ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 13 June 2025

Sec. Sport Psychology

Volume 7 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2025.1501123

This article is part of the Research TopicDeterminants of Achievement in Top SportView all 21 articles

Riding with the flow: study on intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking among Italian motorcycle racers


Paolo Mancin
Paolo Mancin1*Maria Stefania IonelMaria Stefania Ionel2Silvia Cerea,Silvia Cerea1,3Eleonora F. M. RivaEleonora F. M. Riva4Martina RapisardaMartina Rapisarda1Marta Ghisi,
Marta Ghisi1,5
  • 1Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Sport and Exercise Psychology Group, RIDDLE Lab, Department of Psychology, Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania
  • 3Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 4Department of Cultural Heritage and Environment, University of Milano, Milano, Italy
  • 5U.O.C. Hospital Psychology, University-Hospital of Padova, Padova, Italy

Background: There is a limited body of research investigating psychological dimensions involved in motorcycle racing. Exploring intrinsic motivation becomes crucial since it is associated with high performance and sport persistence. Dispositional flow and sensation seeking, psychological traits involved in motorcycle practice, may exert a significant influence on intrinsic motivation. The current study aimed (1) to assess whether higher levels of intrinsic motivation characterize motorcycle racers participating in higher levels of competition, while no clear hypotheses were made for dispositional flow and sensation seeking, and (2) to assess how dispositional flow and sensation seeking would relate with intrinsic motivation in a sample of Italian motorcycle racers.

Method: Data was collected from 75 motorcycle racers, self-identified as men (age 39.44 ± 13.07 years), registered in the Italian Federation of Motorcycling: 43 competed at National/International level and 32 at Regional level as their highest level.

Results: Motorcycle racers competing at Regional level and National/International level did not differ in psychological dimensions. Moreover, dispositional flow was positively associated with all dimensions of intrinsic motivation, while sensation seeking was associated only with intrinsic motivation to stimulate. Finally, intrinsic motivation to stimulate was associated with both psychological dimensions within a hierarchical regression model.

Conclusion: These findings highlighted that motorcycle racing could be intrinsically rewarding and could enable riders to experience flow and to reach intense emotions regardless of the level of the competition. Furthermore, dispositional flow emerged as significantly associated with intrinsic motivation to sport practice in motorcycle racers, while sensation seeking played a marginal role.

1 Introduction

Motorcycle racing is a sport that requires the athlete to interact with a vehicle and a specific track. It comprises multiple disciplines (e.g., motocross, enduro and motorally, trial, speedway, supermoto or motard, and road racing) that are distinguished by specific requirements of the motorcycle and characteristics of the track (e.g., street legal or speed-based motorcycle; natural terrain or asphalt-based surface) (1, 2).

Most studies on motorcycle racing focused on bike functions and human interaction with it. Consequently, there is a paucity of studies focusing on psychological variables (3). Addressing this gap would enable scholars and practitioners to better understand the characteristics of this sport and how to improve the performance of these athletes.

Among the psychological dimensions involved in sport practice, motivation and, more specifically, intrinsic motivation may have important implications in motorcycle racing. Drawing from the Self-Determination Model, intrinsic motivation can be defined as engaging in an activity for the inherent pleasure and satisfaction it provides from being involved in it, and could be placed within a continuum that includes other forms of motivations, namely, extrinsic motivation (i.e., being motivated by external feedback or rewards) and amotivation (i.e., the absence of motivation) (46). Intrinsic motivation is also categorized into three different forms: intrinsic motivation to know (i.e., motivation through learning, exploring, or trying to understand something new), to accomplish (i.e., motivated to accomplish or create something), and to stimulate (i.e., motivated to experience fun, excitement, and stimulation) (7, 8). Intrinsic motivation appears as fundamental in sport practice since it is associated with persistence in sport practice (9), with sport commitment (such as enthusiastic commitment) (10), and with high performance in athletes of various disciplines (11, 12). Relevant finding from previous research indicates that athletes with high levels of intrinsic motivation demonstrate a greater likelihood of achieving higher performance levels in individual sports, whereas athletes characterized by fear of failure exhibit a decreased likelihood (13). Within the field of motorcycle racing, intrinsic motivation could be associated with other psychological dimensions, such as flow and sensation seeking.

Flow could be defined as a psychological and physiological state, highly pleasant and rewarding, associated with complete involvement in the activity performed and the sensation of “clicking into place” (14). According to its definition, flow and intrinsic motivation are often considered as two strongly associated constructs (15): Indeed, the former has been conceptualized as an intrinsically rewarding experience for athletes (16). In addition, these two constructs were found to be positively correlated among motorcycle road racers (17). Flow is mainly considered to be a state dimension, contingent on a specific situation. However, the dispositional flow is also described as the predisposition to experience flow, measured by how frequently it occurs to people in a particular domain (e.g., sport performance in motorcycling) (18). There are different models of flow, but the most referred one involves prerequisites and characteristics of flow (18). The prerequisites include challenge-skills balance (i.e., situations that are perceived as challenging by the athletes, and that require extending beyond typical capabilities), clear goals about the task to perform, and unambiguous feedback, that notify the athletes about the rightness of their progress toward the goal. Characteristics that describe what the athlete experiences during flow include total concentration on the task at hand (i.e., complete focus on the task and avoidance of internal or external distractions), action-awareness merging (i.e., feeling at one with the task performed), sense of control (i.e., feeling of being in total control of the task performed), loss of consciousness (i.e., decreased sense of self- and social evaluation), and transformation of time (i.e., experiencing time as going faster or slower than usual) (18). Finally, the last characteristic of flow is autotelic experience, which refers to flow as an intrinsically rewarding experience (18). As described for intrinsic motivation, there is a positive association between flow and high levels of performance (1922). Thus, flow could be a relevant experience in sport performance among motorcycle racers too.

Lastly, sensation seeking, characterized by the desire to experience novel and thrilling sensations with a willingness to accept potential physical, financial, and social risks solely for the sake of the experience itself (23), may also play a role in motorcycle practice. It is noteworthy that motorcycle racing is frequently classified as an extreme/high-risk sport (24). Extreme sports are defined as “independent adventure activities where a mismanaged mistake or accident is most likely to result in death” (25). They typically involve competitive activities that expose participants to unconventional physical and mental challenges such as speed, height, depth, or natural forces (24), elements that are commonly encountered in various motorcycle disciplines. For example, motocross involves high speed competitions, which take place on natural terrains characterized by dirt, sharp turns, high hills. Enduro competitions could take place on offroad tracks (mule tracks, country roads) and require the racer to face unexpected obstacles and unknown routes (1). J. Olivera Betrán and A. Olivera Betrán (26) proposed a revised taxonomy for extreme sports, classifying motocross, enduro, or rallies under the category of ‘fire wheeled engines’. A meta-analysis examining the relation between personality traits and participation in high-risk sports revealed that sensation seeking is not just the most investigated trait but also has a large effect size (Hedges’ g = 0.80, p < .001) associated with participation in such sports (27). Moreover, several findings have supported the hypothesis that sensation seeking might play a significant role in intrinsically motivating motorcycle racing athletes. Notably, studies have revealed that non-competitive motorcycle riders reported recreation and sensation seeking as primary motivations for practicing this activity (28). Furthermore, previous research has indicated that sensation seeking, including its dimension of thrill seeking, emerged as a predictor for engagement in extreme sports (29) and motorsport involvement (30). Accordingly, individuals involved in high-risk sports (i.e., surfing) demonstrated high levels of both sensation seeking and intrinsic motivation when compared to those involved in low-risk sports (i.e., golfing) (31). Alternatively, the impact of sensation seeking on motivation may exhibit similarities to the potential influence of flow in motorcycle racers. In fact, Schüler and Pfenninger (32) have suggested that sensation seeking and flow might overlap due to their shared pursuit of extraordinary emotions, as reflected in their respective definitions. In accordance with this notion, flow has been identified as a significant predictor of underestimation of risk perception among kayakers (i.e., another extreme sport) (32) and has demonstrated a positive influence on speeding behaviors among riders of heavy motorcycles in Taiwan (33).

The current body of research on motorcycle racing is notably limited, particularly in terms of investigating the interplay between psychological variables involved in this sport. Moreover, existing studies that have explored this topic have predominantly focused on non-competitive motorcyclists (28, 3335); thus, rendering impossible the generalizability of findings to motorcycle athletes. Consequently, there is a distinct need for additional studies that specifically examine the psychological dynamics of motorcycle racing while considering the unique attributes of competitive motorcycle athletes.

Furthermore, there is a significant gap in the literature regarding the examination of intrinsic motivation among motorcycle racers and its association with flow and sensation seeking. To address this gap, the present study aimed to compare motorcycle racers from various disciplines, categorized according to their highest self-reported level of competitions performed, in terms of intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow experienced during motorcycling, and sensation seeking. Secondly, the study aimed to explore how dispositional flow and sensation seeking would be associated with intrinsic motivation. The hypotheses can be outlined as follows:

H1: individuals performing at the National/International level in motorcycle racing were expected to exhibit higher levels of intrinsic motivation compared to those at the Regional level.

Based on previous research highlighting that intrinsic motivation reinforces persistence in sport practice in various disciplines (9, 13), it can be inferred that intrinsic motivation could characterize engagement at a high level of competition in motorcycle racing. Indeed, Regional level competitions, common starting point for motorcycle racers interested in this sport, are generally more accessible, take place at local circuits with easier tracks and shorter durations, receive minimal media coverage, and are commonly self-funded by participants. Conversely, National and International competitions are more demanding, take place in specific locations with advanced circuits and longer durations, receive greater media coverage, and require greater financial support. Hence, motorcycle racers participating in these competitions are usually more skilled and require a team and staff supporting them preparing for the competition (36).

No specific hypotheses were formulated regarding the associations of flow and sensation seeking with levels of competition in this study.

H2: both dispositional flow and sensation seeking were expected to predict levels of intrinsic motivation in motorcycle racers equally.

Consistent with previous findings (17, 29, 30, 32, 33), it was hypothesized that both dispositional flow and sensation seeking should emerge as significantly associated with engagement in motorcycle practice. However, it remained unknown whether these factors hold equal relevance when considered together and whether their associations are similar among motorcycle athletes, as no previous studies have specifically addressed these aspects.

By undertaking this study, we aimed to better understand the psychological dimensions involved in motorcycle racing and their interrelations. Additionally, we made a first attempt in understanding whether psychological models applied in other individual sports were also applicable to motorcycle racing. Finally, this research could contribute to understand the psychological variables associated with motorcycle racing and how to foster persistence in motorcycle sport practice.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

In determining the sample size needed to detect medium effect size, we conducted an a-priori power analysis using G-Power software (version 3.1), and followed the approach of Faul et al. (37). Our analysis indicated that the total number of participants needed for detecting such effect (ƒ2 = .15), for a linear multiple regression that includes two predictors, with a power of.80, was 68 participants.

We collected data from 75 motorcycle racers (43 competed at National/International level and 32 Regional level as their highest level) who were registered in the Italian Federation of Motorcycling, self-identified as competitors, and volunteered to respond to our invitation (see Procedure). All of them self-identified as men. The sample included ages between 18 and 65 (M = 39.44, SD = 13.07), and education ranged between 8 and 18 years (M = 13.77; SD = 2.90). Experience in motorcycling ranged between 6 and 540 months (M = 199.07, SD = 135.82). The different disciplines of motorcycle racing the participants were involved in included: enduro (36%), motocross (33.3%), velocity (road racing) (24%), trial (2.7%), supermoto (1.3%), speedway (1.3%), and motorally (1.3%). Regarding marital status, 17.3% of participants were single, 26.7% were in a relationship, 42.7% were married or cohabitants, and 9.3% were separated or divorced. As for current occupation, 60 participants had a job (80%), 4 participants (5.3%) were students, and the rest did not identify in the previous categories.

2.2 Measures

A brief informative form was used to assess socio-demographic (gender, age, education, marital status, occupation) and sport-related (experience in motorcycling, the highest level of competition performed, type of motorcycle sport practiced) information of each participant.

Sport Motivation Scale (SMS) (7, 8) assesses individuals’ level of motivation towards sport. The seven factors (three types of intrinsic motivation, four types of extrinsic motivation, and amotivation) were measured with 28 items rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (does not correspond at all) to 7 (corresponds completely). For the purposes of the study, we decided to use only the three subscales that assess intrinsic motivation. Internal consistencies for the three selected factors were (Cronbach’s alpha): α = .86 for SMS – intrinsic to know, α = .78 for SMS – intrinsic to accomplish, α = .68 for SMS – intrinsic to stimulate.

Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) (38, 39) measures an individual’s predisposition to experience flow as a stable characteristic or a trait of their personality. Since participants are required to think about a general episode in which they experienced flow, we specifically asked individuals to reflect on an episode related to motorcycle practice. The nine factors were measured with 36 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). For the objective of the study, only the total DFS-2 score was selected; it showed excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .94).

Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS) (40, 41) assesses the personality trait based on seeking new, complex, and intense sensations and the risk of experiencing them. The four dimensions (experience seeking, boredom susceptibility, thrill and adventure seeking, disinhibition) were measured with 8 items rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Overall, the BSSS allows to compute a general score of sensation seeking, that was ultimately utilized for the analyses. Internal consistency reliability as measured with Cronbach’s alpha was .57.

2.3 Procedure

Participants affiliated with the Italian Federation of Motorcycling were recruited through a newsletter sent to all enrolled members. To be eligible for the study, participants needed to be at least 18 years old and to be motorcycle racers. Each participant received an online link consisting of an informed consent, a socio-demographic information schedule, and a battery of self-report questionnaires. The informed consent instructed participants about the purposes of the study, the voluntary nature of their participation, and the possibility to withdraw from the study at any time without penalty.

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Psychological Research of the University of Padova.

2.4 Data analysis

All the statistical analyses were performed with SPSS, version 27.0 for Windows. First of all, we estimated the descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations for all the measured variables); then, a t-test was conducted to compare motorcycle racers that performed at Regional level and at National/International level. Cohen’s d was used to assess the effect sizes and are considered as small (d = .20), medium (d = .50), and large (d = .80) (42). Finally, based on correlational analyses, we employed hierarchical regression, utilizing the SMS scores as dependent variables and the DFS-2 total score and the BSSS total score as independent variables.

3 Results

3.1 Group comparisons between motorcycle racers

After performing a t-test, differences in age, motorcycle expertise (expressed in months), intrinsic motivation to know, to experience, and to accomplish (the scores at three subscales of the SMS), dispositional flow (the DFS-2 total score), and sensation seeking (the BSSS total score) did not emerge as significant between motorcycle racers that competed at Regional level (n = 32) and at National/International level (n = 43) (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparisons between motorcycle racers that compete at regional level and national/international level on age, years of motorcycle expertise, intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking.

3.2 Associations among intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking

Since motorcycle racers did not differ on the above-mentioned variables, we considered the whole sample for further analyses (n = 75). As shown in Table 2, the subscales “intrinsic motivation to know” and “intrinsic motivation to accomplish” of the SMS were positively associated with dispositional flow (respectively, r = .25; p = .03; and r = .40; p < .001), but not with sensation seeking (respectively, r = .12; p = .30; and r = .18; p = .13). On the other hand, the subscale ‘intrinsic motivation to stimulate’ showed a moderate correlation with dispositional flow (r = .43; p < .001) and a weak correlation with sensation seeking (r = .26; p = .02).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bivariate correlation between age, years of motorcycle expertise, intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking.

As a result, only the latter subscale of the SMS was used as a dependent variable for the hierarchical regression model (Table 3). In Step 1 of the analysis, the inclusion of dispositional flow (the DFS-2 total score) as a predictor yielded a significant positive relation and accounted for 18.4% of the variance in the dependent variable. In Step 2, the addition of sensation seeking (the BSSS total score) resulted in a significant change (ΔR2 = 4.4; F change = 4.07; p = .047). In the final model, both independent variables emerged as significant predictors and explained 22.7% of the variance in the subscale ‘intrinsic motivation to stimulate’ of the SMS.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Hierarchical regression model of dispositional flow and sensation seeking on intrinsic motivation to stimulate.

4 Discussion

Given the limited research on psychological variables in motorcycle racers (3), the current study aimed to investigate the relations between intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking in a sample of Italian motorcycle racers enrolled within the Italian Federation of Motorcycling. First, motorcycle racers who competed at Regional level were compared with motorcycle racers who competed at National/International level, focusing on sociodemographic, sport-related, and psychological variables. Subsequently, the associations between intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking were explored.

Concerning the first aim, no significant differences were observed among motorcycle racers. Thus, regardless of their level of competition, they exhibited similar levels of intrinsic motivation in practicing motorcycle racing. Likewise, comparable levels of dispositional flow and sensation seeking were reported. This finding contradicted previous studies (9, 13) that identified higher levels of intrinsic motivation as being relevant for involvement in higher levels of competitions in other sports.

A possible explanation for this discrepancy was that motorcycling itself may be intrinsically rewarding per se, regardless of the level of competition in which individuals perform. In other words, the enjoyment and satisfaction derived from the activity of motorcycling may be a strong motivating factor for individuals, regardless of their competitive level. In contrast with other sports such as basketball, soccer, and volleyball (9) or rowing, mountain biking, swimming, alpine skiing, or equestrian vaulting (13), motorcycle racing might require higher levels of intrinsic motivation to persist even at lower levels of competition. This could be attributed to the unique characteristics of motorcycle racing, which carries a higher risk of fatal injuries and accidents (28, 35). The heightened risk associated with motorcycle racing may intensify the need for strong intrinsic motivation to overcome the inherent challenges and dangers of this sport. This explanation could further support why motorcycle racers did not differ on flow and sensation seeking too. Given the lack of a control group, we cannot draw conclusions regarding whether motorcycle athletes exhibited higher levels of these psychological dimensions compared to athletes in other sports or the general population, which would have further corroborated this hypothesis.

Another possible explanation for these findings could be found in how performance levels have been investigated. In the current study, we relied on a self-report item, asking for the highest level of competition performed, while no objective evaluations were included. Thus, participants could have misinterpreted the item. Moreover, it is possible that some respondents may occasionally engage in high level competitions, such as National and International ones, while being mostly involved in Regional level competitions. This hypothesis could be further supported by lack of differences in age and years of experience. Hence, the two groups identified for the analyses may have areas of overlap.

Similarly, intrinsic motivation, flow, and sensation seeking were not addressed during a performance. Existing findings investigated these psychological dimensions during competitions and with a longitudinal design (9, 11). Competing at National and International levels, compared to Regional level, may elicit higher levels of these psychological dimensions in motorcycle racers at the time of the competition. Future studies should explore these psychological dimensions across different levels of competition recruiting participants during competition seasons.

Finally, the small effect sizes detected suggested that statistically significant differences, if present, may be rare and could potentially emerge with a larger sample size. Hence, these two groups may be indeed characterized by small differences on these psychological dimensions, and the current sample size does not enable to identify them.

As for the second aim, we found small-to-moderate associations between the three forms of intrinsic motivation (i.e., to know, to accomplish, and to stimulate) and dispositional flow experienced while motorcycling. However, sensation seeking showed only a weak association with intrinsic motivation to stimulate. Age and years and experience demonstrated significant correlations only among themselves. In the regression model, we found both dispositional flow and sensation seeking to be significantly associated with intrinsic motivation. However, when examining the standardized coefficients, dispositional flow demonstrated a moderate effect, while sensation seeking demonstrated a weak effect. Overall, these findings highlighted that experiencing flow could be an important factor while engaging in motorcycle racing, as it can foster intrinsic motivation and contribute to the enjoyment of the sport (16).

In contrast, seeking extreme emotions and potentially dangerous situations may not be as relevant in motivating motorcycle racers to practice this sport, despite its considered significance in other extreme sports (29). One possible explanation is that individuals who are highly motivated by sensation seeking may be more likely to drop out sooner from motorcycle racing or withdraw from the Italian Federation of Motorcycling. In fact, this psychological trait has been found to predict disruptive behaviors and potentially dangerous conduct while motorcycling (34), which could undermine involvement in motorcycle races by reducing interest in adhering to rules and fair play. The weak relation between sensation seeking and intrinsic motivation could be ultimately beneficial for motorcycle racers, as it may suggest that they could be less inclined to engage in risky behaviors and experiences, possibly increasing injury risk. Previous research has indicated that risk and experience seeking are associated with a higher risk of injuries in individuals involved in extreme sports (29) and linked to risky behaviors in motorcycling (28). Additionally, motorcyclists who engage in frequent dangerous behaviors on the road, such as following a vehicle too closely, driving through amber traffic lights, or dangerous overtaking, are more likely to be involved in street accidents (35).

Notably, flow and sensation seeking explained only 22.7% of variance in the regression model. Despite being possibly influential, these psychological dimensions do not appear to fully explain intrinsic motivation to stimulate. Future studies should consider other variables (e.g., perceived self-efficacy) that could be involved in motivating motorcycle racers in practicing this sport.

Moreover, the cross-sectional nature of the study did not enable to discuss cause-and-effect relations: thus, it is possible that intrinsic motivation might lead motorcycle racers to experience flow while performing or influence their levels of sensation seeking. Future studies should consider employing more elaborate methods, such as longitudinal designs, and include a control group comprising recreational motorcyclists or other athletes. For instance, evaluation of state flow experiences, measured during a competition instead of retrospectively, would clarify how these suggested relations manifest while performing.

In contrast to the conceptual overlap suggested by previous research (32), the present study found no significant relation between flow and sensation seeking in the sample of motorcycle racers. This indicated that these two constructs may be experienced as distinct phenomena among motorcycle racers. While both flow and sensation seeking involve seeking exceptional experiences, their relation in the context of motorcycle racing appears to be non-significant. This finding points to the need for further investigation into the unique experiences and psychological dimensions associated with flow and sensation seeking in the specific context of motorcycle racing.

The findings of the regression analysis raised questions about the classification of motorcycle racing as an extreme sport, suggesting that flow may be more relevant than sensation seeking in defining engagement in this particular sport. These findings also implied that motorcycle racing shares similarities with other individual sports, as evidenced by the association between intrinsic motivation and dispositional flow, even when accounting for sensation seeking. In line with this hypothesis, the current study did not find a significant correlation between flow and sensation seeking in the sample of motorcycle racers, suggesting that these constructs are experienced as distinct phenomena within this population. Although there may be a conceptual overlap between flow and sensation seeking (3), the present findings supported the notion that they represent different experiences among motorcycle racers.

4.1 Limitations and future directions

This study had several limitations that should be acknowledged. First, we explicitly asked participants to refer to their experiences of flow in the context of motorcycling while filling in the DFS-2, which may have influenced the associations with intrinsic motivation. In fact, this focused approach may have contributed to stronger associations between the measures. Second, some of the measures used in the study exhibited low internal consistency, which could have impacted on the reliability of the findings. In particular, the BSSS showed low internal consistency (α = .57), highlighting the importance of interpreting the findings with caution. To date, the BSSS represents a brief and useful measure to assess sensation seeking; future studies could consider including a different measure to investigate this psychological construct and its associations with intrinsic motivation and flow, as well as competition level. Third, the cross-sectional design of the study and the retrospective investigation of these psychological dimensions prevented the possibility to establish causal relations or determine the direction of the observed associations. As previously mentioned, longitudinal associations should be explored: motorcycle racers may experience variations in their levels of intrinsic motivation, flow, and sensation seeking before, during, and after a competition. Similarly, the associations with intrinsic motivation could change across time: sensation seeking may be influential at the initial stages of motorcycle racing practice, while flow may be more relevant to sustain persistence and continued participation in competitions. Additionally, the absence of a control group, such as a sample of individuals from the general population or recreational motorcyclists, restricted the generalizability of the findings. Fourth, we did not collect objective data pertaining to participating in competitions. As briefly mentioned before, participants may have misinterpreted the item, leading to potentially erroneous classification. Fifth, the sample was composed only by men, limiting the generalizability of the findings to motorcycle racers who self-identifying in other genders (e.g., women, gender non-binary). Sixth, self-selection bias could have influenced participation in the study, allowing responses from highly motivated motorcycle racers only. Finally, even though sample size was adequate according to the power analysis, a larger sample size could provide a more detailed analysis of the individual dimensions of flow, instead of solely relying on the total score, could allow to capture more nuances in the associations among constructs, and could improve the representativeness of the findings. These limitations should be considered when interpreting the results and further research should address these issues to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the psychological dimensions in motorcycle racing.

Future studies in the field of motorcycle racing could consider incorporating an evaluation of racers’ performance to further assess the practical application of current findings. However, assessing performance in this context poses unique challenges, as it involves the complex interaction between human skills and the functioning of the vehicle. Additionally, external factors such as track characteristics, weather conditions, and accidents involving the racer or other participants can significantly impact performance. To address these challenges, experimental studies could be conducted to control potential confounding variables and provide more robust findings. Furthermore, exploring the longitudinal associations between the risks of injuries, flow experiences, and sensation seeking could offer valuable insights into the impact of these psychological variables on the overall well-being of motorcycle racers. This would allow for a better understanding of how these factors interplay over time and potentially influence the racers’ physical and psychological health. Additionally, including a control group would enhance the comparability and generalizability of the findings. This could involve comparing motorcycle racers with individuals engaged in other sports or recreational motorcyclists, providing a broader perspective on the psychological dimensions specific to motorcycle racing. Furthermore, different disciplines of motorcycle racing (e.g., road racing, enduro, motocross) should be considered, exploring different nuances in how intrinsic motivation, flow, and sensation seeking are experienced in different types of motorcycle racers. Addressing these suggestions would contribute to advance the knowledge and understanding of the psychological aspects of motorcycle racing, while also considering the unique challenges and complexities associated with this sport. Moreover, it would also benefit practitioners, who could include intrinsic motivation, flow, or sensation seeking as targets in psychological interventions aimed to improve performance in motorcycle racing.

5 Conclusions

To conclude, this study enhanced our understanding of the psychological aspects within motorcycle racing by uncovering the similarities in psychological variables, namely intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking, among motorcycle racers participating at different levels of competition. The findings suggested that motorcycle racers, regardless of their competitive level, exhibit comparable levels of intrinsic motivation, while also indicating a significant association between intrinsic motivation and the experience of flow. Conversely, the relation between intrinsic motivation and sensation seeking appeared to be relatively weak. These findings contributed to the existing knowledge base and highlighted the importance of intrinsic motivation and flow in the context of motorcycle racing, with implications for further research and the development of interventions aimed at enhancing racer engagement and performance.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of Psychological Research of the University of Padova. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

PM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. MI: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. SC: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ER: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MR: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. MG: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

This study was conducted with the collaboration of Roberto Besi, Head of the Technical-Scientific Department at the Italian Federation of Motorcycling at the time of the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Gobbi AW, Francisco RA, Tuy B, Kvitne RS. Physiological characteristics of top level off-road motorcyclists. Br J Sports Med. (2005) 39:927–31. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2005.018291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Huth V, Füssl E, Risser R. Motorcycle riders’ perceptions, attitudes and strategies: findings from a focus group study. Transp Res F Traffic Psychol Behav. (2014) 25:74–85. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2014.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. D’Artibale E, Laursen PB, Cronin JB. Human performance in motorcycle road racing: a review of the literature. Sports Med. (2018) 48:1345–56. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0895-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. (2000) 55(1):68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq. (2000) 11(4):227–68. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gillet N, Vallerand RJ, Amoura S, Baldes B. Influence of coaches’ autonomy support on athletes’ motivation and sport performance: a test of the hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2010) 11(2):155–61. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Candela F, Zucchetti G, Villosio C. Preliminary validation of the Italian version of the original sport motivation scale. J Hum Sport Exerc. (2014) 9(1):136–47. doi: 10.4100/jhse.2014.91.14

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pelletier LG, Tuson KM, Fortier MS, Vallerand RJ, Briére NM, Blais MR. Toward a new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: the sport motivation scale (SMS). J Sport Exerc Psychol. (1995) 17(1):35–53. doi: 10.1123/jsep.17.1.35

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Jõesaar H, Hein V, Hagger MS. Peer influence on young athletes’ need satisfaction, intrinsic motivation and persistence in sport: a 12-month prospective study. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2011) 12(5):500–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.04.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Berki T, Piko BF, Page RM. The relationship between the models of sport commitment and self-determination among adolescent athletes. Acta fac Educ Phys Univ Comen. (2019) 59(2):79–95. doi: 10.2478/afepuc-2019-0007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Almagro BJ, Sáenz-López P, Fierro-Suero S, Conde C. Perceived performance, intrinsic motivation and adherence in athletes. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17(24):9441. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17249441

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Skugor K, Gilic B, Mladenovic M, Stajer V, Roklicer R, Slacanac K, et al. Motivation profile of youth greco-roman wrestlers; differences according to performance quality. Sports. (2023) 11(2):43. doi: 10.3390/sports11020043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Schmid MJ, Charbonnet B, Conzelmann A, Zuber C. More success with the optimal motivational pattern? A prospective longitudinal study of young athletes in individual sports. Front Psychol. (2021) 11:606272. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606272

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY, US: Harper Collins (1990).

Google Scholar

15. Schüler J, Wolff W, Duda JL. Intrinsic motivation in the context of sports. In: Schüler J, Wegner M, Plessner H, Eklund RC, editors. Sport and Exercise Psychology: Theory and Application. Cham: Springer Cham (2023). p. 171–92.

Google Scholar

16. Swann C, Crust L, Jackman P, Vella SA, Allen MS, Keegan R. Psychological states underlying excellent performance in sport: toward an integrated model of flow and clutch states. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2017) 29(4):375–401. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2016.1272650

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lewis J, Patterson I. How does motivation relate to the experience of flow? Proceeding of the 2nd Victorian Sport Psychology Conference; 2010 February 2–4. Brisbane: Queensland (2010).

Google Scholar

18. Nakamura J, Tse DCK, Shankland S. Flow: the experience of intrinsic motivation. In: Ryan RM, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2019). p. 169–86.

Google Scholar

19. Bakker AB, Oerlemans W, Demerouti E, Slot BB, Ali DK. Flow and performance: a study among talented Dutch soccer players. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2011) 12(4):442–50. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.02.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Garcia WF, Codonhato R, Mizoguchi MV, Nascimento Junior JRA, Aizava PVS, Ribas ML, et al. Dispositional flow and performance in Brazilian triathletes. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:2136. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jackson SA, Thomas PR, Marsh HW, Smethurst CJ. Relationships between flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2001) 13(2):129–53. doi: 10.1080/104132001753149865

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Stamatelopoulou F, Pezirkianidis C, Karakasidou E, Lakioti A, Stalikas A. Being in the zone”: a systematic review on the relationship of psychological correlates and the occurrence of flow experiences in sports’ performance. Psychology. (2018) 9:2011–30. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.98115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zuckerman M. Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (1994).

Google Scholar

24. Cohen R, Baluch B, Duffy LJ. Defining extreme sport: conceptions and misconceptions. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:1974. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01974

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Brymer E. Extreme dude: a phenomenological exploration into the extreme sport experience (dissertation). Doctoral Dissertation-University of Wollongong, Wollongong (2005) http://ro.uow.edu.au/theses/379

Google Scholar

26. Olivera Betrán J, Olivera Betrán A. Adventure physical activities in nature (APAN): review of the taxonomy (1995–2015) and tables for classification and identification of practices. Apunts: Educació Física I Esports. (2016) 124(2):53–70. doi: 10.5672/apunts.2014-0983.cat.(2016/2).124.06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McEwan D, Boudreau P, Curran T, Rhodes RE. Personality traits of high-risk sport participants: a meta-analysis. J Res Pers. (2019) 79:83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Zamani-Alavijeh F, Niknami S, Bazargan M, Mohammadi E, Montazeri A, Ahmadi F, et al. Accident-related risk behaviors associated with motivations for motorcycle use in iran: a country with very high traffic deaths. Traffic Inj Prev. (2009) 10(3):237–42. doi: 10.1080/15389580902822717

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Weishaar MG, Kentopp SD, Wallace GT, Conner BT. An investigation of the effects of sensation seeking and impulsivity on extreme sport participation and injury using path analysis. J Am Coll Health. (2021) 71(5):1522–9. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2021.1942008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Yıldırım-Yenier Z, Vingilis E, Wiesenthal DL, Mann RE, Seeley J. Show more. Relationships between thrill seeking, speeding attitudes, and driving violations among a sample of motorsports spectators and drivers. Accid Anal Prev. (2016) 86:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.09.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Diehm R, Armatas C. Surfing: an avenue for socially acceptable risk-taking, satisfying needs for sensation seeking and experience seeking. Pers Individ Dif. (2004) 36(3):663–77. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00124-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Schüler J, Pfenninger M. Flow impairs risk perception in kayakers. In: Geranto BD, editor. Sport Psychology. New York, NY, US: Nova Science Publishers (2010). p. 237–46.

Google Scholar

33. Chen CF, Chen CW. Speeding for fun? Exploring the speeding behavior of riders of heavy motorcycles using the theory of planned behavior and psychological flow theory. Accid Anal Prev. (2011) 43(3):983–90. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.11.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Antoniazzi D, Klein R. Risky riders: a comparison of personality theories on motorcyclist riding behaviour. Transp Res F Traffic Psychol Behav. (2019) 62:33–44. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.12.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Theofilatos A, Yannis G. Relationship between Motorcyclists’ attitudes, behavior, and other attributes with declared accident involvement in Europe. Traffic Inj Prev. (2014) 15(2):156–64. doi: 10.1080/15389588.2013.801554

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Federazione Motociclistica Italiana. Regolamento Manifestazioni Motociclistiche (RMM). (2025). Available at: https://www.federmoto.it/wp-content/uploads/sites/2/2023/12/RMM-al-8-4-25-v.pdf (Accessed May 24, 2025).

Google Scholar

37. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Jackson SA, Martin AJ, Eklund RC. Long and short measures of flow: the construct validity of the FSS-2, DFS-2, and new brief counterparts. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 30(5):561–87. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.5.561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Riva EFM, Riva G, Talò C, Boffi M, Rainisio N, Pola L, et al. Measuring dispositional flow: validity and reliability of the dispositional flow state scale 2, Italian version. PLoS One. (2017) 12(9):1–16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182201

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hoyle RH, Stephenson MT, Palmgreen P, Lorch EP, Donohew RL. Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Pers Individ Dif. (2002) 32(3):401–14. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Primi C, Narducci R, Benedetti D, Donati M, Chiesi F. Validity and reliability of the Italian version of the brief sensation seeking scale (BSSS) and its invariance across age and gender. TPM Test Psychom Methodol Appl Psychol. (2011) 18(4):231–41.

Google Scholar

42. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Routledge (1988).

Google Scholar

Keywords: motorcycle racer, motorsport, intrinsic motivation, flow, sensation seeking

Citation: Mancin P, Ionel MS, Cerea S, Riva EFM, Rapisarda M and Ghisi M (2025) Riding with the flow: study on intrinsic motivation, dispositional flow, and sensation seeking among Italian motorcycle racers. Front. Sports Act. Living 7:1501123. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2025.1501123

Received: 24 September 2024; Accepted: 27 May 2025;
Published: 13 June 2025.

Edited by:

Tiziano A. Agostini, University of Trieste, Italy

Reviewed by:

Miguel-Angel Gomez-Ruano, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Spain
Matej Maksimiljan Tusak, University of Ljubljana, Slovenia
Erik Monasterio, University of Otago, New Zealand

Copyright: © 2025 Mancin, Ionel, Cerea, Riva, Rapisarda and Ghisi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paolo Mancin, cGFvbG8ubWFuY2luQHVuaXBkLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.