Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 05 October 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic The Notion of the Native Speaker Put to the Test: Recent Research Advances View all 17 articles

Deconstructing the Native Speaker: Further Evidence From Heritage Speakers for Why This Horse Should Be Dead!

  • 1Department of English Linguistics, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
  • 2Center for Cognitive Science, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany

The category “native speaker” is flawed because it fails to consider the diversity between the speaker groups falling under its scope, as highlighted in previous literature. This paper provides further evidence by focusing on the similarities and differences between heritage speakers (HSs) and monolingually-raised speakers (MSs) of their heritage and majority languages. HSs are bilinguals who acquire a family (heritage) language and a societal (majority) language in early childhood. Naturalistic exposure from early childhood qualifies them as native speakers of their heritage language. Some HSs are simultaneous bilinguals, which makes them native speakers of their majority language as well. Others are early second language acquirers who may be indistinguishable from simultaneous bilinguals. Previous research shows that the heritage language productions of German HSs in the United States do not completely overlap with those of German MSs, who are, by default, native speakers. In overall clause type selection (independent main, coordinate main, and subordinate), the HSs differ from German MSs in German but are similar to English MSs in English. The present study examines the distribution of finite subordinate clauses and their types (relative, complement, and adverbial) across registers in 27 adolescent HSs of German in the United States, compared to 32 adolescent MSs of German and 32 MSs of English. All participants described a short video in two settings (formal/informal) and two modes (spoken/written). Results demonstrate that, even with respect to a specific phenomenon (subordinate clauses), HSs show similarities and differences to MSs of both languages. Concerning the distribution of subordinate clause types, HSs behave similarly to both English and German MSs. Concerning subordinate clauses in general, HSs use them less frequently than MSs in German. In English, the difference is more nuanced: HSs differentiate between settings in both modes, while MSs do so only in the written mode. This indicates that the category “native speaker” is not a meaningful descriptor since it covers speakers with varying production patterns. We propose that studies including native speakers should assure transparency and replicability of research by specifying and taking into account speaker characteristics such as bilingualism, proficiency, exposure and dominance.

Introduction

The category “native speaker” has been used to characterize a particular speaker population for many years (see Hopp, 2016; Azar et al., 2019; Ionin et al., 2021; Redl et al., 2021 as recent cases in point). What most researchers seem to agree on is that a native speaker is defined as a speaker who acquires their language naturalistically in early childhood (Cook, 1999; Davies, 2004, 2013). Despite its popularity, this definition can be questioned. It has been criticized for being a political and ideological construct (Bonfiglio, 2010; Dewaele, 2018) and for discrediting late second language (L2) speakers as “deficient versions of natives” (Cook, 2016, p. 186). Another point of criticism is that the category is underspecified because it does not reflect the variation within the subgroups under its scope (Davies, 2004; Lowe, 2020). This criticism holds for the specific native speaker population considered in the present study, namely heritage speakers (HSs). They are broadly defined as “bilinguals who have acquired a family (heritage language) and a majority societal language naturalistically in early childhood” (Pascual et al., 2012, p. 450). Therefore, they are native speakers of both of their languages (Montrul, 2016; Kupisch and Rothman, 2018) irrespective of them being simultaneous bilinguals or early L2 acquirers of the majority language (Rothman and Treffers-Daller, 2014, p. 96).

Comparisons of HSs with monolingually-raised speakers (MSs) reveal areas of difference and similarity (Montrul, 2016, p. 208). The similarities with MSs can be found in both their heritage language (Nagy, 2015; Nagy and Lo, 2019; Łyskawa and Nagy, 2020) and their majority language (Kupisch et al., 2014; Pashkova et al., in press). The differences also become apparent in both their languages (Rothman, 2007; Polinsky, 2018; Scontras et al., 2018 for the heritage language; Scontras et al., 2017; Polinsky, 2018; Paradis, 2019 for the majority language). It is important to mention that the differences are not clear-cut but rather gradient. For example, in a study on clause-type use across registers, we found that German HSs with majority English showed similar distributional patterns in their heritage German productions in independent main clauses and different patterns in coordinate main clauses and subordinate clauses, compared to German MSs (Pashkova et al., in press). These results illustrate a more nuanced difference in clause type productions of MSs and HSs in their heritage language. Taken together, these findings indicate that the category “native speaker” fails to adequately reflect the variation between the speaker groups who fall under its scope, in this case, HSs and MSs.

Consequently, if a linguistic study states that it examined a group of native speakers, we cannot be absolutely certain who these speakers were and if their individual patterns of language use were comparable. The native speaker group could comprise for example MSs, HSs, or late L2 acquirers who emigrated and whose first language (L1) is undergoing attrition. Unquestionably, these speakers use their native language differently. Thus, further specification of the category “native speaker” is necessary to ensure transparency and replicability of research.

In the current study, we continue to address similarities and differences between two groups of native speakers, namely HSs and MSs. Focusing on finite subordinate clauses (SCs), we investigate their general use and the use of their types (complement, adverbial, and relative) across registers. This structural spectrum offers a promising area of variation in the two native speaker sub-groups because it is located at the interface of syntax and discourse (Sorace, 2011).

On the syntactic level, mastery of SCs is a potential source of variation in heritage language due to the complexity of SCs and different word order constraints in SCs in HSs’ heritage and majority language (Pashkova et al., in press). Regarding SC types, differences in acquisition timing, paths, and the language input may play a key role in their later production (Andreou et al., 2020a). Researchers have suggested different acquisition trajectories of subordinate clause types (Vasilyeva et al., 2008; Paradis et al., 2017). In heritage language contexts, HSs and MSs presumably have similar acquisition conditions during infancy and early childhood, which then start to diverge once exposure to the majority language increases (around preschool/kindergarten), and especially once formal schooling sets in. Hence, for the heritage language, we can expect that the earliest acquired SC types will be similar in HSs’ and MSs’ productions, while the later acquired types might show more variation. In the majority language, HSs might experience a delay in late-acquired phenomena but eventually catch up with MSs (Schulz and Grimm, 2019), so we expect, apart from timing, no pronounced qualitative differences between HSs and MSs.

On the discourse level, register awareness creates another source of variation in heritage language use since HSs might not have sufficient exposure to a similarly wide range of registers as MSs of the same language (Polinsky, 2018, pp. 323–324; Aalberse et al., 2019, p. 148). HSs usually experience their heritage language in informal settings, most likely in oral interactions with family members, and might not be as familiar with formal registers. On the other hand, they use their majority language in a greater variety of communicative situations, so they develop a nuanced register awareness comparable to that of MSs of the majority language. Our research has shown that HSs can transfer their register awareness from their majority language to the heritage language, at least while choosing between independent main, coordinate main, and subordinate clauses (Pashkova et al., in press) when all options are available, in principle. What is yet unclear is whether and how this register awareness will manifest itself in a larger speaker sample and within specific sub-domains, such as the use of SC types.

In comparing HSs and MSs in their use of SCs and their types, we will argue that applying the category “native speaker” as a cover term for both these groups obscures a meaningful description of the variation in their patterns of language use. We address this terminological difficulty and propose adding further specification to the category “native speaker,” such as presence of bilingualism, to enhance transparency and replicability. We furthermore briefly explore other variables, such as proficiency, exposure and dominance as potential characteristics for specification.

Theoretical and Conceptual Background

The Native Speaker Spectrum

A native speaker has been defined as “a person who learns a language as a child and continues to use it fluently as a dominant language” (Richards and Schmidt, 2013, p. 386). Other characteristics include grammatical and appropriate usage of the native language, self-identification with the community where it is spoken, and intuitions about (un)grammatical structures in that language. Davies (2013) adds creative performance and the ability to translate and interpret into the native language to the list of native speaker characteristics.

However, within these (extra-)linguistic features included in native speaker definitions, only one is uncontroversial and straightforward, namely the childhood acquisition of their L1 (Cook, 1999, p. 187; Davies, 2003, p. 436). Many of the other features mentioned can also be found in L2 speakers: they can use their L2 fluently, grammatically, appropriately, and intuitively, and be creative performers and translators/interpreters. This is the first point of criticism of the category “native speaker”: how helpful is the category to group people with similar patterns of language use if the majority of its defining features appears in non-native speakers’ productions as well (Lowe, 2020, pp. 21–22)?

Beyond linguistic considerations of fluency, accuracy, and intuition, the category “native speaker” has also been criticized for being politically and ideologically charged. It is noted that being a native speaker is associated with power, language ownership, and even positive personality traits (Bonfiglio, 2010). Race, background, and identity play a role in deciding whether a speaker could be a member of the native speaker group. Holliday (2009) writes that a prototypical English native speaker is a white Anglo-Saxon from an English-speaking western country, and those who do not fit this image might be excluded from native speakerhood. Bonfiglio (2010, p. 12) argues that, in some cases, nativeness is judged based on the speaker’s ethnic/immigrant family background and not their language, for instance, Turkish HSs in Germany might not be readily viewed as German native speakers, even though they grew up in Germany and acquired German as one of their L1s.

Monolinguals and Heritage Speakers on the Native Speaker Spectrum

Monolingual speakers are the least disputed speaker population subsumed under the category “native speaker” as they only acquire their L1 naturalistically. HSs, however, have not always been included in the group of native speakers (Polinsky and Scontras, 2020). On the one hand, this might be surprising because HSs fit the criterion of naturalistic acquisition from early childhood. Some researchers might have excluded HSs from native speakers since they equate nativeness with high proficiency and dominance instead of seeing it as a product of naturalistic L1 acquisition (Kupisch and Rothman, 2018). On the other hand, such a confusion is understandable since we do frequently see differences in HSs’ heritage language productions compared to MSs’. This is, however, an insufficient criterion for excluding HSs from the native speaker continuum as they are not the only group that might differ from a prototypical, highly proficient monolingual native speaker. We also find these differences in MSs with limited experience with the standard language and in late L2 bilinguals who have migrated and shifted dominance to the L2 and are experiencing L1 attrition (Dewaele, 2018; Kupisch and Rothman, 2018).

If the differences between HSs’ and MSs’ productions are not due to HSs being non-native speakers, what could they be attributed to? Many researchers agree that differences in amount and quality of input play a very important role in the eventual outcomes of heritage language acquisition (Montrul, 2016, pp. 117–119; Kupisch and Rothman, 2018; Aalberse et al., 2019, pp. 146–149). These differences in input could lead to variation in heritage language productions, for example, case marking in heritage German (Yager et al., 2015; Zimmer, 2020), inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese (Rothman, 2007), or the encoding of motion events in heritage Turkish (Goschler et al., 2020). However, some areas of the heritage language still display substantial similarity with MSs’ productions, for example, voice onset times in heritage Italian (Nagy, 2015), case morphology in heritage Polish, Russian, and Ukrainian (Łyskawa and Nagy, 2020), or use of classifiers in heritage Cantonese (Nagy and Lo, 2019).

Yet, it would be too simplistic to say that one domain of heritage language grammar and use would show only similarities to MSs’ productions, while another domain would be likely to show only differences. Some areas show both differences and similarities with MSs’ productions. For instance, Brehmer and Usanova (2015) report that verb placement in heritage Russian in Germany is different in SCs compared to monolingual Russian, with an increase in use of the verb in clause-final position, which would be an expected transfer from German. However, main clauses in heritage Russian do not feature more use of the verb in second position (V2, required in German) than those in monolingual Russian. Thus, verb placement in heritage Russian exhibits difference and similarities with monolingual Russian. In a similar vein, our own previous research demonstrated that clause type use across different registers in heritage German also shows a combination of differences and similarities with monolingual German. While independent main clauses are used in the same manner by both speaker groups, coordinate main and subordinate clauses exhibit variation: HSs prefer coordinate main clauses, while MSs choose subordinate clauses more frequently (Pashkova et al., in press).

Concerning HSs’ majority language, their linguistic behavior in everyday interactions is oftentimes comparable to that of MSs, especially once HSs reach early adulthood (Paradis, 2019). For example, HSs have been reported to not have a foreign accent in their majority language (Kupisch et al., 2014). Further, Pashkova et al. (in press) found no evidence that German HSs use different clause type patterns across registers in their majority English, compared to English MSs—overall, both groups used more independent main clauses in the written mode, more coordinate main clauses in the spoken mode, and more subordinate clauses in the formal setting. However, there is experimental evidence that HSs might exhibit more fine-grained differences to English MSs in their majority English, for instance in the release of final stops (Polinsky, 2018, pp. 141–144), grammaticality judgments of subject–verb agreement (Paradis, 2019), and scope assignment (Scontras et al., 2017).

Summing up, HSs are typically native speakers of both of their languages since they typically acquire both languages naturalistically in early childhood. This does not mean, however, that HSs’ linguistic performance is identical to that of prototypical, highly proficient MSs. These two groups of native speakers show differences and similarities in the patterns of their language use. Therefore, we propose further specification of the category “native speaker” in order to reflect this variability. Our study illustrates that an important variable to specify is the presence of bilingualism; additional specifications can include proficiency, exposure, and dominance.

Subordinate Clauses

The use of SCs and their types across registers is complex in that the speaker requires both syntactic knowledge and register awareness to decide on the appropriateness of SCs according to communicative situations (as explained in section “Register Characteristics of Subordinate Clauses,” SCs are often more preferred in formal contexts). As specified in the Interface Hypothesis, structures involving both syntactic and pragmatic choices are particularly open to variation in terms of acquisition timing and/or cross-linguistic influence (Sorace, 2011; Tsimpli, 2014), thus leading to potentially different patterns across different types of natives speakers. We thereby add subordinate clause choice to the phenomena considered in interface research, given that register is a part of pragmatics, a language-external component (Tsimpli, 2014, p. 301). In the following section, we will examine the syntactic mastery of SCs and register awareness in both speaker groups.

Syntactic Characteristics of Subordinate Clauses

Subordinate clauses in general

Syntactically, SCs have the following features (Diessel, 2004, p. 48): they are integrated in the matrix clause, they are dependent structures that are formally incomplete without the matrix clause, and they are part of the same processing and planning unit as the associated matrix clause. This last feature is one of the reasons why SCs have been associated with higher syntactic complexity than juxtaposed matrix clauses (Polinsky, 2008; Neary-Sundquist, 2017; Peristeri et al., 2017; Sánchez Abchi and De Mier, 2017; Housen et al., 2019). Syntactic complexity has been defined, among other things, as the extent to which language users resort to syntactic embedding and SCs or as a structure which requires more steps in the syntactic derivation (Housen et al., 2012; Sanfelici and Schulz, 2021). However, the direct link between SCs and syntactic complexity has also been questioned: several researchers reported that textual complexity correlated not with the number of SCs but rather with mean length of nominal phrases and clauses (Lu, 2011; Wiese et al., 2020; Wang and Tao, 2020). Overall, the evidence for high complexity of SCs appears conflicting. Nevertheless, if SCs reflect textual complexity to some extent, we would expect fewer SCs in HSs’ productions in their heritage language compared to MSs of that language.

In addition to the general complexity of SCs across languages, different word order constraints in HSs’ heritage and majority language might play a role in SC production. This study examines HSs of German with English as their majority language. German and English differ in SC word order: In finite clauses introduced by complementizers and relative pronouns, German canonically exhibits subject-object-verb (SOV) structure,1 while English has subject-verb-object (SVO) structure. This typological mismatch between the two languages of HSs might make the production of SCs in German harder for HSs than for MSs due to higher cognitive load because of the inhibition of one structure in the bilingual mind—in this case, SVO (Abutalebi and Green, 2016). This may lead to avoidance of SCs in the German productions of HSs (see Pashkova et al., in press, for a more detailed discussion).

Subordinate clause types

This section focuses on the syntactic characteristics of SC types and on how they might contribute to the variation between HSs and MSs. We follow previous researchers (e.g., Beaman, 1984; Diessel, 2004; Thompson et al., 2007; Paradis et al., 2017; Andreou and Tsimpli, 2020) in subdividing finite SCs into three categories: complement, adverbial, and relative clauses. In the following, we describe each clause type in detail and provide an overview of their L1 acquisition patterns.

Complement clauses are SCs that function as arguments of a predicate in the matrix clause (e.g., She saw that a car was coming.) (Biber et al., 1999, p. 658; Diessel, 2004, p. 1; Noonan, 2007; Lust et al., 2015, p. 301). Some researchers have suggested that complement clauses emerge early in L1 acquisition (Vasilyeva et al., 2008; Paradis et al., 2017), one of the reasons proposed for this being that they are narrowly syntactic structures that only require the knowledge of verb complement selection patterns and no pragmatic skills in discourse management (Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011; Andreou, 2015; Andreou et al., 2020a). In child HSs, the accurate repetition of complement clauses in a sentence repetition task at the ages of 8–12 was reported to be associated with the amount of exposure to the language between ages 0 and 3 and at the age of 6 (Andreou et al., 2020a). This suggests that there are crucial periods for the development of complement clauses that correlate with their production later on. Hence, in the heritage language, we would expect similar production patterns in HSs and MSs because they received similar input at an age when language exposure could affect their emergence.

Adverbial clauses are SCs that modify main clauses similarly to adverbs and adverbial adjuncts modifying a proposition (e.g., While she was walking, she saw an accident) (Diessel, 2004, p. 1; Thompson et al., 2007). Contrary to narrowly syntactic structures, adverbial clauses, along with relative clauses, involve the syntax–discourse interface because they rely on discourse and pragmatics and call for discourse management skills (Peristeri et al., 2017, pp. 5, 11; Andreou et al., 2020a, b). For this reason, it has been argued that adverbial clauses are acquired later than complement clauses. Moreover, in child HSs, the accurate repetition of adverbial clauses at the ages of 8–12 was shown to be influenced by current language exposure (Andreou et al., 2020a). This suggests that adverbial clause use might be a locus for greater variation between heritage language and monolingual productions due to differences in the speakers’ current language exposure.

Relative clauses are SCs that modify a noun phrase (NP) (e.g., A woman who was pushing a baby stroller was walking down the street) (Andrews, 2007). They are characterized by a syntactic gap that is associated with a relative pronoun at their left periphery and requires as its antecedent the relativized constituent of the matrix clause (Biber et al., 1999, p. 608; Diessel, 2004, p. 117). Similar to adverbial clauses, relative clauses are also located at the syntax-discourse interface and require discourse management skills, i.e., the ability to determine what is needed for referent specification in particular contexts. Therefore, one might expect relative clauses to be more influenced by later exposure, hence leading to greater variation between HSs’ heritage language productions and those of MSs.

In the current study, we investigate whether the suggested differences of the acquisition onset of SC types impacts their use in HSs who are older than those examined in previous research (Andreou et al., 2020a).

Register Characteristics of Subordinate Clauses

Register is a variety definable in terms of situational parameters such as participants, channel, purpose, spoken or written mode, and formality of communication (Biber and Conrad, 2001, p. 175). In this study, we operationalize formality as spoken or written communication with public institutions, and informality as spoken or written communication with friends and family. HSs normally do not have as frequent exposure to a variety of registers in their heritage language compared to MSs of that language (Polinsky, 2018, pp. 323–324; Aalberse et al., 2019, p. 148 for recent mention of this tendency). Since the use of the heritage language is mostly limited to interactions with family members and perhaps members of a heritage language community, HSs are usually expected to be more familiar with informal registers and less familiar with formal registers. At the same time, HSs’ majority language typically follows a different trajectory: they use it in a wider range of communicative situations and thus develop formal and informal register repertoires comparable to those of MSs. It is an interesting question, then, how HSs approach formal registers in their heritage language: would they use language patterns from the informal registers of their heritage language or would they try to rely on the formal register patterns from their majority language? Schleppegrell and Colombi (1997) argued for the latter option: they showed that Spanish HSs used very similar clause types in academic essays in heritage Spanish and majority English, despite being unfamiliar with formal academic registers in their heritage language.

Our recent study (Pashkova et al., in press) identified a similar tendency: German HSs showed similar clause type patterns in formal and informal registers in heritage German and majority English, which we called “an underlying register awareness”—HSs were able to transfer their register awareness from their majority language to their heritage language. Crucially, HSs used similar clause type patterns in heritage German compared not only to majority English but also to monolingual German. This possibility of transfer appears viable when the heritage and majority languages have similar register-related language use of the phenomenon under scrutiny, as was the case for clause type use in German and English (in both languages, MSs preferred independent main clauses in the written mode, coordinate main clauses in the spoken mode, and subordinate clauses in the formal setting). It is as yet unclear if register awareness can be attested in a larger data sample and transferred to another phenomenon, such as SC types. However, it is important to note that similar patterns of SC use in heritage and monolingual German did not mean the same frequency of SCs—HSs still used overall fewer SCs than MSs, most likely due to the syntactic characteristics of SCs outlined above.

Subordinate clauses and their types show variation across registers, which makes them an interesting phenomenon to examine with respect to register-related linguistic behavior of HSs. For instance, Koch and Oesterreicher (2012) outlined syntactic features of the language of immediacy, i.e., spontaneous face-to-face dialogues between familiar speakers, and the language of distance, i.e., carefully planned interactions between strangers in the public sphere. The language of immediacy is characterized by parataxis, whereas the language of distance is associated with hypotaxis. Our previous study (Pashkova et al., in press) confirmed this claim: in both English and German, we found more SCs in formal registers, which were similar to the language of distance, than in informal registers, similar to the language of immediacy.

Subordinate clause types are also subject to register variation. In English, for example, Biber and Gray (2016, pp. 87–100) reported more complement and adverbial finite clauses in conversation than in academic writing, and more wh-relative clauses in academic writing than in conversation. Beaman (1984) showed that nominal and relative subordinations occur more often in spoken narratives than in written ones, while adverbial subordinations are more frequent in written productions. Even though these findings do not map directly on the registers examined in the current study (a formal report to the police vs. an informal message to a close friend), we can still expect a certain variation in SC type productions according to formality. Our data will serve as an addition to the research on register repertoires of HSs because, to the best of our knowledge, there has not been a study that focuses on the systematic analysis of SC types according to formality.

The Present Study

To address the gaps in the literature just discussed, we pursue the following research questions (RQs) concerning the use of SCs in HSs’ productions. Based on findings from the literature, we also lay out hypotheses and predictions for each question.

RQ 1: Do HSs show similarities or differences in the use of SCs according to register in their majority language (English) compared to English MSs and in their heritage language (German) compared to German MSs?

Hypothesis 1: Based on our previous study of clause type use in a smaller participant sample (Pashkova et al., in press), we expect HSs to show similarities to English MSs and to differ from German MSs due to syntactic complexity and SOV word order of German SCs.

Prediction 1: Comparing HSs’ majority English to monolingual English, we expect to find similar frequencies of SCs in all registers. Comparing HSs’ heritage German to monolingual German, we expect to find similar patterns across registers but overall fewer SCs in heritage German.

RQ 2: Do HSs show similarities or differences in the use of SC types (relative, complement, and adverbial) according to formality2 in their majority language (English) compared to English MSs and in their heritage language (German) compared to German MSs?

Hypothesis 2: We expect HSs to show similarities with English MSs, and a combination of differences and similarities with German MSs due to the different acquisition periods of SC types.

Prediction 2.1: Comparing HSs’ majority English to monolingual English, we expect to find similar frequencies of SC types across settings (formal/informal). Comparing HSs’ heritage German to monolingual German, we expect to find similar frequencies of complement clauses but different frequencies of adverbial and relative clauses, since the latter two SC types are assumed to be acquired later than complement clauses.

Prediction 2.2: Concerning the heritage language, we also expect to observe larger differences between HSs and MSs in the formal setting since HSs are less familiar with formal registers and we have no previous evidence that they can transfer their register awareness from majority English to heritage German in the use of SC types.

Materials and Methods

Participants

For this study we looked at 91 adolescents aged 14–18 years (mean age = 16.1, SD = 1.39, 50 females), with 32 in each of the monolingual groups and 27 in the heritage German group with English as their majority language.

1. HSs of German with majority language English (mean age = 15.6, SD = 1.58, 12 females)

2. MSs of German (mean age = 16.6, SD = 0.91, 19 females)

3. MSs of English (mean age = 16.1, SD = 1.49, 19 females).

The HSs of German grew up speaking German with at least one L1 German-speaking parent in the household (21 HSs had one German-speaking parent, five had two, and one participant provided no answer). All speakers were either born in the United States, or moved there before age two. They did not receive bilingual education, but may have participated in German “Saturday schools” or other German-speaking activities in the community. Speakers of established German “language islands” were excluded from the study. We defined monolinguals as speakers whose L1 was the only language spoken at home, but who might have acquired further languages through foreign language instruction.

German HSs were recruited in Boston, Massachusetts; Madison, Wisconsin; and St. Paul, Minnesota by contacting German organizations and institutions as well as via social media platforms. German MSs were recruited via contacting German high schools in Berlin. English MSs were recruited in the same cities as German HSs (and in Long Island, New York) via social media platforms or through personal contacts. The socio-economic status of HSs’ families was slightly higher than that of English and German MSs (see Supplementary Appendix A for detailed information on parental education) due to the nature of our HS participant pool, which mostly consisted of professionals whose move to the United States was work-related.

The German and English productions of the HSs as well as those of the English MSs were elicited in the United States and those of German MSs in Germany. The data for this study is openly accessible via the Research Unit Emerging Grammars (RUEG) 0.4.0 corpus (Wiese et al., 2020). Both English and German productions of HSs were compared to the productions of MSs of the respective language.

Materials and Procedure

The data was collected using the Language Situations methodology (RUEG group, 2018; Wiese, 2020), which elicits controlled, comparable, and quasi-naturalistic productions across registers. Participants watched a short non-verbal video depicting a minor car accident and recounted what they saw, imagining themselves witnesses to the accident. The procedure was divided into two settings. In the formal setting, the elicitor was formally dressed and met with the participant in a room set up like an office. In the informal setting, the elicitor was casually dressed and met with the participant in a more relaxed setting, with snacks and beverages offered. In order to enhance an easy-going, comfortable atmosphere, the elicitor and the participant engaged in 10–15 min of task-unrelated conversation in the target language at the beginning of the informal session. The participant watched the video three times in total (twice in the first setting, once in the second setting) and was then asked to recount it in two different modes: spoken and written.

The formal recounting was operationalized as a voice message to a police hotline (spoken) and a witness report to the police (written), while the informal recounting comprised a WhatsApp voice message (spoken) and a WhatsApp text message (written) to a friend. The order of settings (formal/informal) and modes (spoken/written) was balanced across participants. The MSs completed all tasks in one session. The HSs completed the tasks in two sessions—one for their majority language (English) and one for their heritage language (German)—with an interval of 3–5 days in between to minimize priming effects. The order of language sessions was counterbalanced across participants. Upon completion of all the narrative tasks, the participants filled out an online questionnaire3 about their language background as well as a self-assessment of their abilities in each language. Self-assessment showed that HSs rated their speaking and writing skills higher in their majority English (speaking mean = 5, SD = 0; writing mean = 4.96, SD = 0.19) than in heritage German (speaking mean = 3.66, SD = 0.78; writing mean = 2.81, SD = 1.27). English monolinguals rated their skills comparably high (speaking mean = 4.75, SD = 0.51; writing mean = 4.53, SD = 0.57) to German monolinguals (speaking mean = 4.96, SD = 0.17; writing mean = 4.66, SD = 0.66).

Data Coding

As mentioned above, we investigated the use of SCs and their types (complement, adverbial, and relative) in narratives in English and German. In both languages, we examined only clauses that contained finite verbs to constrain the nature of the question. Morphologically non-canonical clauses, i.e., deviations with respect to person and number agreement, were still included, since they do not affect the type that the clause is assigned to. Subordinations missing complementizers or relative pronouns were included because a large proportion of the data stems from spoken productions and omitting complementizer “that” or relative pronouns “who” and “which” (in English) is common in spoken productions (Biber and Conrad, 2001). Non-finite constructions, such as infinitives, present participles, and past participles were excluded. All narratives were split into finite clauses, and each clause was coded for being an SC or a matrix clause. In German, SCs mostly exhibited finite verb-final structures, with the exception of unintroduced complement clauses (see below).4 Weil V2 clauses were not coded as SCs since weil has lost its status of a subordinator in those constructions (Antomo and Steinbach, 2010; Reis, 2013).

Each SC was coded for its type: complement, adverbial, or relative.5 We included both verb and noun complement clauses in our analysis even though the majority of L1 acquisition literature focuses on verb complements. Noun complement clauses usually complement a certain set of nouns such as question, thought, report, argument (Biber et al., 1999, pp. 645–656), and therefore appeared quite rarely in our data due to the content of the video. Since there were not enough cases to group them into a separate category, they were collapsed with verb complement clauses. Verb complement clauses (1a) should not be confused with what follows multi-word discourse markers I think, I mean, I don’t know, you know, which look like epistemic expressions. In order to differentiate a discourse marker from an epistemic expression, a complementizer test was applied: if a complementizer/wh-pronoun was present or could be added after the expression in question, the expression was not taken to be a discourse marker and, hence, the following part was annotated as a complement clause (1b). If a complementizer was absent and could not be added, the expression was taken to be a discourse marker with no complement clause (1c). Each clause in square brackets in (1) was counted as one complement clause.

(1) a. They weren’t looking and then realized [a car was comingcomplement] (USbi52FE_fwE)6

b. I don’t know [what else happenedcomplement] (USbi50FD_isE)

c. And then these two cars came by and like I dunnodiscourse marker they came to the intersection and the guy dropped his ball (USmo64FE_isE)

In complement clauses, German exhibits finite verb-final structures (2a), but also allows for canonical V2 structures, if the complementizer is omitted after verbs of saying and thinking (2b). Each clause in square brackets in (2) was counted as one complement clause.

(2) a. und konnte daher nicht wissen [ob nach der Ball ein Mensch kommen würdecomplement]

(USbi64MD_fwD)

“And due to this (the driver) could not know if a person would come after the ball.”

b. Ich hoffe [ich konnte ihnen behilflich seincomplement]!7 (DEmo54FD_fwD)

“I hope I could be of help to you!”

All types of adverbial clauses (e.g., temporal, locative, causative, conditional, concessive) were put into one category. Each clause in square brackets in (3) was counted as one adverbial clause.

(3) a. I witnessed the crash [as I was walking along the side of a streetadverbial] (USbi55FD_fwE)

b. The car stopped short [because there was a dog trying to get the balladverbial] (USmo59FE_iwE)

c. [Als sie die straße überqueren wolltenadverbial], ist der Mann den Ball aus dem Hand gefallen.

(USbi64MD_fwD).

“As they wanted to cross the street, the ball dropped out of the man’s hand.”

As for relative clauses, we included not only those modifying an NP (4a,b) but also those modifying an entire proposition (4c,d) (Biber et al., 1999, p. 867). The reasoning here was similar to the inclusion of noun complement clauses: even though the majority of L1 acquisition literature focuses on NP-modifying relative clauses, there were a few cases of proposition-modifying relative clauses, which were, however, not numerous enough to form their own category, so they were collapsed with NP-modifying relative clauses. Even though there has been extensive research on different types of relative clauses in HSs (e.g., Polinsky, 2011; Albirini and Benmamoun, 2014), we did not distinguish between object and subject relative clauses because we did not have sufficient data points to perform a separate comparison of the two types. Each clause in square brackets in (4) was counted as one relative clause.

(4) a. it tried to like stop for this dog [that was running into the streetrelative] (USmo65FE_isE)

b. Ein Mann [der anscheinend mit seiner Frau spazieren warrelative] prellte einen Fußball.

(DEmo69MD_fwD)

“A man who was walking apparently with his wife bounced a soccer ball.”

c. The dog saw the ball and ran for it, [which caused the car in the front to stoprelative].

(USbi51FD_fwE)

d. und is dem ersten auto dann raufgefahren [was zu dem unfall geführt hatrelative]

(DEmo65FD_fsD)

“and drove into the first car which lead to the accident”

Tables 1, 2 show the total number of clause productions in English and German respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. English clause productions by speaker group and register/formality.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. German clause productions by speaker group and register/formality.

Data Analysis

First, the data was coded for SCs and matrix clauses, resulting in a dependent variable “Clause type” with two levels (1 for SC and 0 for matrix clause). We analyzed the use of SCs vs. matrix clauses using generalized binomial linear mixed effect models in R (R Core Team, 2021) and the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015). We maximally specified the fixed effects by including all theoretically relevant independent variables and their interactions: bilingualism (heritage bilingual/monolingual), setting (formal/informal), mode (spoken/written). We contrast-coded the factors using sum contrast coding (–0.5/0.5). We attempted to maximally specify the random effect of participants and included the random slopes for setting and mode (Barr et al., 2013). The maximal specification worked for German SCs, but not for English SCs, where it led to overfitting, so we removed the random slopes and left only the random intercept.

Second, each SC was coded for its type, resulting in a dependent variable “SC type” with three levels (complement, adverbial, and relative). Then, we recoded the dependent variable “SC type” into three separate dependent variables “Complement clause”, “Adverbial clause”, and “Relative clause” with two levels (1 and 0). After this manipulation, each SC type was analyzed independently from the other two types also using generalized binomial linear mixed effect models. Due to the small sample size of each SC type (Tables 1, 2), we collapsed the spoken and written modes within each setting and only included the independent variables of bilingualism (heritage bilingual/monolingual) and setting (formal/informal) and their interaction. We contrast-coded the factors using sum contrast coding (–0.5/0.5). Where possible, we maximally specified the random effect of participants by including the random slopes for setting. If this led to a perfect correlation of fixed effects or a random effect variance estimated at 0 or 1, we removed the random slope. In the next section, we report the z- and p-values of the models, for full model summaries, see Supplementary Appendix B.

Results

Majority and Monolingual English

Subordinate Clauses in English

For English SCs, we observed a main effect of setting (z = 4.70, p ≤ 0.001): speakers produced more SCs in the formal setting more than in the informal setting (Figure 1). In addition, we observed a three-way interaction between bilingualism, setting, and mode (z = 2.02, p = 0.043). To interpret this interaction, we ran separate models for HSs and MSs. HSs showed a main effect of setting (z = 2.71, p = 0.007), while MSs showed a main effect of setting (z = 4.04, p ≤ 0.001) and an interaction between setting and mode (z = –2.46, p = 0.014). Tukey’s multiple comparison test (MCT, run with emmeans package, Lenth, 2021) revealed a significant difference between the formal and the informal settings in the written mode (estimate = –0.51, SE = 0.16, z = –3.26, p = 0.006) and an absence of such a difference in the spoken mode (estimate = 0.19, SE = 0.16, z = 1.24, p = 0.602). This shows that German HSs and English MSs partially overlapped in their SC productions. While they behaved similarly in the written mode, they diverged in the spoken mode: HSs distinguished between the settings whereas MSs did not. Additionally, for both speaker groups, setting played a key role in SC production.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean proportion of SCs in English by speaker group and register.

Subordinate Clause Types in English

For English complement clauses, we observed a main effect of setting (z = –3.73, p ≤ 0.001): there were fewer complement clauses in the formal setting than in the informal one (Figure 2A). For English adverbial clauses and relative clauses, we did not observe any main effects or interactions (Figures 2B,C). These results indicate that German HSs and English MSs performed similarly regarding the production of all SC types, and formality played a role only for complement clauses, with fewer complement clauses in the formal setting.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean proportion of SC types in English by speaker group and formality: (A) Complement Clauses, (B) Adverbial Clauses, (C) Relative Clauses.

Heritage and Monolingual German

Subordinate Clauses in German

For German SCs, we observed two main effects and two interactions. First, there was a main effect of bilingualism (z = –3.55, p ≤ 0.001), with HSs producing fewer SCs than MSs (Figure 3). Second, we found a main effect of setting (z = 6.35, p ≤ 0.001): there were more SCs in the formal setting than in the informal setting. Then, we observed an interaction of setting and mode (z = –2.98, p = 0.003), with a greater difference between the formal and informal settings in the written mode (estimate = 1.08, SE = 0.18, z = 5.94, p ≤ 0.001) than in the spoken mode (estimate = 0.45, SE = 0.13, z = 3.37, p = 0.004), according to Tukey’s MCT. Finally, we observed a three-way interaction between bilingualism, setting, and mode. To interpret it, we ran separate models for HSs and MSs. The HS model indicated a main effect of setting (z = 4.61, p ≤ 0.001), with more SCs in the formal setting than in the informal setting. In addition, there was an interaction of setting and mode. Tukey’s MCT revealed a difference between the formal and informal setting in the written mode (estimate = 1.45, SE = 0.30, z = 4.84, p ≤ 0.001) but not in the spoken mode (estimate = 0.22, SE = 0.20, z = 1.09, p = 0.698). The MS model showed only a main effect of setting (z = 4.36, p ≤ 0.001). This shows that German HSs and MSs differed in the overall SC productions: while HSs distinguished between the settings only in the written mode, MSs did so in both modes. In addition, for both speaker groups, setting played a key role in SC production.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean proportion of SCs in German by speaker group and register.

Subordinate Clause Types in German

For German complement clauses, we observed a main effect of setting (z = –5.74, p ≤ 0.001), with fewer complement clauses in the formal setting than in the informal setting (Figure 4A). For adverbial clauses, we observed a main effect of setting (z = 2.90, p = 0.004), with more adverbial clauses in the formal setting than the informal setting (Figure 4B). For relative clauses, we observed a main effect of setting (z = 2.30, p = 0.022), with more relative clauses in the formal setting than the informal setting (Figure 4C). These results indicate that German HSs and German MSs performed similarly regarding the production of all SC types. Formality played a role for both speaker groups: they produced fewer complement clauses but more adverbial clauses and relative clauses in the formal setting than the informal setting.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean proportion of SC types in German by speaker group and formality: (A) Complement Clauses, (B) Adverbial Clauses, (C) Relative Clauses.

Discussion

This study aimed at presenting reasons for why the category “native speaker” is flawed and should be further specified to account for the variation between the groups that fall under its scope. Such a specification would enhance transparency and replicability of research. We analyzed two native speaker groups—HSs and MSs—to argue that there are differences and similarities, as well as a combination of both, between the groups. In particular, we compared German HSs residing in the United States with English and German MSs. We looked at the use of SCs and their types (complement, adverbial, and relative) in spoken and written narratives across registers.

Our first research question focused on whether HSs use finite SCs in a similar or different way in their majority language compared to English MSs and in their heritage language compared to German MSs. With respect to HSs’ majority language, our data does not confirm Hypothesis 1 and Prediction 1, which state that in their majority language, German HSs will perform similarly to English MSs. Overall, both speaker groups produce more SCs in the formal setting, confirming previous results, thus exhibiting similarity (see Pashkova et al., in press). This similarity is however only partial because a closer look at SC productions across registers reveals that HSs distinguished between the settings in spoken and written modes while MSs did so only in the written mode. With respect to HSs’ heritage language, our data confirms Hypothesis 1 and Prediction 1, which state that in their heritage language, German HSs will produce significantly fewer SCs than German MSs. Additionally, HSs distinguished between the settings only in the written mode, while MSs did so in both modes. This can be attributed to the cognitive load of spoken online productions in combination with the general complexity of SCs and word order differences in SCs in English and German (Pashkova et al., in press).

Our second research question zoomed in on the use of finite SC types according to formality. We wanted to know whether HSs would show similarities or differences in their majority language compared to English MSs and in their heritage language compared to German MSs. With respect to HSs’ majority language, our data confirms Hypothesis 2 and Prediction 2.1, which state that HSs and MSs should show similar frequencies of SC types across settings. With respect to HSs’ heritage language, our data does not confirm Hypothesis 2 and Prediction 2.1, which expect a combination of differences and similarities between HSs and MSs, because both speaker groups in fact behaved similarly regarding the frequencies of SC types across settings. Consequently, we did not find any support for Prediction 2.2, which argued for a bigger difference between HSs and MSs in the formal setting.

Overall, the results show that the locus of variation between HSs and MSs is not where we predicted it to be. For English SCs, we expected to find only similarities between HSs and MSs, and instead we observed a combination of differences and similarities. HSs adhere to formality distinctions regardless of mode, unlike English MSs, who do so only in the written mode. This could be attributed to the different attitudes toward our study among HSs and MSs: HSs were well aware that their language competence was under scrutiny, and were probably trying to show their best language skills. This is especially true for the heritage language but could also have influenced their performance in the majority language, which might explain their strict adherence to the formality distinction in both modes. This illustrates that the two groups of native speakers show variation in their performance, potentially due to extralinguistic factors such as their perception of the situation. Therefore, the category “native speaker” groups together speakers with different patterns of language use and is not specific enough to allow comparability in a speaker population.

Another unpredicted result is that in German, HSs behave similarly to MSs with regard to all SC types, even adverbial and relative clauses, which we expected to differ between the speaker groups due to their later acquisition and location at the syntax-discourse interface. This is contrary to the previous findings by Andreou et al. (2020a), who showed that the current language exposure influences the production of adverbial clauses by child HSs in a sentence repetition task. However, their participants were much younger than ours (mean age 9.01 vs. mean age 15.6), which could be the reason for the discrepancy in our results. Perhaps, the use of adverbial clauses is influenced not only by the current language exposure but also by the overall cognitive maturity of the speaker (see Paradis et al., 2017 on the advantages of higher cognitive maturity in early L2 acquisition). Furthermore, the absence of difference could be attributed to the relatively small sample size in this study, which could have prevented us from capturing it. Productions of more speakers need to be analyzed to confirm our result. The analysis of SC types and SCs in German illustrated that we can still find similarities within a narrower phenomenon (SC types) between the sub-groups of native speakers even if a more general phenomenon (SCs) shows differences between the same speaker groups.

An additional unexpected finding was that concerning SC types, HSs behaved similarly to German MSs in their heritage language and similarly to English MSs in their majority language, even though the MSs of the respective languages behaved differently—in English, formality only had an effect on complement clauses, whereas in German, formality had an effect on all SC types. This shows that German and English differ in their formality-related language use and that HSs are able to adapt to the MS pattern in both their languages. This is surprising since the HSs’ ability to adjust their SC type productions in their heritage language does not appear to originate from their exposure to formal registers in German or from transfer of their formality awareness from English into German. Further research is needed to pinpoint the source of this behavior.

The presented findings lead us to the conclusion that the category “native speaker” is too general to adequately define a speaker population because the speakers subsumed under this category may well differ in their linguistic behavior. Therefore, we argue for a more specific categorization, which provides more fine-grained information on their language background, allowing the possibility of capturing both group and individual variation, which are gradient (Ortega, 2020). Previous literature suggests that the category” “native speaker” should be replaced with “L1 user” (Dewaele, 2018). We argue for the necessity of further specification since even within L1 users, we can see differences as illustrated throughout this paper. This specification could include information on bilingualism, language exposure, proficiency, and dominance. In the current statistical analysis, we included only the variable of bilingualism in heritage language context. Further studies are needed to examine the influence of proficiency, language exposure, and dominance, which we expect to play a role in the variability among native speakers. Following this suggestion, for example, the majority of our German HSs could be described as bilinguals who are simultaneously raised in German and English, residing in the United States, with English as their current dominant language and German as their less dominant language. A typical German MS could be described as a monolingually-raised German speaker, residing in Germany, with German as their current dominant language.

One limitation of the present study, as already mentioned, is the relatively small sample size of the three SC types, which did not allow us to look into the interaction of bilingualism, formality and spoken/written mode. Since this interaction proved significant in the SC use, it would be very interesting to examine it in SC types as well. Due to a small sample size, we also were not able to assess potential qualitative differences in SC types (word order, choice of complementizer, or verb placement). Another possible extension of the current study is to examine further heritage-majority language pairs, probably typologically more distant, to see whether the patterns we describe here would manifest themselves in other native speaker groups. The RUEG corpus, which provided the data analyzed in this study, is a useful resource for such an extension since it contains comparable data for Greek, Turkish, and Russian HSs in Germany and the United States, plus data for their monolingual counterparts. Another aspect that could be addressed in future studies is the register-related language use in English, German, and possibly other languages. It is noteworthy that English and German MSs in our study did not behave similarly with respect to formality, and further research would be needed to uncover the possible sources of this difference. An additional step could be the inclusion of a wider range of registers with the same formality and mode distinctions, to see whether the formality sensitivity is tied to a particular situation (e.g., a police report) or if it is more general.

Conclusion

This study investigated the appropriateness of the category “native speaker” by comparing productions of two native speaker groups, namely heritage and monolingual speakers. We assessed the use of SCs and their types (complement, adverbial, and relative) in narratives produced by adolescent HSs of German in the United States in comparison with adolescent German and English MSs. We provided evidence that there are similarities, differences, and a combination of both in the productions of HSs and MSs. Our results show similarities in the production of SC types between HSs’ majority English and monolingual English, as well as between heritage and monolingual German. Differences were found in SC productions between heritage and monolingual German. A combination of differences and similarities was found in SC productions between majority and monolingual English. These findings support existing criticism of the category “native speaker” and further highlight its underspecification. As is, the category fails to adequately reflect the variation among speaker groups who fall under its scope. Therefore, we argue that we should enhance the category “native speaker” with more specific descriptions of speaker groups in order to provide unambiguous information about them.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: RUEG corpus https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3236068.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft ethics committee and the Institutional Review Board (IRB) University of Maryland at College-Park. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The research results presented in this publication were funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) as part of the research unit Emerging grammars in language contact situations: a comparative approach (FOR 2537) in projects P5 (project no. 394995401, GZ TR 238/5-1 and P2 (project no. 394837597, GZ AL 1886/2-1).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank all project members and research assistants. The publication of this article was funded by the Baden-Württemberg Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts and by the University of Mannheim.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.717352/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ Unintroduced subordinate clauses require verb-raising into second position, as in main clauses. Those cases are also accounted for in this study, see section “Data Coding.”
  2. ^ Due to the small sample size of SC types, we decided to collapse the four registers (formal spoken, formal written, informal spoken, informal written) into two formality conditions—formal vs. informal.
  3. ^ Questionnaire for adolescent participants of the Research Unit Emerging Grammars; https://osf.io/qhupg/
  4. ^ We also included seven non-canonical V2 clauses clearly conceptualized as SCs: three complement clauses, two adverbial clauses, two relative clauses. We did not conduct a separate analysis V2 SCs due to their low frequency.
  5. ^ We did not conduct fine-grained qualitative analyses of SC types such as examining word order, choice of complementizers or verb placement, although these characteristics are definitely worth exploring in further research. We did so since any further subdivision on the data would result in a too low number of data points in each subcategory to conduct a statistical analysis.
  6. ^ The participant code in the examples includes the following information: US/DE, country of elicitation, United States or Germany; bi/mo, bilingual/monolingual speaker; 01, speaker number; M/F, speaker’s sex; D/E, HS’s heritage language (Deutsch for German) or monolinguals’ L1 (English or German); f/i, formal/informal setting; s/w, spoken/written mode; D/E, language of elicitation, D for German or E for English.
  7. ^ We preserved the original orthography of the written productions.

References

Aalberse, S., Backus, A., and Muysken, P. (2019). Heritage languages: A language contact approach, Vol. 58. Amsterdam: Benjamins, doi: 10.1075/sibil.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abutalebi, J., and Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 19, 689–698. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albirini, A., and Benmamoun, E. (2014). Aspects of second-language transfer in the oral production of Egyptian and Palestinian heritage speakers. Int. J. Bilingual. 18, 244–273. doi: 10.1177/1367006912441729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreou, M. (2015). The Effects of Bilingualism on Verbal and Non Verbal Cognition: The Micro- and Macro-Structure of Narratives in the Weak and the Dominant Language of the Bilingual Child. Ph.D thesis. Thessaloniki: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.11670.29767

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreou, M., and Tsimpli, I. M. (2020). “Bilingualism, biliteracy and syntactic complexity: The role of crosslinguistic influence and cognitive skills,” in Language Acquisition, Processing and Bilingualism: Selected Papers from the Romance Turn VII, eds A. Cardinaletti, C. Branchini, G. Giusti, and F. Volpato (Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing), 132–159.

Google Scholar

Andreou, M., Torregrossa, J., and Bongartz, C. (2020a). “Sentence Repetition Task as a Measure of Language Dominance,” in BUCLD 45 Proceedings, (Boston: Boston University).

Google Scholar

Andreou, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Durrleman, S., and Peristeri, E. (2020b). Theory of Mind, Executive Functions, and Syntax in Bilingual Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Languages 5:67. doi: 10.3390/languages5040067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews, A. D. (2007). “Relative clauses,” in Language Typology and Syntactic Description, ed. T. Shopen (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 206–236. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511619434.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antomo, M., and Steinbach, M. (2010). Desintegration und interpretation: Weil-V2-sätze an der schnittstelle zwischen syntax, semantik und pragmatik. Zeitschrift Fur Sprachwissenschaft 29, 1–37. doi: 10.1515/ZFSW.2010.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azar, Z., Backus, A., and Özyürek, A. (2019). General- and Language-Specific Factors Influence Reference Tracking in Speech and Gesture in Discourse. Discourse Proces. 56, 553–574. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2018.1519368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., and Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaman, K. (1984). “Coordination and subordination revisited: Syntactic complexity in spoken and written narrative discourse,” in Coherence in Spoken and Written Discourse, eds D. Tannen and R. Freedle (Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing), 45–80.

Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Conrad, S. (2001). “Register Variation: A Corpus Approach,” in The Handbook of Discourse Analysis, eds D. Schiffrin, D. Tannen, and H. E. Hamilton (New Jersey, NJ: Blackwell Publishers), 175–196.

Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Gray, B. (2016). Grammatical Complexity in Academic English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511920776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., and Finegan, E. (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. London: Pearson Educaiton Limited.

Google Scholar

Bonfiglio, T. P. (2010). Mother tongues and nations: The invention of the native speaker. Berlin: De Gruyter.

Google Scholar

Brehmer, B., and Usanova, I. (2015). “Let’s fix it? Cross-linguistic influence in word order patterns of Russian heritage speakers in Germany,” in Transfer Effects in Multilingual Language Development, ed. H. Peukert (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 161–188. doi: 10.1075/hsld.4.08bre

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulte, B., and Housen, A. (2014). Conceptualizing and measuring short-term changes in L2 writing complexity. J. Second Lang. Writ. 26, 42–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2014.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, V. (1999). Going beyond the Native Speaker in Language Teaching. TESOL Quart. 33:185. doi: 10.2307/3587717

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, V. (2016). Where Is the Native Speaker Now? TESOL Quart. 50, 186–189. doi: 10.1002/tesq.286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, A. (2003). The native speaker: Myth and reality. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, doi: 10.21832/9781853596247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, A. (2004). “The native speaker in applied linguistics,” in The Handbook of Applied Linguistics, eds A. Davies and C. Elder (Oxford: Blackwell), 431–450. doi: 10.1002/9780470757000.ch17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, A. (2013). Native Speakers and Native Users. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139022316

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J. M. (2018). Why the Dichotomy “L1 Versus LX User” is Better than “Native Versus Non-native Speaker.”. Appl. Linguist. 39, 236–240. doi: 10.1093/applin/amw055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diessel, H. (2004). The Acquisition of Complex Sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511486531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goschler, J., Schroeder, C., and Woerfel, T. (2020). “Convergence in the encoding of motion events in heritage Turkish in Germany,” in Studies in Turkish as a Heritagae Language, ed. F. Bayram (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 87–103. doi: 10.1075/sibil.60.05gos

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holliday, A. (2009). “English as a lingua franca, ‘non-native speakers’ and cosmopolitan realities,” in English as an International Language: Perspectives and Pedagogical Issues, ed. F. Sharifian (Bristol: Channel View Publications), 19–33. doi: 10.21832/9781847691231-005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopp, H. (2016). Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Lang. Res. 32, 277–307. doi: 10.1177/0267658315624960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Housen, A., Kuiken, F., and Vedder, I. (2012). “Complexity, accuracy and fluency,” in Dimensions of L2 Performance and Proficiency: Complexity, Accuracy and Fluency in SLA, eds A. Housen, F. Kuiken, and I. Vedder (Amsterdam: John Bejamins), 1–20. doi: 10.1075/lllt.32.01hou

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Housen, A., De Clercq, B., Kuiken, F., and Vedder, I. (2019). Multiple approaches to complexity in second language research. Second Lang. Res. 35, 3–21. doi: 10.1177/0267658318809765

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ionin, T., Choi, S. H., and Liu, Q. (2021). Knowledge of indefinite articles in L2-English: Online vs. offline performance. Second Lang. Res. 37, 121–160. doi: 10.1177/0267658319857466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, P., and Oesterreicher, W. (2012). “Language of immediacy – language of distance: orality and literacy from the perspective of language theory and linguistic history,” in Communicative Spaces: Variation, Contact, Change. Papers in Honour of Ursula Schaefer, eds C. Lange, B. Weber, and G. Wolf (Frankfurt: Peter Lang), 441–473.

Google Scholar

Kupisch, T., and Rothman, J. (2018). Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. Int. J. Bilingual. 22, 564–582. doi: 10.1177/1367006916654355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupisch, T., Barton, D., Hailer, K., Klaschik, E., Stangen, I., Lein, T., et al. (2014). Foreign Accent in Adult Simultaneous Bilinguals. Heritage Lang. J. 11, 123–150. doi: 10.46538/hlj.11.2.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenth, R. V. (2021). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R Package Version 1.6.1. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans

Google Scholar

Lowe, R. J. (2020). Uncovering Ideology in English Language Teaching. Berlin: Springer.

Google Scholar

Lust, B., Foley, C., and Dye, C. D. (2015). “The first language acquisition of complex sentences,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language, eds E. L. Bavin and L. R. Naigles (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 298–323.

Google Scholar

Łyskawa, P., and Nagy, N. (2020). Case Marking Variation in Heritage Slavic Languages in Toronto: Not So Different. Lang. Learn. 70, 122–156. doi: 10.1111/lang.12348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastropavlou, M., and Tsimpli, I. M. (2011). Complementizers and subordination in typical language acquisition and SLI. Lingua 121, 442–462. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montrul, S. (2016). The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139030502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, N. (2015). A sociolinguistic view of null subjects and VOT in Toronto heritage languages. Lingua 164, 309–327. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2014.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, N., and Lo, S. (2019). Classifier use in Heritage and Hong Kong Cantonese. Asia Pacific Lang. Variat. 5, 84–108. doi: 10.1075/aplv.17001.nag

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neary-Sundquist, C. A. (2017). Syntactic complexity at multiple proficiency levels of L2 German speech. Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 27, 242–262. doi: 10.1111/ijal.12128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noonan, M. (2007). “Complementation,” in Language Typology and Syntactic Description, ed. T. Shopen (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 52–150. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511619434.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega, L. (2020). The Study of Heritage Language Development From a Bilingualism and Social Justice Perspective. Lang. Learn. 70, 15–53. doi: 10.1111/lang.12347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradis, J. (2019). “English Second Language Acquisition from Early Childhood to Adulthood: The Role of Age, First Language, Cognitive, and Input Factors,” in Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development, eds M. M. Brown and B. Dailey (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press), 11–26.

Google Scholar

Paradis, J., Rusk, B., Duncan, T. S., and Govindarajan, K. (2017). Children’s Second Language Acquisition of English Complex Syntax: The Role of Age, Input, and Cognitive Factors. Annu. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 37, 148–167. doi: 10.1017/S0267190517000022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual, Y., Cabo, D., and Rothman, J. (2012). The (IL)logical problem of heritage speaker bilingualism and incomplete acquisition. Appl. Linguist. 33, 450–455. doi: 10.1093/applin/ams037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peristeri, E., Andreou, M., and Tsimpli, I. M. (2017). Syntactic and Story Structure Complexity in the Narratives of High- and Low-Language Ability Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychol. 8:02027. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polinsky, M. (2008). Gender under incomplete acquisition: heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Herit. Lang. J. 6, 40–71. doi: 10.46538/hlj.6.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polinsky, M. (2011). Reanalysis in Adult Heritage Language: New evidence in support of attrition. Stud. Second Lang. Acquisit. 33, 305–328. doi: 10.1017/S027226311000077X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polinsky, M. (2018). Heritage Languages and Their Speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/9781107252349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polinsky, M., and Scontras, G. (2020). A roadmap for heritage language research. Bilingual. Lang. Cognit. 23, 50–55. doi: 10.1017/S1366728919000555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Redl, T., Frank, S. L., de Swart, P., and de Hoop, H. (2021). The male bias of a generically-intended masculine pronoun: Evidence from eye-tracking and sentence evaluation. PLoS One 16:e0249309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, M. (2013). Weil-V2”-Sätze und (k)ein Ende? Anmerkungen zur Analyse von Antomo & Steinbach (2010). Zeitschrift Fur Sprachwissenschaft 32, 221–262. doi: 10.1515/zfs-2013-0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. C., and Schmidt, R. W. (2013). Longman Dictionary of Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics, 4th Edn. London: Routledge, doi: 10.4324/9781315833835

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, J. (2007). Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in Heritage Brazilian Portuguese. Int. J. Bilingual. 11, 359–389. doi: 10.1177/13670069070110040201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, J., and Treffers-Daller, J. (2014). A prolegomenon to the construct of the native speaker: Heritage speaker Bilinguals are Natives Too! Appl. Linguist. 35, 93–98. doi: 10.1093/applin/amt049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RUEG group (2018). RUEG Language Situations Elicitation 2018/19. Charlottesville, VA: OSF.

Google Scholar

Sánchez Abchi, V., and De Mier, V. (2017). Syntactic complexity in narratives written by Spanish heritage speakers. Vigo Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 14, 125–148.

Google Scholar

Sanfelici, E., and Schulz, P. (2021). Can frequency account for the grammatical choices of children and adults in nominal modification contexts? Evidence from elicited production and child-directed speech. Languages 6, 1–31. doi: 10.3390/languages6010035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schleppegrell, M. J., and Colombi, M. C. (1997). Text Organization by Bilingual Writers. Written Commun. 14, 481–503.

Google Scholar

Schulz, P., and Grimm, A. (2019). The age factor revisited: Timing in acquisition interacts with age of onset in bilingual acquisition. Front. Psychol. 9:02732. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., and Fuchs, Z. (2018). In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa J. General Linguist. 3:1. doi: 10.5334/gjgl.164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., Tsai, C.-Y. E., and Mai, K. (2017). Cross-linguistic scope ambiguity: When two systems meet. Glossa J. General Linguist. 2, 1–28. doi: 10.5334/gjgl.198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguist. Approaches Bilingual. 1, 1–33. doi: 10.1075/lab.1.1.01sor

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S. A., Longacre, R. E., and Hwang, S. J. J. (2007). “Adverbial clauses,” in Language Typology and Syntactic Description, Second Edition: Volume II: Complex Constructions, ed. T. Shopen (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 237–300. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511619434.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsimpli, I. M. (2014). Early, late or very late? Linguist. Approac. Bilingual. 4, 283–313. doi: 10.1075/lab.4.3.01tsi

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., and Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Emergence of syntax: Commonalities and differences across children. Dev. Sci. 11, 84–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00656.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Tao, S. (2020). “The dynamic co-development of linguistic and discourse-semantic complexity in advanced L2 writing,” in Complex Dynamic Systems Theory and L2 Writing Development, eds G. G. Fogal, and M. H. Verspoor (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 49–78.

Google Scholar

Wiese, H. (2020). “Language Situations: A method for capturing variation within speakers’ repertoires,” in Methods in Dialectology XVI, ed. Y. Asahi (Bern: Peter Lang), 105–117.

Google Scholar

Wiese, H., Alexiadou, A., Allen, S., Bunk, O., Gagarina, N., Iefremenko, K., et al. (2020). RUEG Corpus. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.3236068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yager, L., Hellmold, N., Joo, H. A., Putnam, M. T., Rossi, E., Stafford, C., et al. (2015). New structural patterns in moribund grammar: Case marking in heritage German. Front. Psychol. 6:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer, C. (2020). Kasus im Namdeutschen. Zeitschrift Für Germanistische Linguistik 48, 298–335. doi: 10.1515/zgl-2020-2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: native speakers, heritage speakers, subordinate clauses, heritage German, majority language

Citation: Tsehaye W, Pashkova T, Tracy R and Allen SEM (2021) Deconstructing the Native Speaker: Further Evidence From Heritage Speakers for Why This Horse Should Be Dead! Front. Psychol. 12:717352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.717352

Received: 30 May 2021; Accepted: 13 September 2021;
Published: 05 October 2021.

Edited by:

Valentin Vulchanov, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Norway

Reviewed by:

Maria M. Arredondo, University of Texas at Austin, United States
Jason Rothman, Arctic University of Norway, Norway

Copyright © 2021 Tsehaye, Pashkova, Tracy and Allen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wintai Tsehaye, wtsehaye@mail.uni-mannheim.de; Tatiana Pashkova, pashkova@rhrk.uni-kl.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.