ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living

Sec. Sports Science, Technology and Engineering

Volume 7 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/fspor.2025.1482976

Cognitive costs in motor-cognitive performance assessments depend on movement complexity and cognitive task design

Provisionally accepted
  • 1Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Hesse, Germany
  • 2Lunex University, Differdange, Luxembourg

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

ObjectiveCognitive performance is typically assessed using computer-based tests where participants respond via a simple upper extremity motor task such as a button press. This type of assessment has been criticised for its low ecological validity that does not consider the interaction between cognitive and more complex motor skills in sports and everyday life situations, which results in motor-cognitive interference. Consequently, motor-cognitive assessments integrating a more complex motor response into a cognitive test have gained popularity. However, the cognitive costs in motor-cognitive tests due to the interference of cognitive and motor processes have not yet been determined. Therefore, this study aimed to quantify the cognitive costs in motor-cognitive tests.MethodsThirty-three healthy athletes performed four identical cognitive tests (simple reaction, choice-reaction, working memory, cognitive flexibility) in a cognitive and motor-cognitive setting. For the cognitive task, participants performed a computer-based cognitive assessment by responding with a button press on a keyboard. In the motor-cognitive condition, participants conducted a stepping movement which was identified by a Light Detection and Ranging (LiDAR) system integrated into motor-cognitive testing and training technology (SKILLCOURT®). Cognitive costs were determined by comparing reaction time and error rate between conditions (cognitive vs motor-cognitive) while controlling for differences in measurement technology, neuromuscular conduction delay, and movement amplitude. Correlation analyses quantified the relationship between cognitive and motor-cognitive performance. ResultsThere were cognitive costs, as indicated by slower reaction times in the motor-cognitive test, for the choice-reaction (p=0.014) and working memory (p<0.001) tests. There were inverse cognitive costs, denoted by faster reactions, in the motor-cognitive compared with the cognitive condition for the cognitive flexibility test (p<0.001). There were strong correlations for the simple-reaction (r31=0.79, p<0.001), choice-reaction (r31=0.60, p<0.001), and cognitive flexibility (r28=0.83, p<0.001) tests. The working memory task revealed a moderate correlation (r31=0.46, p=0.009). ConclusionThe results confirm the presence of cognitive costs in motor-cognitive assessments. The type of motor response and test design influence cognitive costs and test performance and can even result in inverse cognitive costs during motor-cognitive tasks. This must be considered when interpreting motor-cognitive tests and suggest that computer-based assessments cannot simply be replaced by motor-cognitive alternatives.

Keywords: Executive Function, motor-cognitive testing, Reaction Time, SKILLCOURT, motorcognitive interference Formatted: Space Before: 0 pt, After: 0 pt Formatted: Indent: First line: 0.49", Space After: 0 pt

Received: 19 Aug 2024; Accepted: 20 May 2025.

Copyright: © 2025 Erdogan, Vogt, Giesche, Friebe, Banzer, Mierau and Hülsdünker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Thorben Hülsdünker, Lunex University, Differdange, Luxembourg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.