have lots of good topics…the administrator is also kind, once I hurt but I forgot to ask for leave in the system, she let me submit again.

These findings suggest that while systemic rigidity may sporadically trigger powerlessness, positive teacher-student rapport and administrative empathy largely mitigate its demotivational effects in CFL contexts. This contrasts with EFL settings, where power imbalances more directly drive disengagement.

However, our sampled cohort, currently navigating the early stages of cultural adaptation (4 months), appears to occupy a transitional phase between the initial “honeymoon period” and subsequent acculturative challenges. As Ward et al. (2001) noted “The general satisfaction of the sojourners with their new lives, often defined in terms of their well-being.” This developmental trajectory implies potential shifts in motivational dynamics as learners progress through later adaptation phases—a hypothesis necessitating longitudinal validation via stage-specific monitoring (Galchenko and van de Vijver, 2007).

These observations partially align with Dörnyei’s (2005) motivational threshold theory while introducing a critical qualification: when institutional rigidity surpasses learners’ cultural adaptation thresholds, even robust teacher support demonstrates limited efficacy in mitigating systemic powerlessness. This threshold effect underscores the importance of proactive institutional interventions (e.g., flexible curricula, responsive administrative protocols) during early acculturation stages, which may prove more effective than retrospective support in sustaining long-term motivation.

Normlessness-mediated factors

Normlessness, defined as the adoption of illegitimate means to achieve socially sanctioned goals, manifests in Chinese CFL programs through institutionalized prioritization of HSK Level 4 certification as a graduation prerequisite. This policy reduces language learning to a transactional process centered on test performance, compelling pedagogical practices to align rigidly with HSK syllabi. Interview data revealed two normlessness-mediated demotivators: (1) Exam-Oriented Pedagogy: Curricula overwhelmingly focus on HSK preparation, limiting opportunities for communicative competence development. As Interviewee CB noted, Learning Chinese feels meaningless—it’s just endless HSK drills.” (2) High-Stakes Testing Pressures: Institutional emphasis on HSK pass rates incentivizes maladaptive behaviors (e.g., plagiarism, cheating), particularly when learners perceive a misalignment between test demands and their capabilities. These systemic constraints echo prior findings that credential-driven language policies erode intrinsic learning motivation (Liu et al., 2020). Most universities make HSK4 one of the graduation criteria for international students. This thus makes Chinese learning meaningless to the students but for exams.

The institutional emphasis on HSK Level 4 pass rates thus exerts systemic pressure on CFL instructors to prioritize frequent high-stakes testing aligned with exam benchmarks. This credential-focused environment incentivizes learners to adopt maladaptive strategies, including academic misconduct (e.g., plagiarism, cheating), to meet institutional demands. As Rafalides and Hoy (1971) theorized, students may rationalize norm-transgressive behaviors as necessary to fulfill externally imposed achievement thresholds, particularly when perceived capabilities misalign with test rigor. Failure to meet these expectations often results in diminished motivation for Chinese language acquisition, as learners disengage from the learning process amid perceived futility.

Meaninglessness-mediated factors

The analysis revealed minimal influence of meaninglessness on participants’ demotivation, likely attributable to the cohort’s strong instrumental motivation. Notably, most Moroccan students in the study self-funded their education in China, reflecting a deliberate commitment to Chinese language mastery as a pathway to academic advancement and graduate certification. This aligns with Pepanyan et al.’s (2019) assertion that international students’ determination to integrate into host societies often coexists with a pragmatic focus on credential acquisition. Central to this dynamic is learners’ perception of Chinese proficiency as a certification-driven imperative (Liu et al., 2020), wherein examinations—not linguistic engagement—serve as the primary locus of purpose. As Interviewee LL stated: “I must pass HSK4 to graduate; there’s no alternative.” This redefines Gardner and Lambert’s (1972) instrumental motivation framework, prioritizing institutional compliance over communicative competence, and underscores the dominance of credential pragmatism in high-stakes language learning ecologies.

Social isolation/estrangement-mediated factors

Social isolation, characterized by a perceived disconnect from sociocultural integration goals, emerged as a critical demotivator among participants. Interview data highlighted four interrelated factors: (1) emotional loneliness (e.g., longing for familial/friend connections), (2) lack of dependable social networks, (3) identity dissonance (e.g., tension between heritage preservation and host-culture adaptation), and (4) linguistic exclusion. As Berry’s (2005) sociocultural adaptation model posits, international students often grapple with marginalization when balancing cultural retention and assimilation. Interviewee NL’s reflection— “I feel alone here; I miss my close friends back home”—exemplifies this struggle, echoing Trice’s (2004) findings on the psychological toll of social detachment.

While forming bonds with culturally similar peers may alleviate isolation, such reliance can impede Chinese language immersion, as noted by Interviewee SE: “once I hurt my leg, I need one speaking Chinese to help me make a phone call to the insurance, but I cannot…. I even feel that some locals are unfriendly to me.” This linguistic barrier intensifies alienation for Arabic-speaking Moroccan students during crises, compounding feelings of helplessness. As noted by Pepanyan et al. (2019), domestic students may not harbor hostility towards international peers but may struggle to navigate cultural boundaries, inadvertently contributing to a sense of isolation among international students.

These findings underscore social isolation as both a cause and consequence of demotivation in high-stakes language ecologies, where linguistic and cultural marginalization erode learners’ sense of belonging and purpose.

Conclusion

This study pioneers an alienation theory perspective on CFL demotivation, highlighting systemic and sociocultural mediators often overlooked in L2 research. Findings revealed that social isolation and normlessness emerged as the primary mediators of demotivation, driven by two context-specific factors: (1) the systemic pressure of HSK Level 4 certification as a graduation requirement, and (2) the sociocultural challenges of integrating into a localized Chinese academic environment.

The study advances current scholarship in three key ways. First, it addresses a critical gap in L2 demotivation research by applying alienation theory to understudied CFL populations, particularly Moroccan students in non-elite Chinese institutions. Second, it provides empirical validation for the utility of alienation theory in analyzing the interplay of personal, institutional, and sociocultural factors shaping motivational attrition. Third, it illuminates how credential-driven pragmatism (e.g., HSK-focused curricula) and cultural-linguistic marginalization collectively erode intrinsic learning motivation—a dynamic previously underexplored in transnational education contexts. These insights underscore the necessity of re-evaluating high-stakes language policies and fostering inclusive pedagogical practices to mitigate alienation among international CFL learners.

This study yields critical pedagogical implications for Chinese technological universities hosting international students. While educators have implemented supportive measures to enhance CFL engagement—including learner-centered instruction and culturally responsive practices—findings suggest that peer-related social dynamics, rather than teacher inadequacy, constitute the primary driver of alienation and motivational attrition. Despite participants’ widespread appreciation for instructors’ dedication (“My Chinese teacher is incredibly supportive”) and positive evaluations of classroom activities (“Lessons are interactive and fun”), many reported social exclusion and limited peer connections, which fostered resistance to the linguistic environment and diminished learning investment. To address this, institutions should prioritize structured intercultural peer interactions (e.g., mixed-nationality project teams, language partner programs) alongside curricular reforms aligning Chinese courses with students’ professional needs (e.g., discipline-specific vocabulary modules). Such initiatives would mitigate social isolation while contextualizing language acquisition within learners’ academic trajectories, thereby bridging the gap between linguistic and vocational objectives.

This study acknowledges three primary limitations. First, the exclusive focus on Moroccan students at a single Chinese university limits the ecological validity of findings, restricting generalizability to broader CFL contexts. Replicating this work across institutions and national cohorts would clarify how sociocultural and institutional variables modulate alienation dynamics. Second, reliance on self-reported data risks common-method bias, potentially inflating correlations between measured constructs. Future studies should integrate multimodal assessments—such as teacher evaluations, behavioral tracking, and institutional performance metrics—to triangulate alienation manifestations. Third, participants’ limited immersion duration (4 months) may capture transient motivational fluctuations rather than stabilized patterns. Longitudinal designs tracking learners across proficiency milestones (e.g., HSK Level 4 to 6 transitions) are needed to disentangle temporal trajectories of demotivation.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the ethics committee of China Jiliang University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

FP: Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. QL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by Zhejiang Province “14th Five-Year Plan” Higher Education Teaching Reform Project (Grant No. JGBA2024228), Research Project of the China Association of Higher Education under the Higher Education Science Research Program (Grant No. 24LX0420) and “14th Five-Year Plan” Special Project for Teacher Research and Development, China Tao Xingzhi Research Association (Grant No. ZTHJS2024131).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Correction note

This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the scientific content of the article.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Akay, C. (2017). Turkish high school students’ English demotivation and their seeking for remotivation: a mixed method research. Engl. Lang. Teach. 10, 107–122. doi: 10.5539/elt.v10n8p107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alamer, A., Morin, A. J. S., Alrabai, F., and Alharfi, A. (2023). Introducing the basic psychological needs frustration in second language scale (BPNF-L2): examining its factor structure and effect on L2 motivation and achievement. Acta Psychol. 240:104021. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2023.104021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Albalawi, F. H., and Al-Hoorie, A. H. (2021). From demotivation to remotivation: a mixed-methods investigation. SAGE Open 11, 1–14. doi: 10.1177/21582440211041101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ancis, J. R., Sedlacek, W. E., and Mohr, J. J. (2000). Student perceptions of campus cultural climate by race. J. Couns. Dev. 78, 180–185. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb02576.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., and Pagani, L. (2009). Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in school: relationship to dropout. J. Sch. Health 79, 408–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00428.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barua, S. (2022). Language anxiety variables and their negative effects on SLA: a psychosocial reality in Bangladesh. Int. J. Res. Innov. Soc. Sci. 6, 400–408. doi: 10.47772/ijriss.2022.6816

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, J. W. (2005). Acculturation: living successfully in two cultures. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 29, 697–712. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.07.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Biasco, F., Goodwin, E. A., and Vitale, K. L. (2001). College students’ attitudes toward racial discrimination. Coll. Stud. J. 35, 523–528.

Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bribesh, S. (2024). Demotivation factors in L2 learning: a case study of graduate students. J. Engl. Lang. Teach. Appl. Linguist. 6, 69–74. doi: 10.32996/jeltal.2024.6.1.9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, M. R., Higgins, K., and Paulsen, K. (2003). Adolescent alienation: what is it and what can educators do about it? Interv. Sch. Clin. 39, 3–9. doi: 10.1177/10534512030390010101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, G. N. (1993). Taking the ‘de’ out of demotivation. Lang. Learn. J. 7, 13–16. doi: 10.1080/09571739385200051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dakhi, A. S. (2020). The learn from home and alienation. Int. J. Sci. Technol. Manag. 1, 133–137. doi: 10.46729/ijstm.v1i2.33

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, D. G. (1961). Alienation: its meaning and measurement. Am. Sociol. Rev. 26, 753–758. doi: 10.2307/2090204

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J., Magdalena, A. F., and Saito, K. (2019). The effect of perception of teacher characteristics on Spanish EFL learners’ anxiety and enjoyment. Mod. Lang. J. 103, 412–427. doi: 10.1111/modl.12555

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Z. (1998) Demotivation in foreign language learning [Conference paper]. TESOL ‘98 Congress, Seattle, WA, United States

Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Z. (2001). Teaching and researching motivation: Applied linguistics in action. 1st ed. New York: Pearson Education ESL.

Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Alfeld, C. A. (2007). “Not you! Not here! Not now” in Approaches to positive youth development. eds. R. K. Silbereisen and R. M. Lerner (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 133–156.

Google Scholar

Eccles, J., Brown, B. V., and Templeton, J. (2008). “A developmental framework for selecting indicators of well-being during the adolescent and young adult years” in Key indicators of child and youth well-being: Completing the picture. ed. B. V. Brown. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. 197–236.

Google Scholar

Escudero, P., Escobar, G. P., Diskin-Holdaway, C., and Hajek, J. (2024). Enhancing heritage and additional language learning in the preschool years: Longitudinal implementation of the little multilingual minds program. OSF Preprints. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/rvjcg

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Falout, J., Elwood, J. A., and Hood, M. (2009). Demotivation: affective states and learning outcomes. System 37, 403–417. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2009.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Falout, J., and Maruyama, M. (2004). A comparative study of proficiency and learner demotivation. Lang. Teach. 28, 3–9.

Google Scholar

Galbo, J. J. (1980). It bears repeating: adolescent alienation in secondary schools: a literature review. High School J. 64, 26–31.

Google Scholar

Galchenko, I., and van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). The role of perceived cultural distance in the acculturation of exchange students in Russia. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 31, 181–197. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2006.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, R. C., and Lambert, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and motivation in second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers.

Google Scholar

Ghadirzadeh, R., Hashtroudi, F. P., and Shokri, O. (2012). Demotivating factors for English language learning among university students. J. Soc. Sci. 8, 189–195. doi: 10.3844/jssp.2012.189.195

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjar, A., Backes, S., and Gysin, S. (2015). School alienation, patriarchal gender-role orientations and the lower educational success of boys. A mixed-method study. Masculinities Soc. Change 4, 85–116. doi: 10.4471/mcs.2014.61

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjar, A., and Lupatsch, J. (2010). Der Schul(miss)erfolg der Jungen. Kölner Z. Soziol. Sozialpsychol. 62, 599–622. doi: 10.1007/s11577-010-0116-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanassab, S. (2006). Diversity, international students, and perceived discrimination: implications for educators and counselors. J. Stud. Int. Educ. 10, 157–172. doi: 10.1177/1028315305283051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hascher, T., and Hadjar, A. (2017). School alienation – A review of theoretical approaches and educational research. Educational Research. 60, 171–188. doi: 10.1080/00131881.2018.1443021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hascher, T., and Hagenauer, G. (2010). Alienation from school. Int. J. Educ. Res. 49, 220–232. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2011.03.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, W. X., and Cai, J. T. (2010). Construction of model of demotivation in english learning. For. Lang. Educ. 31, 41–49. doi: 10.16362/j.cnki.cn61-1023/h.2010.03.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iftanti, E., Imelda, I., and Yunita, W. (2023). Uncovering EFL learners’ demotivation towards English online learning during the Covid-19 pandemic in Indonesia. Stud. Engl. Lang. Educ. 10, 96–116. doi: 10.24815/siele.v10i1.25401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessor, R., and Jessor, S. (1977). Problem behavior and psychosocial development. New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Jin, Y., and Zhang, L. J. (2018). The dimensions of foreign language classroom enjoyment and their effect on foreign language achievement. Int. J. Bilingual Educ. Bilingualism 24, 948–962. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2018.1526253

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karaca, M., and Inan, S. (2020). A measure of possible sources of demotivation in L2 writing: a scale development and validation study. Assess. Writing 43:100438. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2019.100438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, M. A., Khan, M. R., and Chughtai, I. A. (2020). ESL learners’ sense of alienation: an exploratory mixed method research on the role of ESL teachers’ remarking practices. Engl. Lang. Teach. 13:59. doi: 10.5539/elt.v13n11p59

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuchi, K. (2009). Listening to our learners’ voices: what demotivates Japanese high school students? Lang. Teach. Res. 13, 453–471. doi: 10.1177/1362168809341520

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuchi, K. (2019). Motivation and demotivation over two years: a case study of English language learners in Japan. Stud. Second Lang. Learn. Teach. 9, 157–175. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2019.9.1.7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, T. Y., and Kim, Y. K. (2015). Elderly Korean learners’ participation in english learning through lifelong education: focusing on motivation and demotivation. Educ. Gerontol. 41, 120–135. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2014.929345

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, T. Y., Kim, Y., and Kim, J. Y. (2018). A qualitative inquiry on EFL learning demotivation and resilience: a study of primary and secondary EFL students in South Korea. Asia Pac. Educ. Res. 27, 55–64. doi: 10.1007/s40299-017-0365-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimova, B. (2025). Use of machine translation in foreign language education. Cogent Arts Humanit. 12, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/23311983.2025.2491183

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klomegah, R. Y. (2006). Social factors relating to alienation experienced by international students in the United States. Coll. Stud. J. 40, 303–315.

Google Scholar

Lalwani, A., Green, W. M., and Mansour, K. R. H. (2023). How campus alienation exacerbated international students’ difficulties in accessing campus services remotely during COVID-19: notes on policy and programming. Am. Behav. Sci. 67, 1574–1590. doi: 10.1177/00027642221118257

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. J., and Rice, C. (2007). Welcome to America? International student perceptions of discrimination. High. Educ. 53, 381–409. doi: 10.1007/s10734-005-4508-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L. (2013). A study on internal factors of Chinese college EFL learners’ demotivation. J. PLA Univ. For. Lang. 36, 65–69. doi: 10.1002-722X(2013)02-0065-05

Google Scholar

Li, L., and Zhou, C. (2013). Different faces of demotivation: a comparative study on Chinese and Korean college EFL learners’ demotivators. J. Appl. Sci. 13, 800–809. doi: 10.3923/jas.2013.800.809

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., and Han, X. (2022). Exploring senior high school students’ English academic resilience in the Chinese context. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 45, 49–68. doi: 10.1515/CJAL-2022-0105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., and Song, X. (2021). Exploring flow in young Chinese EFL learners’ online English learning activities. System 96:102425. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2020.102425

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, M., Zheng, Y., Ma, X., and Wei, Y. (2020). Sustaining multilingualism in Chinese universities: Uzbekistani students’ demotivation while learning Chinese. Sustain. For. 12, 1–16. doi: 10.3390/su12187570

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and personality. 2nd Edn. New York: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Moiinvaziri, M., and Razmjoo, S. A. (2014). Demotivating factors affecting undergraduate learners of non-english majors studying general english: a case of Iranian EFL context. J. Teach. Lang. Skills 5, 41–61. doi: 10.22099/JTLS.2014.1859

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morinaj, J., Scharf, J., Grecu, A. L., Hadjar, A., Hascher, T., and Marcin, K. (2017). School alienation: a construct validation study. Front Learn. Res. 5:298. doi: 10.14786/FLR.V5I2.298

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newmann, F. (1981). Reducing student alienation in high schools: implications of theory. Harv. Educ. Rev. 51, 546–564. doi: 10.17763/haer.51.4.xj67887u87l5t66t

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nottingham, C. R., Rosen, D. H., and Parks, C. (1992). Psychological well-being among African American university students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 33, 356–362.

Google Scholar

Oxford, R., and Shearin, J. (1994). Language learning motivation: expanding the theoretical framework. Mod. Lang. J. 78, 12–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1994.tb02011.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pabodha, G., and Abeywickrama, R. (2021). Language alienation of Sri Lankan students studying overseas: an empirical study. Sabaragamuwa Univ. J. 19, 48–59. doi: 10.4038/suslj.v19i1.7773

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pepanyan, M., Meacham, S., and Logan, S. (2019). International students’ alienation in a US higher education institution. J. Multicult. Educ. 13, 122–139. doi: 10.1108/JME-10-2017-0057

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rafalides, M., and Hoy, W. K. (1971). Student sense of alienation and pupil control orientation of high schools. High Sch. J. 55, 101–111.

Google Scholar

Rashidi, N., Rahimi, M., and Alimorad, Z. (2014). Iranian university English learners’ discursive demotivation construction. Iran. J. Lang. Teach. Res. 2, 35–49. doi: 10.30466/ijltr.2014.20412

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakai, H., and Kikuchi, K. (2009). An analysis of demotivators in the EFL classroom. System 37, 57–69. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2008.09.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarfraz, H. (1997). Alienation: a theoretical overview. Pak. J. Psychol. Res. 12, 45–60.

Google Scholar

Schram, J. L., and Lauver, P. J. (1988). Alienation in international students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 29, 146–150.

Google Scholar

Seeman, M. (1991). “Alienation and anomie” in Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. eds. J. P. Robinson, P. R. Saver, and L. S. Wrightsman, vol. 1. 1st ed (San Diego: Academic Press), 291–371.

Google Scholar

Sherry, M., Thomas, P., and Chui, W. H. (2010). International students: a vulnerable student population. High. Educ. 60, 33–46. doi: 10.1007/s10734-009-9284-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solórzano, D., Ceja, M., and Yosso, T. (2000). Critical race theory, racial microaggressions, and campus racial climate: the experiences of African American college students. J. Negro Educ. 69, 60–73.

Google Scholar

Song, B. S., and Kim, T. Y. (2017). The dynamics of demotivation and remotivation among Korean high school EFL students. System 65, 90–103. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2016.12.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trang, T. T. T., and Baldauf, R. B. (2007). Demotivation: understanding resistance to English language learning-the case of Vietnamese students. J. Asia TEFL 4, 79–105.

Google Scholar

Trice, A. G. (2004). Mixing it up: international graduate students' social interactions with American students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 45, 671–687. doi: 10.1353/csd.2004.0074

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, C., Bochner, S., and Furnham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture shock. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Weda, S. (2018). Demotivational teaching practices in EFL classroom: perceptions of English among Indonesian learners. Asian EFL J. 20, 400–414.

Google Scholar

Xie, J., Wei, T., Zeng, Y., Lobsenz, J., and Chen, X. (2018). Learner perceptions of demotivators in the EFL classroom: experiences of failure on learning outcomes. J. Asia TEFL 15, 491–501. doi: 10.18823/asiatefl.2018.15.2.17.491

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L. (2024). The influence of international online distance learning experiences on L2 motivation and ideal L2 selves of master students in language education. Region Educ. Res. Rev. 6:252. doi: 10.32629/rerr.v6i9.3100

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, C. (2012). A comparative study of Chinese and Japanese college EFL learners’ demotivators. J. Ningbo Univ. 3, 51–55.

Google Scholar

Zsuzsanna, R. (1996). Demotivation in learning English among secondary school students in Budapest. Budapest: ELTE Angol Alkalmazott Nyelvészeti Tanszék.

Google Scholar

Keywords: L2 demotivation, Moroccan CFL learners, alienation theory, mediational factors, formulating mechanism

Citation: Peng F and Lou Q (2025) Investigating the demotivation of Moroccan CFL learners from an alienation theory perspective. Front. Psychol. 16:1471486. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1471486

Received: 27 July 2024; Accepted: 21 May 2025;
Published: 03 June 2025;
Corrected: 04 June 2025.

Edited by:

Douglas F. Kauffman, Consultant, Greater Boston, United States

Reviewed by:

Huajie Shen, Fujian University of Technology, China
Gizem Mutlu Gülbak, Biruni University, Türkiye

Copyright © 2025 Peng and Lou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fei Peng, cGVuZ2ZlaUBjamx1LmVkdS5jbg==; Qiuyue LoucDIzNTkwNDUzMDE3QGNqbHUuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.