Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 29 October 2021
Sec. Positive Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Positive Psychology in Everyday Life View all 38 articles

A Summary of Current Findings on Quality of Life Domains and a Proposal for Their Inclusion in Clinical Interventions

  • College of Science, Health, Engineering and Education (SHEE), Murdoch University, Perth, WA, Australia

Whilst the assessment of quality of life (QoL) and well-being has burgeoned in the past 50 years, there still remains relatively little research into its treatment in psychology, in spite of the launching of such approaches as positive psychology to widen the ambit of interventions to promote well-being. We posit that there are a number of outstanding QoL areas that could be integrated into standard therapeutic procedures, and that this would this result in an increase in well-being as a therapeutic outcome. To investigate this an exploratory search of the literature was undertaken of associations between improvements in a life domain and increased well-being or QoL. Ten domains (relationships, work, money, health, and leisure, mindfulness, self-esteem, resolution of past life events, mental style and life management skills) were identified. In view of the substantial evidence of the cumulative impact of these domains upon well-being, it is proposed that conducting a unidimensional clinical intervention that focuses only on the presenting issue is not sufficient. Implications and possible therapeutic pathways are discussed and it is recommended that practitioners include such QoL domains in their assessment, case formulation, and intervention planning.

Introduction

The range of evidence-based clinical interventions has certainly grown over recent decades and has gained both popularity and public credibility (Barlow et al., 2016). One approach, however, that has received comparatively less attention is the investigation of domains that are both therapeutically amenable to change and correlate with perceived quality of life (QoL).

Whilst there is now a sizeable amount of research into the predictors of QoL there remains a lack of communication between researchers of QoL and clinicians to assist in the integration of relevant findings, so much so this has even spawned review articles entitled “Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being (SWB)” (Diener et al., 2017) to try to address this dilemma. This lack of cross-pollination has led to research vital to well-being being left undiscovered in clinical fields.

Objectives

In response to this, we raise the question as to whether there are a number of outstanding QoL areas that could be integrated into standard therapeutic procedures to improve intervention outcomes. And if so we ask what some of the possible approaches could be that may be taken for this to be achieved. As such the objective of this study is to both investigate QoL domains relevant to clinical interventions and, if applicable to identify feasible avenues for their inclusion by clinicians seeking to improve intervention efficacy. The above objectives take up Veenhoven’s (1991) early and largely unheeded call that research that dealt with some of the principles or practices of happiness would provide a good foundation for the development of QoL interventions.

It is postulated that the addition of such domains as part of a therapeutic intervention, will result in an overall increase in both perceived and self-reported well-being, a stated goal of therapy. Such a view is founded on the notion that QoL is a multi-factorial phenomenon, one impacted by a broad range of both objective and subjective influences. Hence it seems reasonable to hypothesize that including more of the domains that make up the overall experience of well-being, will result in an overall increased happiness quotient, as opposed to primarily targeting the presenting issue.

Conceptualizing Quality of Life

In the early research into QoL, major life areas such as relationships, work, money, health and leisure were seen to play a critical role and hence received much attention (Andrews and Withey, 1976; Campbell et al., 1976; Diener, 1984; Argyle, 1987; Veenhoven, 1991; Diener et al., 1999). For example, in seminal research by Campbell et al. (1976) involving 2,164 respondents, life satisfaction correlated with the following domains: Family Life (0.41); Marriage (0.36); Financial Situation (0.33); Housing (0.30); Job (0.27); Friendship (0.26); Health (0.22); and Leisure (0.21). However, the authors found that the major domains of life satisfaction still only accounted for 15% of the variance in happiness.

Similarly, Andrews and Withey (1976) found that whilst age, sex and occupation correlated with well-being, these variables accounted for only 8% of the variance. Likewise, Kammann (1983) reported that most objective life experiences accounted for no more than a staggering 5% of the variance in measures of well-being. Furthermore, he found that the combination of dozens of life satisfaction domains accounted for fewer than 10% of the variance, leaving him to assert that “objective” life circumstances had a counter-intuitively small impact because they were influenced by multiple “subjective” cognitive processes. It is possibly the complexity of this interaction that has made it hard to integrate into standardized interventions. As such we often find a polarization between goal setting programs (Locke and Latham, 2013) and cognitively based interventions (Hawthorne et al., 2019) rather than a multi-dimensional intervention.

Again, in early seminal research Easterlin (1974, 1995) reported that Americans in the post war decades did not demonstrate an increase in happiness even though income per head had doubled, leading to the “Easterlin paradox” which asserts that circumstances will influence well-being for a limited time before moving back to a set point. Myers (2000) later review over a five-decade span post World War II again found that income in real terms (accounting for inflation) had risen dramatically over this period, yet well-being had not shifted significantly. These findings were in line with Biswas-Diener and Diener’s (2001) research that income had a controversial but generally weak association with happiness, with average estimates being not more than 2–5% of SWB.

Argyle (1999) found that competencies such as intelligence and physical attractiveness had very weak positive correlations with happiness. This accorded with Lyubomirsky (2001) who also found that objective circumstances, demographic variables and life events correlated minimally with SWB. Argyle also found that age had a small positive effect on some aspects of happiness but suggested that this was due to a declining goal-achievement gap. However, this contrasted with later studies that found either no substantial difference in well-being across age (Siedlecki et al., 2013) or U-shaped age profiles in positive hedonic well-being after middle age and a decline in negative hedonic well-being variables such as stress and anger from the early 20s (Blanchflower and Oswald, 2008; Stone et al., 2010; Muhli and Svensson, 2017).

Again, early studies such as that of Brickman et al. (1978) on the transient psychological impact of lottery winning and paraplegia represented sine qua non-examples of the failure of objective variables to predict shifts in well-being. This phenomenon was replicated in terms of health which correlated minimally with well-being (Okun and George, 1984). To highlight again the complexity of the relationship between subjective and objective factors, research on the “perception” of one’s general health status was found to correlate more strongly with QoL ratings than “actual” health (George and Landerman, 1984; Argyle, 1987). This is another example of how research findings may lead clinicians to be ambivalent about what they should focus on.

There were attempts, however, to integrate this and explain why such a broad range of life areas, both individually and collectively, were so poorly correlated with the perception of QoL. For example, the Hedonic Adaptation Prevention model was proposed which suggests that people adapt to positive life changes due to the progressive decline in the number of positive events and emotions associated with the once new change. This is coupled with an increase in people’s aspirations for more once they have achieved a goal, which leads to a dissatisfaction with their current level and a gradual return to their well-being baseline (Armenta et al., 2014).

Cummins (2003, 2016) has subsequently suggested that the interaction of these variables occurs within a psycho-social system that maintains well-being homeostasis within a narrow range. He suggested that internal buffers such as self-esteem, optimism, and belief in perceived control along with external buffers made up of resources such as receiving personal assistance, ameliorate the impact of negative events. Cummins acknowledged that poor objective conditions could defeat homeostasis, however, there was typically an expected covariation between objective and subjective indicators.

Over time in response to the poor predictive performance of objective factors there was a shift toward the subjective factors that might play a role in predicting QoL ratings (Sirgy et al., 2006). As the research evolved, along with both straddling the issues of measurement (Kimberlin and Winterstein, 2008), and beginning to include the concepts of well-being and life satisfaction, subjective factors began to take on a far larger role, highlighting that the perception of QoL was indeed a multi-factorial construct. These included the dominant role of affect (Lykken and Tellegen, 1996; Carter, 2004) and the diverse range of cognitive processes implicated in the assessment of one’s well-being (Lyubomirsky, 2001; Layous and Lyubomirsky, 2013).

In an attempt to capture the construct more fully, Diener et al. (1999) carried out an extensive three-decade review of the QoL research and found further evidence to support the multifactorial quality of happiness. In a somewhat reductionist distillation of findings, they found that happy people were most likely to be at the top of the social ladder, typically married and got on well with friends and family. They usually were healthy both physically and mentally, were active and open-minded and felt in control of their lives. In addition, their aspirations concerned social and moral matters rather than money-making, and they frequently were on the conservative side of the political divide.

In terms of objective domains, Diener et al. (1999) in their review identified six domain satisfactions: work, leisure, health, finances, family and one’s group. In a subsequent review, Sin and Lyubomirsky (2009), combined results from 51 randomized controlled interventions and found, in terms of subjective factors, that people engaging in a positive mental style such as thinking gratefully or optimistically or demonstrating increased mindfulness, became significantly happier (Layous and Lyubomirsky, 2013).

In addition, people’s well-being was also found to receive a boost from a range of other factors like positive life events (Kahneman et al., 1999; Lucas et al., 2003), stable positive self-concept (Huebner et al., 1999; Hutz et al., 2014), or the ability to set goals and be self-directing (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Steca et al., 2015; Threadgill and Gable, 2018). With the added inclusion of subjective factors along with the objective life domains (how I feel about my life circumstances), much more of the variance in well-being was explained (Vella-Brodrick et al., 2008; Geerling and Diener, 2018).

Previous Efforts to Integrate Happiness Research and Therapy

When we look at the early research, we find the call to integrate QoL research into standard therapeutic procedures has been ever present. For example, seminal researchers like Fordyce (1977) lamented that “There is, however, one very important area of happiness research that has not yet received attention—attempts to increase personal happiness” (p. 510). In response to this he developed one of the first packaged happiness enhancement training programs known as the Personal Happiness Enhancement Program (PHEP). Based on the happiness literature Fordyce (1983) subsequently identified what he saw as fundamental cognitions and behaviors which he operationalized into 14 fundamental strategies.

Fordyce (1983) tested his program with 64 both male and female students (mean age = 24.5 years). He reported that “81% of subjects claimed actual increases in their happiness as a result of their learnings” (p. 489). Smith et al. (1995) added to this program by investigating the effect of combining the PHEP with meditation training. They found that the meditation plus PHEP group significantly improved on all dependent measures over the control group and the PHEP group.

One would have thought that such early and promising beginnings would have heralded a new age in clinically oriented QoL interventions. However, most QoL researchers maintained their focus on predictors and assessment, whilst the majority of clinical interventions research continued to favor pathology and symptom reduction over QoL and well-being promotion.

There have, however, been some exceptions and approaches that have aimed to bring QoL research into clinical settings. For example, Seligman and Csíkszentmihályi’s (2000) launching of positive psychology aimed to widen the ambit of clinical interventions to promote well-being and focused on three components: positive institutions, positive experiences and emotions, and positive attributes. Over the years positive psychology has become a credible based-evidence field focused on how to assist people to improve their lives and flourish. It’s focus is on developing practical tools and techniques to improve peoples’ lives.

Peterson and Seligman (2004) subsequently produced the Values-in-Action Classification of Character Strengths which identified 24 character strengths that together produced six higher order virtues: wisdom, courage, humanity, justice, temperance, and transcendence (Ruch et al., 2019). Similarly, around this time, Frisch (1994, 2016) developed QoL therapy, where participants are taught skills to set goals in sixteen areas of life correlated with well-being. Participants are encouraged to either: change their circumstances, attitude, goals and standards, priorities, or boost their satisfaction in other areas.

Over the years several models developed that can be divided into hedonic and eudaimonic conceptualizations of well-being (Hanley et al., 2014). The former, typically referred to as SWB, defines well-being more in terms of the presence of positive affect, absence of negative affect (Kahneman et al., 1999), and the cognitive appraisal of life satisfaction (Diener et al., 1985). By contrast the eudaimonic approach, often referred to as psychological well-being or happiness, focuses more on psychological functioning, human potential and meaning (Ryff and Keyes, 1995; Peterson and Seligman, 2004).

In some research this division has been reconceptualized as well-being processes (eudaimonic processes of engagement) and well-being outcomes (hedonic affects such as life satisfaction and affect) (Bhullar et al., 2013). However, what is shared in these diverse conceptualizations is an agreement that well-being is a multi-factorial construct (Michalos, 2017), hence targeting it with unidimensional interventions could be naïve and even unscientific.

The above observations indicate that there have been sporadic and yet convincing attempts to bring more of a health focus to traditionally pathology focused approaches, yet the latter remains the dominant paradigm. We argue, in view of the insufficient dialogue, that it is imperative to make headway in building the relationship between psychological interventions and QoL research. To bring to the surface more well-being data relevant to therapy, we will briefly summarize some of the findings from 10 QoL domains and discuss the well-being implications for clinical interventions.

Methods

In response to the research question that asked what influential QoL areas could be integrated into therapy, an exploratory search was conducted of empirical studies that reported significant correlations between a life domain and well-being or perceived QoL. Psychology databases PsycINFO and PsycARTICLES were searched with the keywords of “quality of life,” “subjective well-being,” and “empirical studies,” between 1985 and 2017 to include foundational literature and more recent contributions that built upon these foundations. Search results were divided into so-called objective life areas (such as money or health) and subjective components (mental style topics such as self-esteem or mindfulness).

As the rationale of the inquiry was to show evidence of the impact of a range of life domains upon QoL, and to then argue for their inclusion in therapy and group programs, the number of areas chosen was limited to a clinically manageable 10 areas (five objective and five subjective life domains). A limited number of life areas is seen as manageable within planned clinical interventions (Sakiris and Berle, 2019), and was in line with recommendations from Barlow et al. (2016) who propose “the possibility of distilling a set of psychological procedures that would comprise a unified intervention” (p. 838).

The selection criteria for the final 10 areas were based upon weighing up frequently occurring domains in the literature with the areas reported as having the most impact upon well-being or perceived QoL. For the objective domains there was sufficient evidence that the following areas had a significant correlation with well-being and were worthy of further consideration: relationships, work, money, health and leisure. Similarly for the more subjective domains, mindfulness, self-esteem, resolution of past life events, mental style, and life management (administration) were chosen for investigation. A minimum of 10 studies per domain was seen as a sufficient number to cover different dimensions of each domain.

Summary of Current Findings on 10 Quality of Life Areas

The following section briefly summarizes the findings from the QoL areas that were identified in the exploratory search of empirical studies that reported significant correlations between life domains and well-being (for more detail on the specific facets see Supplementary Appendix).

Relationships

Seen as pivotal in the experience of well-being, relationships are the most significant of all the life conditions (Argyle, 1987; Headey and Wearing, 1988; Inglehart, 1990; Demir and Weitekamp, 2007; Proulx et al., 2007; Carr et al., 2014). Multi-axial in nature, relationships can include intimate relationships, family, friends, and colleagues, with satisfaction mediated by the perception of connection, bonding and nourishing interaction with others (Lucas and Dyrenforth, 2006; Gere and Schimmack, 2011).

Close bonds may generate both positive and negative effects on well-being. A case in point is Warr and Payne’s (1982) early study that found that for 1,964 men and 1,113 women in Britain, the family emerged as a major source of pleasure and strain. For women (41%), particularly housewives (56%) the family was a source of strain, although conversely it was identified as a source of pleasure by 31% of the men and 37% of the women. Of all relationship states, marriage has been found to have the strongest effect, with the married being on average most happy, the divorced second and separated last (Myers, 1999; Gottman et al., 2017).

Relationships deliver the experience of intimacy and closeness (Reis, 2012), provide a buffer for stress and social support (Brannan et al., 2012), and help to achieve better health outcomes (Taylor, 2010; Tay et al., 2013). Similarly, the more social relationships people have, the more positive their sense of well-being (Lucas et al., 2008; Tan and Tay, 2017). Whilst falling out with others or reporting a lower relationship quality can have a negative effect upon happiness (Williams, 2009; Bookwala, 2014), relationship health has responded well to both cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and behavioral marital therapy (Shadish and Baldwin, 2005; Nirmalan, 2014).

Work

Work is also considered to play a significant role in the perception of QoL (Argyle, 1999; Van Katwyk et al., 2000) and includes the work conditions and psychological processes under which a sense of well-being at work evolves (Cranny et al., 1992). Employee counseling (Ahmad, 2013) and supportive management has been found to affect vocational satisfaction and QoL reports (Monnot and Beehr, 2014), whilst obstructive management and conflict with program mandates (Shier and Graham, 2013) has been linked to lower SWB (Mathieu et al., 2014).

Feeling stimulated and achieving success at work has also been consistently linked to better health and greater life satisfaction (Shimazu et al., 2015). Conscientiousness or the propensity to be goal directed, plan and delay gratification in work settings is positively related to job satisfaction and SWB (Judge et al., 2005; Carter et al., 2015), although some have argued it has a curvilinear relationship to work outcomes (Astakhova, 2014). The satisfaction of an individual’s basic psychological needs at work relates positively both to their performance and personal well-being (Bryson et al., 2017), and having autonomy for decision making and work planning plays a significant role in the maintenance of that well-being.

Whilst a comprehensive research focus for decades has identified many variables associated with work satisfaction it has also drawn criticism at the lack of defined job satisfaction constructs (Büssing and Bissels, 1998; Eschleman and Bowling, 2009). Similarly, it has been criticized for distortion effects such as the unclear partialling out of work satisfaction from other independent dimensions such as social desirability, cognitive dissonance or attitudinal variables (Unanue et al., 2017).

Money

Money also appears to play a small role in the perception of QoL, albeit a limited but complex one. Haggerty and Veenhoven’s (2003) longitudinal study of the relationship between life satisfaction and gross domestic product/person across 21 nations revealed that increasing national income correlates with increased national happiness. Whilst the findings were mixed (Blanchflower and Oswald, 2004; Davies, 2010), the effects were generally positive (Stevenson and Wolfers, 2008).

Diener and Biswas-Diener (2009) found that whilst there are strong correlations between national wealth and reports of SWB, there are mostly small correlations between income and well-being within those nations. Similarly, whilst economic growth is accompanied by only a small rise in SWB (Böckerman et al., 2015), increases in individual income can lead to increased well-being when it means avoiding poverty or allows the individual to fulfill material desires in keeping with their values (Srivastava et al., 2001).

However, whilst money can be a robust indicator of SWB (Dolan and White, 2007), if material goals are prized more than other values people appear to be less happy (Diener and Biswas-Diener, 2009). By contrast, financial problems are a strong predictor of clinically diagnosed depression (Huppert and So, 2011), and how people use their income (e.g., spending income on others’ predicted happiness) also impacts on well-being (Dunn et al., 2008). In response to this, financial counseling and goal setting have been found to have positive outcomes on a range of measures including financial capability, health and well-being (Brackertz, 2013).

Health

The burgeoning area of health-related QoL research highlights how highly health is held in terms of its impact upon QoL (Cummins et al., 2004; Salvador-Carulla et al., 2014; Brazier and Tsuchiya, 2015). Lee et al. (2013) highlight a reciprocal relationship between health status and QoL by arguing for the relevance of well-being for resource allocation decisions within health and social care. For example, positive and negative emotions have been found to correlate with symptom checklists (Krampen, 1999), along with other health-related QoL indicators such as asthma (Christie et al., 1991), physical functioning, pain and general health (Jenkinson et al., 1993; Karimi and Brazier, 2016).

The relationship between health and SWB also appears to be bidirectional. For example, people with illnesses such as heart disease, arthritis, and lung disease often present with increased levels of depressed mood and impaired hedonic and eudemonic well-being (Boehm and Kubzansky, 2012). Conversely, Steptoe et al. (2015) analysis of a longitudinal study identified that reduced well-being was associated with decreased survival (29.3% of people in the lowest well-being quartile died during the 8 year follow-up period compared with only 9.3% of those in the highest quartile).

In terms of interventions, this two-way relationship can be influenced by either regular physical activity to increase well-being (Windle et al., 2010), or actions taken to protect well-being by reducing cortisol output during the day (Steptoe et al., 2012). Personal training interventions that incorporate both aerobic and anaerobic exercise (Patel et al., 2017) improve health-related QoL and cognitive function (Sjøgaard et al., 2016).

Leisure

The research on leisure has identified a small but significant positive association with mental health, personal needs gratification (Tinsley and Eldredge, 1995; Lloyd and Auld, 2002; Tinsley et al., 2002) and life satisfaction (Rodríguez et al., 2007). Identified as a multidimensional construct that encompasses both structural and subjective aspects, the presence of either discretionary time free from obligations (Kelly and Godbey, 1992; Mitas, 2010), recreational activity under voluntary control (Argyle, 1999) and the pursuit of rewarding activities that transcend daily concerns (Larson et al., 1986) all promote well-being (Sonnentag and Niessen, 2008).

Newman et al. (2014) found five core psychological mechanisms of leisure linked to SWB: detachment-recovery, autonomy, mastery, meaning, and affiliation. Advocating a bottom-up theory of well-being, they posited that the presence of these components leads to an enhanced global experience of well-being. Similarly, Csíkszentmihályi (1990, 2003) suggests in his theory of flow that leisure could also offer peak states of well-being if it included complete concentration, effortlessness and ease and the sense of leisure being intrinsically rewarding (Csíkszentmihályi, 2014). As it can act as a buffer of stress, educating people about leisure has been found to facilitate a sense of resilience that can play a protective role in the maintenance of well-being (Denovan and Macaskill, 2016).

Mindfulness

In respect to the subjective domains identified as mediating well-being, an individual’s ability to have sufficient control over their cognitive or affective processes (Ingram et al., 1998; Segal et al., 2002; Diener, 2017) is critical. As such, researchers have found that an individual’s capacity for self-examination (Carver and Scheier, 2011) and self-regulation (Linehan, 1993, 2000), are components of mindfulness which may influence well-being (Kabat-Zinn, 1990, 2011; Tacon et al., 2004; Cahn et al., 2013).

Modern mindfulness includes a group of methods that teach individuals to develop non-judgmental awareness that is conducive not only to insight and analysis but also to well-being (Williams et al., 2000). Mindfulness practitioners report a host of benefits including heightened awareness of consciousness or meta-awareness (Schooler et al., 2011), and an improvement in cognitive processing (Lutz et al., 2008) such as improved attentional regulation (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Vago and Nakamura, 2011), executive monitoring (Jha et al., 2007), orienting ability and improved alerting-related processes (Ganaden and Smith, 2011), and better working memory (Brewer et al., 2011). These outcomes are in turn associated with increased QoL (Vibe et al., 2017).

Self-Esteem

Similarly, the well-researched area of self-esteem presents much evidence that persistent and stable positive self-concept plays a role in the maintenance of well-being (Heatherton and Polivy, 1991; Wong, 2010; Hutz et al., 2014) and life satisfaction (Kwan et al., 1997; Moksnes and Espnes, 2013). Whilst there appears to be a global concept of self-esteem, there is evidence that it is constructed from multiple components which are both internal (e.g., sense of inherent self-worth) and external (e.g., physical appearance, intelligence, social status, level of success) (Marsh, 1990; Harter, 1999; Huebner et al., 1999).

Self-esteem also has a bidirectional relationship with well-being and is strongly and inversely associated with state depression and state anxiety, lending further support to its role in relation to emotional health and well-being (Huppert and So, 2011; Freire and Ferreira, 2018). It is not only responsive to intervention but, in being found to play a mediating role in mental well-being (Kong et al., 2013), has prompted clinicians to develop interventions that specifically address and target self-esteem in the attainment of well-being (Bajaj et al., 2016).

Life Events

The effects of life events upon well-being have received less focus than the more traditionally associated areas such as self-esteem, work or relationships (Klumb and Baltes, 2004). In earlier research Suh et al. (1996) suggested that “the potential influence of events on positive outcomes, such as SWB, remains relatively unexplored” (p. 1091). This deficit could be due to the methodological challenges of reliably mapping this independent of other variables.

In more recent research Clark and Georgellis (2013) found that life events such as marriage, divorce and widowhood had no long-term effect (contrary to early research by Headey et al., 1984, who found death of a spouse to be significant 2 years later), whereas unemployment still had a negative impact on SWB after 5 years. Margolis and Myrskylä (2011) found that happiness increased around the birth of the first child but, in line with set point theory which suggests that well-being is offset positively within a small range (Cummins et al., 2014), it subsequently returned to the pre-birth level.

There also appears to be gender differences in processing challenging or traumatic life events. For example, Lamoureux-Lamarche and Vasiliadis (2017) found that exposure to violence, an accident or sexual trauma were more likely to be associated with post-traumatic stress syndrome in women than men. Conversely, a life-threatening disease was associated with a reduced life satisfaction only in men whereas life-threatening disease of a close one was significant for women. However, like most subjective variables, the effect was ameliorated by the presence of some kind of intervention or support (Pocnet et al., 2016).

Part of the complexity and hence comparative scarcity of research on the impact of life events is that cross-sectional designs are limited by the measurement of people to a single point in time and hence can’t account for the impact of adaptation. Furthermore, as its mediated by multiple variables such as the temporal distance between events (Bar-Anan et al., 2006), cognitive style, level of affect and personality, knowledge of the status of objective life conditions in itself is not sufficient to predict levels of well-being.

Similarly, as past events can be used as a standard of comparison to judge present circumstances (Strack et al., 1985; Pedersen and Schmidt, 2011), recent negative events have been found to affect people’s appraisal of their well-being (Campbell et al., 1976; Lyubomirsky, 2001). One method to address this has been the development of self-report measures that simply measured the number of recorded positive minus negative life events. This method aimed to remove some of the above confounds and long-term SWB has been found to be correlated with these kinds of calculations (Pavot et al., 1991; Diener and Fujita, 1995; Plagnol and Scott, 2011). Further research into the impact of life events upon well-being could benefit from controlling for such variables.

Mental Style

A range of psychological mechanisms have been found to mediate the effect of life events upon self-report of happiness (Day et al., 2010) such as resilience, coping style, level of affect or capacity for adaptation (Brickman et al., 1978; Luhmann et al., 2012). Cognitive adaptation includes the sets of basic adaptive processes that intervene between stress and its psychological (including the presence of positive affect and negative affect), social and physiological outcomes (Wong and Lim, 2009; Luhmann et al., 2012; Lyubomirsky, 2013).

For example, through the triggering of cognitive and affective regulatory functions that switch on either behavioral activation or inhibition (Higgins et al., 1999; Freund and Baltes, 2002), individuals could choose the best responses to rebound from the unhappiness of an abusive relationship once the relationship ends (Arriaga et al., 2013). This in turn serves a broader evolutionary function (White, 1974) of protecting, maintaining or enhancing well-being.

Sometimes described as the construal theories of happiness (Van Doorn et al., 2012), this area of research explores the multiple cognitive and affective processes utilized in the attainment of well-being (Lyubomirsky, 2001; Layous and Lyubomirsky, 2013). For example, in semantic network theories it is suggested that emotion can serve the function of activating a network of memories that enhances the accessibility of relevant beliefs about the self and specific life experience (Forgas and Bower, 1987; McIntosh, 2000; Forgas, 2012).

A ruminative, self-focused mental style, however, interferes with these problem-solving skills and the ability to generate effective solutions which, in turn, increases depressed mood and compromises well-being (Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow, 1991; Carver and Scheier, 2015). By contrast, strong self-determination (Ryan and Deci, 2014), positive self-rating in social comparisons (Pavot et al., 1997; Hoorens and Damme, 2012), and a positive interpretation of negative events (Pauls, 2007), are all associated with heightened well-being. Therapeutic modalities that specifically target mental style such as cognitive behavioral therapy and mindfulness are consistently effective in achieving improvements in ongoing well-being including now online interventions (Carlbring et al., 2009).

Life Management

Finally, the ability to be self-directing, make decisions and set personal goals has traditionally been seen as a sign of psychological maturity (Allport, 1961; Carver and Scheier, 1990; Deci and Ryan, 2000), and indicative of healthy or mature functioning (Ford and Urban, 1963; Strickland, 2016). One’s self-management or successful life administration has often been associated with motivation (Sheldon and Elliot, 1999), and has some role in the perception of QoL and well-being (Prenda and Lachman, 2001; Kleiber et al., 2011; Steca et al., 2015).

Having a clear strategy to achieve one’s life goals is also seen to play a critical part in SWB maintenance (Conrad et al., 2010; Coote and MacLeod, 2012). It has been associated with the reduction in anxiety about one’s life direction (McGrath and Adams, 1999; Coughlan et al., 2016), and focus on life goals has been found to improve the well-being outcomes and treatment engagement of patients with mental illness (Stanhope et al., 2013). In fact training in goal setting to improve perceived progress along the expectation-outcome gap (Michalos, 1985; Levesque et al., 2004) has demonstrated significant results in the augmentation of health and well-being markers, with new gamification technology replicating these results in virtual training (Johnson et al., 2016).

Discussion

In the exploratory search of studies reporting correlations between improvements in life domains and increased well-being and QoL, we raised the question as to whether there were a number of outstanding areas that warranted inclusion in clinical work. We hypothesized that, in view of the goal of therapy to improve a client’s QoL and well-being, that such an integration of these influential areas, would improve intervention efficacy.

In the investigation we found that there is in fact substantial evidence that each of the reported domains play differing, but mediating roles in the perception of QoL. Considering this it is recommended that they need to be considered by practitioners in the assessment, case formulation and intervention planning stages of their interventions.

Limitations

Several limitations to the methods chosen in the current investigation have emerged that need to be addressed. For example, the exploratory examination of the literature, whilst serving as a preliminary search to gather information for a more detailed investigation into selected domains, could have had more weight and less selection bias if it had been conducted as a systematic review in line with PRISMA guidelines, included gray literature and two independent reviewers that could have assessed quality more thoroughly (Panic et al., 2013).

Secondly subsequent to the initial exploratory literature search, as there was an interest in integrating the identified domains into a clinical intervention, a decision was made to limit the number for further investigation to 10 areas. In view of the presence in the literature of both objective and subjective life domains (Diener et al., 1999; Rodríguez et al., 2007; Diener and Biswas-Diener, 2009), it was expected that a similar type of division would occur, hence, a decision was made to split evenly the number of areas chosen for examination into objective and subjective domains. On reflection both the constraint of 10 areas and the equality of the division created an unnecessary limitation which may have narrowed the range of identified factors.

Furthermore, the selection criteria for this choice were based upon the 10 most frequently occurring domains with empirical evidence of a significant relationship with SWB or perceived QoL. This selection process could have been more formalized with a standardized approach that included a quality appraisal for internal validity of the individual studies (Forbes et al., 2017).

Implications

As each of the reported domains have been found to not only have an impact upon well-being but also interact with each other, it seems that to conduct a unidimensional intervention that focuses on one domain such as relationships, or a distressing past life event, does not seem to be sufficient. As there are many other factors that impact an individual’s experience of happiness, we maintain that it would be more clinically responsible to address the larger set of factors that are responsible for the multi-factorial phenomenon of happiness.

There appear to be a few ways to approach this, with one option being the creation of an individual or group interventions that target these areas with the goal to improve QoL and well-being. To practically address the concern that clients typically present with one specific issue that causes them distress, intervention design could include, along with diagnosis-specific treatment, both psycho-education on the impact of these areas upon well-being and achieving a balanced QoL, and exercises to address the indicated cognitive and behavioral repertoires associated with well-being.

That is the delivery of treatment could be integrated into an existing clinical intervention or be a stand-alone QoL goal setting intervention. For example, for the former option, along with regular treatment the client could be trained in the skillsets associated with each of the life domains such as health, relationships and so on. Concurrently with standard cognitive restructuring and behavioral schedules, clients could be given worksheets and guided to set goals, strategies and time-frames across the major QoL domains. Finally, in view of the typical client’s lack of knowledge of these factors, including a psycho-educational component on the identified predictors of QoL (akin to education on the fight-flight response in anxiety interventions), also seems warranted.

Once treatment for the initial presenting issue was complete, the intervention could move to a well-being focus with the client designing and building their own QoL plan. Clients could be taught how to set goals and strategies in each domain with the aim to build a robust foundation for the maintenance of well-being. Counseling could move closer to a coaching or accountability model with ongoing booster or follow up sessions to assist in the maintenance of goals. If new issues emerged, the model could seamlessly integrate standard cognitive-behavioral techniques, before returning to the well-being maintenance model.

Another extension of this research could be the development of a scale constructed from sub-domains of the domains found to have an impact upon well-being. For example, as communication skills are a major component of relationship satisfaction, items could be derived from the components that make up this domain. Such a scale could include items from each of the domains and could be used in conjunction with group programs and individual clinical work to assess pre-post intervention gains.

In view of the critique leveled at unidimensional interventions that they may be inferior to multidimensional approaches further testing of this hypothesis is warranted. To address this, an active control group of a known treatment could be compared to a proposed QoL intervention. Using standardized psychometric well-being measures, pre-post-follow up data could shed more light on the veracity of this claim.

It would also be prudent to test the hypothesis that the proposed ten domains are all relevant for inclusion in therapy by investigating whether any of them have common underlying factors. For example, could the domain of mindfulness and being in the present moment have something in common with the domain of mental style? A factor analysis on the ten suggested areas to identify underlying domains could clarify any overlap. If some domains could be collapsed into each other, this could simplify the intervention.

Conclusion

An exploration of objective and subjective life domains revealed evidence that they cumulatively impact overall QoL and the perception of well-being. As such it is suggested that they are relevant to intervention design and treatment. It is proposed that unidimensional clinical interventions that miss out these key areas, may be inferior to multidimensional interventions that include them. Further research is required to test this proposition, and it is recommended that both researchers and practicing clinicians consider these domains and their possible impact in the delivery of clinical interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

PJ contributed to the conception and design of the research and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. PD contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747435/full#supplementary-material

References

Ahmad, S. (2013). Paradigms of quality of work life. J. Hum. Values 19, 73–82. doi: 10.1177/0971685812470345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allport, G. (1961). Patterns and Growth in Personality. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Google Scholar

Andrews, F. M., and Withey, S. B. (1976). Social Indicators of Well-Being: America’s Perception of Life Quality. New York, NY: Plenum Press. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-2253-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argyle, M. (1987). The Psychology of Happiness. London: Methuen.

Google Scholar

Argyle, M. (1999). “Causes and correlates of happiness,” in Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, Chap. 18, eds D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz (New York: Russell Sage Foundation), 353–373.

Google Scholar

Armenta, C., Bao, K. J., Lyubomirsky, S., and Sheldon, K. M. (2014). Is lasting change possible? lessons from the hedonic adaptation prevention model. Stabil. Happin. 4, 57–74. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-411478-4.00004-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arriaga, X. B., Capezza, N. M., Goodfriend, W., Rayl, E. S., and Sands, K. J. (2013). Individual well-being and relationship maintenance at odds: the unexpected perils of maintaining a relationship with an aggressive partner. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 4, 676–684. doi: 10.1177/1948550613480822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astakhova, M. N. (2014). The curvilinear relationship between work passion and organizational citizenship behavior. J. Bus. Ethics 130, 361–374. doi: 10.1007/s10551-014-2233-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajaj, B., Gupta, R., and Pande, N. (2016). Self-esteem mediates the relationship between mindfulness and well-being. Pers. Individ. Diff. 94, 96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (2006). The association between psychological distance and construal level: evidence from an implicit association test. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 609–622. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, D. H., Allen, L. B., and Choate, M. L. (2016). Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders – republished article. Behav. Ther. 47, 838–853. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhullar, N., Schutte, N. S., and Malouff, J. M. (2013). The nature of well-being: the roles of hedonic and eudaimonic processes and trait emotional intelligence. J. Psychol. 147, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2012.667016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswas-Diener, R., and Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad situation: satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta. Soc. Indic. Res. 55, 329–352. doi: 10.1023/A:1010905029386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchflower, D. G., and Oswald, A. J. (2004). Money, sex and happiness: an empirical study. Scand. J. Econ. 106, 393–415. doi: 10.1111/j.0347-0520.2004.00369.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchflower, D. G., and Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Soc. Sci. Med. 66, 1733–1749. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Böckerman, P., Laamanen, J.-P., and Palosaari, E. (2015). The role of social ties in explaining heterogeneity in the association between economic growth and subjective well-being. J. Happin. Stud. 17, 2457–2479.

Google Scholar

Boehm, J. K., and Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). The heart’s content: the association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychol. Bull. 138, 655–691. doi: 10.1037/a0027448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookwala, J. (2014). Spouse health status, depressed affect, and resilience in mid and late life: a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 50, 1241–1249. doi: 10.1037/a0035124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brackertz, N. (2013). The impact of financial counselling on alleviating financial stress in low income households: a national australian empirical study. Soc. Policy Soc. 13, 389–407. doi: 10.1017/S1474746413000511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brannan, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Mohr, C. D., Mortazavi, S., and Stein, N. (2012). Friends and family, a cross-cultural investigation of social support and subjective well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 8, 65–75. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.743573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazier, J., and Tsuchiya, A. (2015). Improving cross-sector comparisons: going beyond the health-related QALY. Appl. Health Econ. Health Policy 13, 557–565. doi: 10.1007/s40258-015-0194-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y., Weber, J., and Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20254–20259.

Google Scholar

Brickman, P., Coates, D., and Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36, 917–927. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.8.917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryson, A., Forth, J., and Stokes, L. (2017). Does employees’ subjective well-being affect workplace performance? Hum. Relat. 70, 1017–1037. doi: 10.1177/0018726717693073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büssing, A., and Bissels, T. (1998). Different forms of work satisfaction. Eur. Psychol. 3, 209–218. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.3.3.209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., and Polich, J. (2013). Event-related delta, theta, alpha and gamma correlates to auditory oddball processing during vipassana meditation. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 8, 100–111. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., and Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The Quality of American Life. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Google Scholar

Carlbring, P., Nordgren, L. B., Furmark, T., and Andersson, G. (2009). Long-term outcome of Internet-delivered cognitive–behavioural therapy for social phobia: a 30-month follow-up. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 848–850. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, D., Freedman, V. A., Cornman, J. C., and Schwarz, N. (2014). Happy marriage, happy life? Marital quality and subjective well-being in later life. J. Marriage Fam. 76, 930–948. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, N. T., Guan, L., Maples, J. L., Williamson, R. L., and Miller, J. D. (2015). The downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological well-being: the role of obsessive compulsive tendencies. J. Pers. 84, 510–522. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, S. D. (2004). Reexamining the temporal aspects of affect: relationships between repeatedly measured affective state, subjective well-being, and affective disposition. Pers. Individ. Diff. 36, 381–391. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00103-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: a control-process view. Psychol. Rev. 97, 19–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2011). “Self-regulation of action and affect,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 3–21.

Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2015). Optimism. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/med:psych/9780195119343.003.0009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christie, M. J., French, D., Weatherstone, L., and West, A. The Applied Psychology Research Group (1991). The patients’ perceptions of chronic disease and its management: psychosomatics, holism and Quality of Life in contemporary management of childhood asthma. Psychother. Psychos. 56, 197–203. doi: 10.1159/000288556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. E., and Georgellis, Y. (2013). Back to baseline in Britain: adaptation in the BHPS. Economica 80, 496–512. doi: 10.1111/ecca.12007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conrad, N., Doering, B. K., Rief, W., and Exner, C. (2010). Looking beyond the importance of life goals. The personal goal model of subjective well-being in neuropsychological rehabilitation. Clin. Rehabil. 24, 431–443. doi: 10.1177/0269215509358930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coote, H. M. J., and MacLeod, A. K. (2012). A self-help, positive goal-focused intervention to increase well-being in people with depression. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 19, 305–315. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coughlan, K., Tata, P., and MacLeod, A. K. (2016). Personal goals, well-being and deliberate self-harm. Cogn. Ther. Res. 41, 434–443. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9769-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cranny, C. J., Smith, P. C., and Stone, E. F. (1992). Job Satisfaction: How People Feel About Their Jobs and How it Affects Their Performance. New York, NY: Lexington.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper- Perennial.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2003). Good Business: Leadership, Flow, and the Making of Meaning. New York: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2014). The Systems Model of Creativity: The Collected Works of Mihály Csíkszentmihályi. Dordrecht: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-9085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A. (2003). Normative life satisfaction: measurement issues and a homeostatic model. Soc. Indic. Res. 64, 159–190. doi: 10.1023/A:1024704320683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A. (2016). The theory of subjective wellbeing homeostasis: a contribution to understanding life quality. Soc. Indic. Res. Ser. 60, 61–79. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-20568-7_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A., Lau, A. L., and Stokes, M. (2004). HRQOL and subjective well-being: noncomplementary forms of outcome measurement. Expert Rev. Pharm. Outcomes Res. 4, 413–420. doi: 10.1586/14737167.4.4.413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, R. A., Li, L., Wooden, M., and Stokes, M. (2014). A demonstration of set-points for subjective wellbeing. J. Happin. Stud. 15, 183–206. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9444-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, W. (2010). The Contribution of Income to our Subjective Well-Being. Available online at: www.positivepsychology.org.uk/the-contribution-of-income-to-our-subjective-well-being (accessed March 15, 2021).

Google Scholar

Day, L., Hanson, K., Maltby, J., Proctor, C., and Wood, A. (2010). Hope uniquely predicts objective academic achievement above intelligence, personality, and previous academic achievement. J. Res. Pers. 44, 550–553. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 4, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demir, M., and Weitekamp, L. A. (2007). I am so happy ’cause today I found my friend: friendship and personality as predictors of happiness. J. Happin. Stud. 8, 181–211. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9012-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denovan, A., and Macaskill, A. (2016). Building resilience to stress through leisure activities: a qualitative analysis. Ann. Leisure Res. 20, 446–466. doi: 10.1080/11745398.2016.1211943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 95, 542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (2017). “Happiness: the science of subjective well-being,” in Noba Textbook Series: Psychology, eds R. Biswas-Diener and E. Diener (Champaign, IL: DEF publishers).

Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Biswas-Diener, R. (2009). “Will money increase subjective well-being?: a literature review and guide to needed research,” in The Science of Well-Being. Social Indicators Research Series, 37, ed. E. Diener (Dordrecht: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: a nomothetic and idiographic approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 926–935. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Heintzelman, S. J., Kushlev, K., Tay, L., Wirtz, D., Lutes, L. D., et al. (2017). Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Can. Psychol. 58, 87–104. doi: 10.1037/cap0000063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., and Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 125, 276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolan, P., and White, M. P. (2007). How can measures of subjective well-being be used to inform public policy? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 71–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00030.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L., and Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319, 1687–1688. doi: 10.1126/science.1150952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easterlin, R. A. (1974). “Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence,” in Nations and Households in Economic Growth, eds P. A. David and M. W. Reder (New York: Academic Press). doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-205050-3.50008-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 27, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/0167-2681(95)00003-B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eschleman, K. J., and Bowling, N. A. (2009). The Job Satisfaction and Life Satisfaction Relationship: A Meta-Analysis. Washington, DC: APA. doi: 10.1037/e518422013-344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, L. J., Marchand, C., Doran, T., and Peckham, S. (2017). The role of the Quality and Outcomes Framework in the care of long-term conditions: a systematic review. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 67, e775–e784. doi: 10.3399/bjgp17X693077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, D. H., and Urban, H. B. (1963). Systems of Psychotherapy: A Comparative Study. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi: 10.1037/10782-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fordyce, M. W. (1977). Development of a program to increase personal happiness. J. Counsel. Psychol. 24, 511–521. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.24.6.511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fordyce, M. W. (1983). A program to increase happiness: further studies. J. Counsel. Psychol. 30, 483–498. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.30.4.483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P. (2012). Social Thinking and Interpersonal Behavior. London: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9780203139677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P., and Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 53–60. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freire, T., and Ferreira, G. (2018). Health-related quality of life of adolescents: relations with positive and negative psychological dimensions. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 23, 11–24. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2016.1262268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, A. M., and Baltes, P. B. (2002). Life-management strategies of selection, optimization, and compensation: measurement by self-report and construct validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 642–662. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frisch, M. B. (1994). Manual and treatment guide for the Quality of Life Inventory (QOLI). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments and National Computer Systems.

Google Scholar

Frisch, M. B. (2016). “Quality of life therapy,” in The Wiley Handbook of Positive Clinical Psychology, eds A. M. Wood and J. Johnson (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 409–425. doi: 10.1002/9781118468197.ch27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganaden, R., and Smith, S. D. (2011). The effects of trait mindfulness on multiple components of attention: evidence from an emotional attention networks test. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 64, 328–328.

Google Scholar

Geerling, D. M., and Diener, E. (2018). Effect size strengths in subjective well-being research. Appl. Res. Q. Life 15, 167–185. doi: 10.1007/s11482-018-9670-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, L. K., and Landerman, R. (1984). Health and subjective well-being: a replicated secondary data analysis. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 19, 133–156. doi: 10.2190/FHHT-25R8-F8KT-MAJD

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gere, J., and Schimmack, U. (2011). When romantic partners’ goals conflict: effects on relationship quality and subjective well-being. J. Happin. Stud. 14, 37–49. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9314-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottman, J., Cole, C., and Cole, D. L. (2017). “Gottman method couples therapy,” in Encyclopedia of Couple and Family Therapy, eds J. Lebow, A. Chambers, and D. C. Breunlin (Berlin: Springer), 1–10. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-15877-8_195-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggerty, M. R., and Veenhoven, R. (2003). Wealth and happiness revisited-growing national income does go with greater happiness. Soc. Indic. Res. 64, 1–27. doi: 10.1023/A:1024790530822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanley, A., Warner, A., and Garland, E. L. (2014). Associations between mindfulness, psychological well-being, and subjective well-being with respect to contemplative practice. J. Happin. Stud. 16, 1423–1436. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9569-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harter, S. (1999). The Construction of the Self. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hawthorne, B. S., Vella-Brodrick, D. A., and Hattie, J. (2019). Well-being as a cognitive load reducing agent: a review of the literature. Front. Educ. 4:121. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2019.00121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headey, B., Holmstrom, E., and Wearing, A. (1984). The impact of life events and changes in domain satisfactions on well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 15, 203–227. doi: 10.1007/BF00668671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headey, B., and Wearing, A. (1988). The sense of relative superiority—central to well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 20, 497–516. doi: 10.1007/BF03359554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., and Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 895–910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T., Grant, H., and Shah, J. (1999). “Self-regulation and quality of life: emotional and non-emotional life experiences,” in Well-Being: The foundations of Hedonic Psychology, 244-266), eds D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz (New York: Russell Sage Foundation).

Google Scholar

Hoorens, V., and Damme, C. V. (2012). What do people infer from social comparisons? Bridges between social comparison and person perception. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 6, 607–618. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00451.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, E. S., Gilman, R., and Laughlin, J. E. (1999). A multimethod investigation of the multidimensionality of children’s well-being reports: discriminant validity of life satisfaction and self-esteem. Soc. Indic. Res. 46, 1–22. doi: 10.1023/A:1006821510832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, F. A., and So, T. T. C. (2011). Flourishing across europe: application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Soc. Indic. Res. 110, 837–861. doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9966-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutz, C. S., Midgett, A., Pacico, J. C., Bastianello, M. R., and Zanon, C. (2014). The relationship of hope, optimism, self-esteem, subjective well-being, and personality in Brazilians and Americans. Psychology 5, 514–522. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.56061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglehart, R. (1990). Culture Shift in Advanced Industrial Society. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9780691186740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., and Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Jenkinson, C., Wright, L., and Coulter, A. (1993). Criterion validity and reliability of the SF-36 in a population sample. Q. Life Res. Int. J. Qual. Life Aspects Treat. Care Rehabil. 3, 7–12. doi: 10.1007/BF00647843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., and Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 109–119. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.2.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, D., Deterding, S., Kuhn, K.-A., Staneva, A., Stoyanov, S., and Hides, L. (2016). Gamification for health and wellbeing: a systematic review of the literature. Internet Intervent. 6, 89–106. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2016.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., and Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self-evaluations and job and life satisfaction: the role of self-concordance and goal attainment. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 257–268. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.2.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain and Illness. New York, NY: Delacorte.

Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Some reflections on the origins of MBSR, skilful means, and the trouble with maps. Contemp. Buddh. 12, 281–306. doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., Diener, E., and Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Google Scholar

Kammann, R. (1983). Objective circumstances, life satisfactions, and sense of well-being: consistencies across time and place. N. Zeal. J. Psychol. 12, 14–22.

Google Scholar

Karimi, M., and Brazier, J. (2016). Health, health-related quality of life, and quality of life: what is the difference? PharmacoEconomics 34, 645–649. doi: 10.1007/s40273-016-0389-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, J. R., and Godbey, G. (1992). The Sociology of Leisure. State College, PA: Venture.

Google Scholar

Kimberlin, C. L., and Winterstein, A. G. (2008). Validity and reliability of measurement instruments used in research. Am. J. Health Syst. Pharm. 65, 2276–2284. doi: 10.2146/ajhp070364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiber, D. A., Walker, G. J., and Mannell, R. C. (2011). A Social Psychology of Leisure, 2nd Edn. State College, PA: Venture.

Google Scholar

Klumb, P. L., and Baltes, M. M. (2004). Adverse life events in late life: their manifestation and management in daily life. Int. J. Stress Manag. 11, 3–20. doi: 10.1037/1072-5245.11.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kong, F., Zhao, J., and You, X. (2013). Self-esteem as mediator and moderator of the relationship between social support and subjective well-being among Chinese university students. Soc. Indic. Res. 112, 151–161. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0044-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krampen, G. (1999). Evaluation of the effectiveness of autogenic training in gerontopsychology. Eur. Psychol. 1, 243–254. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.1.4.243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., and Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction: adding relationship harmony to self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1038–1051. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamoureux-Lamarche, C., and Vasiliadis, H. M. (2017). Lifetime traumatic events, health-related quality of life, and satisfaction with life in older adults. Qual. Life Res. 26, 2683–2692. doi: 10.1007/s11136-017-1593-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, R., Mannell, R., and Zuzanek, J. (1986). Daily well-being of older adults with friends and family. Psychol. Aging 1, 117–126. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.1.2.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Layous, K., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). How Do Simple Positive Activities Increase Well-Being?. Washington, DC: APA. doi: 10.1037/e574802013-182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., Vlaev, I., King, D., Mayer, E., Darzi, A., and Dolan, P. (2013). Subjective well-being and the measurement of quality in healthcare. Soc. Sci. Med. 99, 27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levesque, C., Zuehlke, A. N., Stanek, L. R., and Ryan, R. M. (2004). Autonomy and competence in german and american university students: a comparative study based on self-determination theory. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 68–84. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.68

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive-Behavioural Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Linehan, M. M. (2000). “Borderline personality disorder,” in Encyclopedia of Psychology, ed. A. E. Kazdin (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association). doi: 10.1037/10516-163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, K. M., and Auld, C. J. (2002). The role of leisure in determining quality of life: issues of content and measurement. Soc. Indic. Res. 57, 43–71. doi: 10.1023/A:1013879518210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Locke, E. A., and Latham, G. P. (2013). New Developments in Goal Setting and Task Performance. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203082744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., and Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 527–539. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, R. E., and Dyrenforth, P. S. (2006). “Does the existence of social relationships matter for subjective well-being?,” in Self and Relationships: Connecting Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Processes, eds K. D. Vohs and E. J. Finkel (New York, NY: Guilford), 254–273.

Google Scholar

Lucas, R. E., Dyrenforth, P. S., and Diener, E. (2008). Four myths about subjective well-being. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 2001–2015. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00140.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., and Lucas, R. E. (2012). Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: a meta-analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 592–615. doi: 10.1037/a0025948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lykken, D., and Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychol. Sci. 7, 186–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00355.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? The role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. Am. Psychol. 56, 239–249. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). The Myths of Happiness: What Should Make You Happy, But Doesn’t, What Shouldn’t Make You Happy, But Does. New York, NY: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Margolis, R., and Myrskylä, M. (2011). A global perspective on happiness and fertility. Populat. Dev. Rev. 37, 29–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2011.00389.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W. (1990). A multidimensional, hierarchical model of self-concept: theoretical and empirical justification. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2, 77–172. doi: 10.1007/BF01322177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathieu, C., Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., and Babiak, P. (2014). A dark side of leadership: corporate psychopathy and its influence on employee well-being and job satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Diff. 59, 83–88.

Google Scholar

McGrath, J. R., and Adams, L. (1999). Patient-centered goal planning: a systemic psychological therapy? Top Stroke Rehabil. 6, 43–50. doi: 10.1310/J2G9-UVA8-UE5D-N693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R. (2000). Towards a network theory of cognition. Neural Netw. 13, 861–870. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00059-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies theory (MDT). Soc. Indic. Res. 16, 347–413. doi: 10.1007/BF00333288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalos A. C.(ed.) (2017). “What did stiglitz, sen and fitoussi get right and what did they get wrong?,” in Development of Quality of Life Theory and Its Instruments, (Berlin: Springer), 287–300. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-51149-8_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitas, O. (2010). Positive Emotions in Mature Adults’ Leisure Travel Experiences. Doctoral dissertation. State College, PA: The Pennsylvania State University.

Google Scholar

Moksnes, U. K., and Espnes, G. A. (2013). Self-esteem and life satisfaction in adolescents—gender and age as potential moderators. Qual. Life Res. 22, 2921–2928. doi: 10.1007/s11136-013-0427-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monnot, M. J., and Beehr, T. A. (2014). Subjective well-being at work: disentangling source effects of stress and support on enthusiasm, contentment, and meaningfulness. J. Vocation. Behav. 85, 204–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2014.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muhli, U. H., and Svensson, A. M. (2017). “Well-being in old age: a question of both continuity and change,” in Well-Being and Quality of Life - Medical Perspective, ed. M. Mollaoglu (Norderstedt: BoD – Books on Demand). doi: 10.5772/intechopen.68680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, D. G. (1999). “Close relationships and quality of life,” in Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, eds D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 374–391.

Google Scholar

Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. Am. Psychol. 55, 56–67. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, D., Tay, L., and Diener, E. (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: a model of psychological mechanisms as mediating factors. J. Happin. Stud. 15, 555–578. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9435-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nirmalan, P. K. (2014). Quality of life and marital adjustment after cognitive behavioural therapy and behavioural marital therapy in couples with anxiety disorders. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. 8, WC1–WC4. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/9692.4752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 115–121. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okun, M. A., and George, L. K. (1984). Physician- and self-ratings of health, neuroticism, and subjective well-being among men and women. Pers. Individ. Diff. 5, 533–539. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(84)90027-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panic, N., Leoncini, E., de Belvis, G., Ricciardi, W., and Boccia, S. (2013). Evaluation of the endorsement of the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement on the quality of published systematic review and meta-analyses. PLoS One 8:e83138. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, H., Alkhawam, H., Madanieh, R., Shah, N., Kosmas, C. E., and Vittorio, T. J. (2017). Aerobic vs anaerobic exercise training effects on the cardiovascular system. World J. Cardiol. 9:134. doi: 10.4330/wjc.v9.i2.134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauls, C. A. (2007). Is a repressive-defensive coping style really so bad? Comment on DeNeve and Cooper (1998). Psychol. Bull. 133, 907–909. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavot, W., Diener, E., Colvin, R., and Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the satisfaction with life scale: evidence for the cross-method convergence of self-report well-being measures. J. Pers. Assess. 57, 149–161. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5701_17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavot, W., Fujita, F., and Diener, E. (1997). The relation between self-aspect congruence, personality and subjective well-being. Pers. Individ. Diff. 22, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00196-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedersen, P. J., and Schmidt, T. D. (2011). Happiness in Europe. Cross-country differences in the determinants of satisfaction with main activity. J. Socio Econ. 40, 480–489. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook and Classification. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Plagnol, A. C., and Scott, J. (2011). What matters for well-being: individual perceptions of quality of life before and after important life events. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 6, 115–138. doi: 10.1007/s11482-010-9119-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pocnet, C., Antonietti, J. P., Strippoli, M. P. F., Glaus, J., Preisig, M., and Rossier, J. (2016). Individuals’ quality of life linked to major life events, perceived social support, and personality traits. Qual. Life Res. 25, 2897–2908. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1296-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prenda, K. M., and Lachman, M. E. (2001). Planning as a life management strategy in adulthood: implications for perceived control and life satisfaction. Psychol. Aging 16, 206–216. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., and Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 576–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00393.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, H. T. (2012). “Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing theme for the study of relationships and well-being,” in Interdisciplinary Research on Close Relationships: The Case for Integration, eds L. Campbell and T. J. Loving (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 27–52.

Google Scholar

Rodríguez, A., Látková, P., and Sun, Y. Y. (2007). The relationship between leisure and life satisfaction: application of activity and need theory. Soc. Indic. Res. 86, 163–175. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9101-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruch, W., Gander, F., Wagner, L., and Giuliani, F. (2019). The structure of character: on the relationships between character strengths and virtues. J. Posit. Psychol. 16, 116–128. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1689418

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. (2014). “Self-determination theory,” in Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, ed. A. C. Michalos (Berlin: Springer), 5755–5760. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_2630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., and Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakiris, N., and Berle, D. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the Unified Protocol as a transdiagnostic emotion regulation based intervention. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 72:101751. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salvador-Carulla, L., Lucas, R., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., and Miret, M. (2014). Use of the terms “Wellbeing” and “Quality of Life” in health sciences: a conceptual framework. Eur. J. Psychiatry 28, 50–65. doi: 10.4321/S0213-61632014000100005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., and Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 319–326. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segal, Z., Williams, J. M. G., and Teasdale, J. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., and Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadish, W. R., and Baldwin, S. A. (2005). Effects of behavioral marital therapy: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 6–14. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., and Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: the self-concordance model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 482–497. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.3.482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shier, M. L., and Graham, J. R. (2013). Subjective well-being, social work, and the environment: the impact of the socio-political context of practice on social worker happiness. J. Soc. Work 15, 3–23. doi: 10.1177/1468017313503449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimazu, A., Schaufeli, W. B., Kamiyama, K., and Kawakami, N. (2015). Workaholism vs. work engagement: the two different predictors of future well-being and performance. Int. J. Behav. Med. 22, 18–23. doi: 10.1007/s12529-014-9410-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., Oishi, S., and Jeswani, S. (2013). The relationship between social support and subjective well-being across age. Soc. Indic. Res. 117, 561–576. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sin, N. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 65, 467–487. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirgy, J. M., Michalos, A. C., Ferriss, A. L., Easterlin, R. A., Patrick, D., and Pavot, W. (2006). The quality-of-life (QOL) research movement: past, present, and future. Soc. Indic. Res. 76, 343–466. doi: 10.1007/s11205-005-2877-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sjøgaard, G., Christensen, J. R., Justesen, J. B., Murray, M., Dalager, T., Fredslund, G., et al. (2016). Exercise is more than medicine: the working age population’s wellbeing and productivity. J. Sport Health Sci. 5, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2016.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, W. P., Compton, W. C., and West, W. B. (1995). Meditation as an adjunct to a happiness enhancement program. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 269–273. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199503)51:2<269::AID-JCLP2270510217>3.0.CO;2-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnentag, S., and Niessen, C. (2008). Staying vigorous until work is over: the role of trait vigour, day-specific work experiences and recovery. J. Occupation. Organ. Psychol. 81, 435–458. doi: 10.1348/096317908X310256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srivastava, A., Locke, E. A., and Bartol, K. M. (2001). Motives for Making Money Scale. Washington, DC: APA. doi: 10.1037/t09068-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanhope, V., Ingoglia, C., Schmelter, B., and Marcus, S. C. (2013). Impact of person-centered planning and collaborative documentation on treatment adherence. Psychiatr. Serv. 64, 76–79. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201100489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steca, P., Monzani, D., Greco, A., D’Addario, M., Cappelletti, E., and Pancani, L. (2015). The effects of short-term personal goals on subjective well-being. J. Happin. Stud. 17, 1435–1450. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9651-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Deaton, A., and Stone, A. A. (2015). Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet 385, 640–648. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Demakakos, P., de Oliveira, C., and Wardle, J. (2012). Distinctive biological correlates of positive psychological well-being in older men and women. Psychos. Med. 74, 501–508. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31824f82c8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, B., and Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic Growth and Subjective Well-Being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. doi: 10.3386/w14282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., and Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 9985–9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003744107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., Schwarz, N., and Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: the role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1460–1469. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strickland, B. R. (2016). Internal versus external locus of control. Perceiv. Control 5, 23–44. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190257040.003.0002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suh, E., Diener, E., and Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: only recent events matter. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1091–1102. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tacon, A. M., Caldera, Y. M., and Ronaghan, C. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in women with breast cancer. Fam. Syst. Health 22, 193–203. doi: 10.1037/1091-7527.22.2.193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, K., and Tay, L. (2017). “Relationships and well-being,” in Noba textbook series: Psychology, eds R. Biswas-Diener and E. Diener (Champaign, IL: DEF publishers).

Google Scholar

Tay, L., Tan, K., Diener, E., and Gonzalez, E. (2013). Social relations, health behaviors, and health outcomes: a survey and synthesis. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 5, 28–78. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (2010). “Social support: a review,” in Oxford Handbook of Health Psychology, ed. H. S. Friedman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Tinsley, H. A., Tinsley, D. J., and Croskeys, C. E. (2002). Park usage, social milieu, and psychosocial benefits of park use reported by older urban park users from four ethnic groups. Leisure Sci. 24, 199–218. doi: 10.1080/01490400252900158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinsley, H. E., and Eldredge, B. D. (1995). Psychological benefits of leisure participation: a taxonomy of leisure activities based on their need-gratifying properties. J. Counsel. Psychol. 42, 123–132. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.42.2.123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Threadgill, A. H., and Gable, P. A. (2018). The sweetness of successful goal pursuit: approach-motivated pregoal states enhance the reward positivity during goal pursuit. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 132, 277–286. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unanue, W., Gómez, M. E., Cortez, D., Oyanedel, J. C., and Mendiburo-Seguel, A. (2017). Revisiting the link between job satisfaction and life satisfaction: the role of basic psychological needs. Front. Psychol. 8:680. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vago, D. R., and Nakamura, Y. (2011). Selective attentional bias towards pain-related threat in fibromyalgia: preliminary evidence for effects of mindfulness meditation training. Cogn. Ther. Res. 35, 581–594. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9391-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Doorn, E. A., Heerdink, M. W., and Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Emotion and the construal of social situations: inferences of cooperation versus competition from expressions of anger, happiness, and disappointment. Cogn. Emot. 26, 442–461. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.648174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Katwyk, P. T., Fox, S., Spector, P. E., and Kelloway, E. K. (2000). Using the Job-Related Affective Well-being Scale (JAWS) to investigate affective responses to work stressors. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 52, 219–230. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.2.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Soc. Indic. Res. 24, 1–34. doi: 10.1007/BF00292648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vella-Brodrick, D. A., Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2008). Three ways to be happy: pleasure, engagement, and meaning—findings from Australian and US Samples. Soc. Indic. Res. 90, 165–179. doi: 10.1007/s11205-008-9251-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vibe, M., Bjørndal, A., Fattah, S., Dyrdal, G. M., Halland, E., and Tanner-Smith, E. E. (2017). Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) for improving health, quality of life and social functioning in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Campbell Syst. Rev. 13, 1–264. doi: 10.4073/csr.2017.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warr, P., and Payne, R. (1982). Experiences of strain and pleasure among British adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 16, 1691–1697. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(82)90094-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, R. W. (1974). “Strategies of adaptation: an attempt at systematic description,” in Coping and Adaptation, eds G. V. Coelho, D. A. Hamburg, and J. E. Adams (New York: Basic Books), 47–68.

Google Scholar

Williams, J. M. G., Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., and Soulsby, J. (2000). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory in formerly depressed patients. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 150–155. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.1.150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, K. D. (2009). “Ostracism: a temporal need-threat model,” in Advances in Experimental Psychology, ed. M. Zanna (New York, NY: Academic Press), 279–314.

Google Scholar

Windle, G., Hughes, D., Linck, P., Russell, I., and Woods, B. (2010). Is exercise effective in promoting mental well-being in older age? A systematic review. Aging Ment. Health 14, 652–669. doi: 10.1080/13607861003713232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2010). What is existential positive psychology? Int. J. Existential Psychol. Psychother. 3, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Wong, S. S., and Lim, T. (2009). Hope versus optimism in singaporean adolescents: contributions to depression in life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Diff. 46, 648–652. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: quality of life, well-being, intervention, assessment, case formulation, positive psychology, therapy, counselling

Citation: Jones P and Drummond PD (2021) A Summary of Current Findings on Quality of Life Domains and a Proposal for Their Inclusion in Clinical Interventions. Front. Psychol. 12:747435. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747435

Received: 26 July 2021; Accepted: 07 October 2021;
Published: 29 October 2021.

Edited by:

Soraia Garcês, University of Madeira, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Veena Easvaradoss, Women’s Christian College, India
Seyyed Mohammad Ali Soozandehfar, University of Hormozgan, Iran

Copyright © 2021 Jones and Drummond. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrick Jones, drpatrickjonesaustralia@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.