Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 21 October 2021
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities

Sexual Orientation Identity Development Milestones Among Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Queer People: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

  • School of Social Work, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, United States

This paper is a systematic review and meta-analysis on sexual orientation identity development milestones among people who are lesbian, gay, bisexual, or another sexual minority identity (LGB+). Common milestones measured in the 30 studies reviewed were becoming aware of queer attractions, questioning one’s sexual orientation, self-identifying as LGB+, coming out to others, engaging in sexual activity, and initiating a romantic relationship. Milestones occurred in different sequences, although attraction was almost always first, often followed by self-identification and/or sexual activity; coming out and initiating a romantic relationship often followed these milestones. Meta-analysis results showed that the mean effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals varied by milestone: attraction [Mage=12.7 (10.1, 15.3)], questioning one’s orientation [Mage=13.2 [12.8, 13.6]), self-identifying [Mage=17.8 (11.6, 24.0)], sexual activity [Mage=18.1 (17.6, 18.6)], coming out [Mage=19.6 (17.2, 22.0)], and romantic relationship [Mage=20.9 (13.2, 28.6)]. Nonetheless, results also showed substantial heterogeneity in the mean effect sizes. Additional meta-analyses showed that milestone timing varied by sex, sexual orientation, race/ethnicity, and birth cohort. Although patterns were found in LGB+ identity development, there was considerable diversity in milestone trajectories.

Introduction

Sexual orientation identity development refers to changes, processes, and experiences over time that can involve awareness, exploration, appraisal, commitment, integration, and communication concerning a person’s identity as a sexual being, which is based on their patterns of sexual attractions and behaviors. Although commonalities exist in sexual orientation identity development, there are diverse trajectories across individuals and groups. For example, people who identify as lesbian, gay, bisexual, or another sexual minority identity (e.g., queer and pansexual; LGB+) navigate different tasks and milestones related to their stigmatized minority sexual orientation. Sexual orientation is a multidimensional construct, referring to an individual’s positioning on key dimensions of sexuality: sexual attraction, sexual behavior, romantic orientation, and sexual orientation identity (Mustanski et al., 2014; Hall, 2019). Historically, scholarship on LGB+ identity development was rooted in heterosexist notions and hindered by the limited number of completed studies; however, research in this area has continued to emerge and evolve. This article is a systematic review and meta-analysis of the recent empirical literature on sexual orientation identity development milestones among LGB+ people.

Historical Overview of Scholarship on LGB+ Identity Development

Scholarship regarding the sexual orientation identity development of LGB+ people has evolved over the past 100years. For most of the 20th century, queer desires, behaviors, and identities that exist outside the norm of heterosexuality have been generally viewed as deviant and abnormal. According to Sigmund Freud’s theory of psychosexual development, humans were innately bisexual and became heterosexual or homosexual based on childhood experiences with parents (Freud, 1905). Freud theorized that homosexuality was a result of problems that arise during psychosexual development, such as boys becoming overly attached to and identifying with their mother instead of their father, feeling intense castration anxiety that leads boys to reject women because they are “castrated,” and narcissistic self-obsession that leads boys to choose an object of attraction that resembles themselves (Lewes, 1988). In later writings, Freud (1935) asserted that homosexuality was not a “vice,” “degradation,” or “illness.” Rather, he seemed to view homosexuality as an atypical variation in sexuality due to unresolved intrapsychic conflicts during childhood psychosexual development.

Psychoanalysts who followed Freud, including Sandor Rado, Irving Bieber, and Charles Socarides, took pathological views regarding homosexuality and asserted that homosexuality could be cured through psychoanalysis (Drescher, 2015). Rado (1940, 1949) rejected Freud’s assumption of innate bisexuality and contended that heterosexuality was the only natural and normal form of sexuality. Bieber (1962, 1967, 1969) claimed that male homosexuality was caused by boys having a possessive and overly involved mother, as well as a hostile or distant father; these dynamics led boys to bond with their mother and prevented them from developing their masculinity, which led him to effeminate homosexuality. For female homosexuality, Bieber (1967, 1969) claimed it was caused by various parent-child relationship dynamics, such as mothers being overly rejecting and critical of their daughters, showing little warmth and affection; this, combined with “defeminizing” behaviors, such as not dressing their daughter in pretty clothes and not teaching her cooking and housekeeping skills, contributed to homosexuality. Bieber (1962) also claimed that 27% of the patients in his clinical study were cured of their homosexuality. Socarides (1968) also maintained that homosexuality was an “illness” and “perversion” (p. 1) caused by problematic relationships between parents and their children. Socarides (1978) found a 44% success rate in curing patients of their homosexuality. Socarides also helped found the National Association of Research and Therapy of Homosexuality, which stated that “homosexuality is a treatable developmental disorder” (as cited in Mondimore, 1996, p. 224). These pathological views of homosexuality supplanted Freud’s theories and dominated psychology from the 1940s to the 1970s. Homosexuality was classified as an illness in 1952 with the initial publication of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM).

Countering the psychopathology perspective was the work of Alfred Kinsey, which forwarded that homosexuality was a normal variation in human sexuality. Unlike prior psychoanalytic studies, Kinsey and his colleagues used non-psychiatric samples and found that homosexuality was not uncommon among men and women in the United States (Kinsey et al., 1948, 1953). Likewise, the work of Evelyn Hooker was also significant in countering the abnormality narrative of homosexuality. Hooker (1957) gathered psychological test results from heterosexual and gay men living in the community and then asked psychologists to appraise their psychological adjustment without knowing the participants’ sexual orientations. The psychologists classified the heterosexual and gay participants into equal levels of mental adjustment and they could not distinguish which participants were gay or heterosexual based on the test results. Hooker concluded that homosexuality was not inherently psychopathological. In 1973, members of the American Psychiatric Association voted on the removal of homosexuality from the DSM, which succeeded by a narrow majority of 58% (Drescher, 2015).

Sexual minority identity research during the 1970s, 1980s, and 1990s primarily focused on stage models to capture what was thought by many scholars to be a linear and universal progression in identity development (e.g., Cass, 1979, 1984, 1996). The Cass model has been the most widely recognized and cited of the stage models. The first stage of the Cass model involves initial awareness of feeling different in terms of sexual and/or romantic feelings or attractions that are not exclusively heterosexual. Next, the individual goes back and forth between feeling that they may likely be LGB+ to denial of this potential reality. Later, the person accepts the likelihood of being LGB+ but only later fully accepts a LGB+ identity. Next, the individual begins coming out to others, becomes involved with the LGB+ community and its culture, becomes highly aware of heterosexism and its effects, and may feel anger toward heterosexist people and institutions. In the final stage, the individual realizes that being LGB+ is just one part of who they are and synthesizes their LGB+ identity into a holistic sense of self.

However, critiques of the stage models and limited empirical validation of them (Kenneady and Oswalt, 2014; Savin-Williams and Cohen, 2015) led researchers to study the milestones that people experience as they develop a LGB+ identity. Milestones are events that mark significant points in human development in terms of life changes or achievements. Significant identity-related milestones for LGB+ people include one’s first awareness of queer or non-heterosexual desires, self-identifying as LGB+, and initially coming out as LGB+ to friends and family. Unlike the stage models, milestone-focused research did not presume a singular or ideal pathway of LGB+ identity development, but rather has attempted to examine patterns and variation in sexuality trajectories among LGB+ people, as well as to understand factors that shape the timing and sequence of milestones (e.g., Rosario et al., 1996; Savin-Williams and Diamond, 2000; Floyd and Stein, 2002; Maguen et al., 2002; Calzo et al., 2011). Milestone research has been conducted during the 1990s, 2000s, and 2010s.

Implications of LGB+ Identity for Health and Psychosocial Functioning

In recent decades, scholars have also investigated disparities in mental and behavioral health problems facing LGB+ people. A growing body of evidence has documented many sexual orientation disparities where LGB+ people have significantly higher rates of psychological disorders and behavioral health problems compared to their heterosexual counterparts. Noteworthy disparities facing LGB+ people include depression, general anxiety, substance use/abuse (e.g., tobacco, alcohol, marijuana, and illicit drugs) and substance use disorders, suicidal ideation and suicidal behavior, eating disorders, body image and weight-related problems, and risky sexual behavior and sexually transmitted infections (e.g., Balsam et al., 2005; Marshal et al., 2008, 2011; Friedman et al., 2011; Everett, 2013; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2013; Operario et al., 2015; Strutz et al., 2015; Choi and Meyer, 2016; Gonzales and Henning-Smith, 2017; Ross et al., 2018; Schuler et al., 2018; Miller and Luk, 2019; Rice et al., 2019; Kamody et al., 2020; Raifman et al., 2020). Making these disparities more alarming is evidence that they are present across the life span among LGB+ adolescents, adults, and older adults. These disparities have multiple causes, including negative experiences during LGB+ identity development. Research shows that failing to positively integrate one’s LGB+ identity into one’s overall identity predicts depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem (Rosario et al., 2011). Negative reactions from parents and friends after initially coming out as LGB+ are associated with low self-esteem, depression, suicidal ideation, and suicide attempts (Rosario et al., 2011; Baams et al., 2015; Ryan et al., 2015; Puckett et al., 2017).

Alternatively, many LGB+ people have positive identity development experiences that can contribute to psychological wellbeing and adaptive social functioning. The emerging scholarship on positive LGB+ identity (e.g., Baiocco et al., 2018; Riggle et al., 2014) indicates it is a psychosocial construct with multiple dimensions, such as enhanced self-awareness and personal insight, intrapersonal and interpersonal authenticity, freedom from gender roles, awareness of oppression and commitment to social justice, and connection with the queer community. Research shows that a positive LGB+ identity is inversely associated with depressive symptoms and positively associated with emotional self-awareness, self-compassion, emotional intimacy, social wellbeing, psychological wellbeing, and life satisfaction (Kertzner et al., 2009; Riggle et al., 2014; Riggle et al., 2017; Rostosky et al., 2018; Petrocchi et al., 2020). To reduce disparities and promote healthy and adaptive development among LGB+ people, we must understand the sexual orientation identity development process, which is a central aspect in the lives of LGB+ people.

Purpose of This Review

Although several narrative literature reviews on this topic have been published (Reynolds and Hanjorgiris, 2000; Savin-Williams, 2011, 2019; Morgan, 2012; Mustanski et al., 2014; Savin-Williams and Cohen, 2015), they tend to focus on identity development for adolescents and emerging adults, and these reviews were not systematic reviews. Systematic reviews answer specific research questions on a topic using systematic, transparent, and replicable strategies to minimize bias and error. Systematic reviews are useful in understanding the state of the science in an area by summarizing what is known and moving science forward by providing directions for future research to improve upon limitations in prior studies and to address gaps identified in the literature. The purpose of this review was to systematically review the methodological characteristics and substantive findings of studies examining sexual orientation identity development milestones among LGB+ people. The following research questions drove this review: (1) What are the primary sexual orientation identity development milestones for LGB+ people? (2) At what ages do these milestones occur? (3) In what sequences to these milestones occur? (4) Does the chronology of milestones vary by sex, sexual orientation, race/ethnicity, or birth cohort? These four demographics were chosen because they are important biopsychosocial variables by which the timing of milestones may vary, and preliminary review of the literature indicated that there would be a sufficient number of studies to answer this research question, whereas there would be insufficient studies for other demographics (e.g., socioeconomic status, religious orientation, and ability/disability status).

Materials and Methods

In preparing this review, the authors adhered to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses criteria (Moher et al., 2015). Before undertaking the search for relevant studies, the authors developed a protocol for bibliographic database searches, study inclusion and exclusion criteria, and a data extraction tool. This review was registered with PROSPERO, an international database of systematic reviews regarding health and social wellbeing.

Inclusion Criteria

Studies were included in the review if they met the following criteria: (1) collected data from lesbian, gay, bisexual, and/or queer people about the timing of their identity development milestones; (2) collected data in the United States; (3) were written in English; and (4) were published or completed on or after January 1, 1990. The time period selected allowed for a contemporary review of the empirical literature completed since the shift in focus toward understanding sexuality milestones. Studies conducted outside the United States were excluded because different countries typically have different social constructions of sexual orientation, cultural values about sexuality, and institutional policies and practices about sexual orientation (Human Rights Watch, 2009; Pew Research Center, 2013b).

Search Procedure

A behavioral and social sciences librarian was consulted to assist with developing a search string and identifying relevant computerized bibliographic databases in which to search. The following search string was used to search the PsycINFO and Sociological Abstracts databases for studies published between January 1, 1990 and September 4, 2019 (i.e., the day the searches were performed): (identity OR milestone OR development) in Abstract AND (gay OR lesbian OR bisexual OR homosexual OR queer OR “sexual minority” OR “sexual minorities”) in Abstract AND (“sexual orientation” in Subjects for PsycINFO; sexuality in Subject Heading for Sociological Abstracts). The EBSCO platform was used for the PsycINFO searches and ProQuest was used for the Sociological Abstracts searches. The English language filter was also used in the searches. The PsycINFO and Sociological Abstracts databases include published empirical literature, as well as gray literature sources (e.g., unpublished dissertations, conference proceedings, and working papers). These more formal bibliographic database searches were supplemented with searches of Google Scholar, which also contains published and gray literature. The first 100 Google Scholar search results were examined.

Study Screening

After performing the bibliographic searches, 3,618 results were imported into the F1000 program to assist with organization and duplicate removal. Following duplicate removal, 3,267 studies remained. The first author and a trained research assistant independently screened each study to determine eligibility. A checklist of the inclusion and exclusion criteria was created prior to the search and was used for eligibility assessment. Most studies were included or excluded after reading the title and abstract; however, it was also necessary to examine the full-text documents of some studies to determine eligibility. To examine inter-rater agreement, the decisions of the two screeners were compared and Cohen’s kappa statistics were calculated with SPSS (version 21), which showed excellent agreement: kappa=0.96, p<0.001. There were only 14 disagreements between the screeners, which were resolved by the first author examining the full-text documents. After screening, 3,258 studies were excluded because they did not meet all of the inclusion criteria. More specifically, 1,625 studies were excluded because they did not focus on sexual orientation identity development milestones among LGB+ people, 1,177 studies were excluded because they were not empirical papers (e.g., theoretical or conceptual papers, book reviews, introductions to special issues, commentaries, and position statements), and 456 studies were excluded because they were conducted outside of the United States. After completing the search and screening processes, 30 studies were included for extraction and review (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart Depicting the Identification, Screening and Inclusion or Exclusion of Studies.

Data Extraction

A data extraction spreadsheet was developed to assist with identifying and collecting relevant information from the 30 included studies. Information extracted included the citation, study design, sampling strategy and location, sample size and characteristics, time of data collection, types of milestones measured, timing of milestones, sequences of milestones, and comparisons of milestone timing by group characteristics of interest (i.e., sex, race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and birth cohort). The first author extracted this information, and then a trained research assistant compared the extraction spreadsheet completed by the first author with the full-text documents to check the accuracy of the extractions. There were only four points of disagreement between the extractor and checker, which were resolved together by examining the full-text documents and extractions simultaneously.

Data Synthesis

Meta-Analysis Methods

Initial review of the included studies revealed that a quantitative synthesis of milestone timing in the form of a meta-analysis could be undertaken but would need to be conducted selectively because of the methodological heterogeneity of the studies. This heterogeneity included variation in the rigor of study designs and methods, which indicated limitations regarding internal and external validity for some studies. For example, most studies used non-probability sampling, only one-third of studies used national United States samples, and over one-quarter of studies had relatively small sample sizes (N<200). Therefore, instead of performing meta-analyses of milestone ages using data from all 30 studies, we used the best available evidence from a select group of studies. The best evidence was from two studies that used national probability sampling and had large samples (N>650; Herek et al., 2010; Pew Research Center, 2013a). Meta-analysis can be performed with data from two studies (Borenstein et al., 2009); however, these two studies did not measure all of the main milestones identified in the literature. Therefore, in these instances, we used data from studies from the next level of methodological rigor: those that used national non-probability sampling and relatively large sample sizes (N>500; Fox, 1993; D’Augelli, 2002; Morris et al., 2002; Drasin et al., 2008; Dunlap, 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a; Grov et al., 2018). Many of these studies also reported milestone ages by sex, sexual orientation, and birth cohort, but not by race/ethnicity. Therefore, meta-analyses for milestone ages by race/ethnicity used the handful of studies that reported these data (Dubé, 1997; Parks et al., 2004; Martos et al., 2015; Hoenig, 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017), though most of them were not studies in the top or second tiers of methodological rigor. Meta-analyses by sex, sexual orientation, race/ethnicity, and birth cohort were performed on five of the primary milestones of interest because data for other milestones were not available.

Multiple meta-analysis models were run, one for each of the milestones of interest. All the analyses used mean age as the effect size metric. When effect size data needed for meta-analysis (i.e., standard error of the mean) were reported in other formats (i.e., standard deviation), the data were transformed using formulas recommended by Higgins et al. (2019). Meta-analyses were performed using the Meta-Essential program (version 1.5; Van Rhee et al., 2015; Suurmond et al., 2017). Random-effect models were used because we assumed that the true mean effects could vary across samples and studies. An inverse variance weighting method with an additive between-studies variance component based on the DerSimonian-Laird estimator was used (Van Rhee et al., 2015). The confidence intervals (CIs) for the mean effects were estimated using the weighted variance method for random-effect models (Sánchez-Meca and Marín-Martínez, 2008), and the CIs for the individual study effect sizes were calculated using the Student’s t-distribution. Three studies (Drasin et al., 2008; Herek et al., 2010; Grov et al., 2018) were missing data needed for the standard error (SE) of the mean. In these cases, mean imputation was used to replace the missing value with the mean of the SEs from the other studies for the same milestone. Outliers were identified if the CI of an individual study effect size did not overlap with the CI of the overall mean effect size (Harrer et al., 2019). Following recommendations from Lipsey and Wilson (2001), outliers were Winsorized by recoding each to the next eligible nearest neighbor.

Narrative Synthesis

We also used a narrative synthesis approach recommended by Petticrew and Roberts (2006) to combine findings across the 30 studies. Following this approach, findings from studies were grouped under the four research questions they corresponded to. Next, the findings were examined in each domain to identify themes, including dominant trends and patterns as well as variation in findings, such as results that deviated from dominant trends. Descriptive summaries of the evidence synthesis were then crafted for each research question. A narrative summary of the methodological characteristics of the 30 studies was also created to illustrate the strengths and limitations of the studies.

Results

A total of 30 studies were included in this review (Supplementary Table 1). Given that multiple publications or research products may have derived from the same study, we use the term “study” to refer to the original research project where data were collected from a unique sample. For simplicity, in-text citations to the 30 studies will use the first citation listed in Supplementary Table 1 for multiple citations from the same study. Next, we will present a summary appraisal of the methodological characteristics of the 30 studies. Supplementary Table 1 displays an overview of the methodological characteristics of the studies. Then, we present the results of the evidence syntheses for each research question: (1) LGB+ identity development milestones measured; (2) the timing of these milestones; (3) the sequences of these milestones; and (4) comparisons of milestone timing by sex, race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and birth cohort.

Methodological Quality of Studies

Designs

In terms of study design, 20 studies were cross-sectional with four of those 20 using multiple cross-sectional samples, and 10 studies used longitudinal designs. However, the studies with longitudinal designs only measured milestones at one wave retrospectively. Therefore, no studies used prospective longitudinal methods to assess milestones. Almost all studies (n=28) used quantitative methods; only two studies used mixed methods. Four studies used some form of probability sampling with two of these studies using national probability sampling; 26 studies used some form of non-probability sampling (e.g., convenience sampling, purposive sampling, snowball sampling, and quota sampling). One-third of studies (n=10) used national United States samples, nine studies sampled participants from a single city or urban area (e.g., San Francisco Bay Area), five studies sampled participants from a single state, four studies sampled participants from multiple cities, and two studies sampled participants from one or two United States regions (e.g., the Midwest). Among the non-national studies, samples were primarily drawn from large metropolitan areas (i.e., Chicago, Los Angeles, New York City, and San Francisco). Few studies sampled participants from the Southeast, Southwest, and Mountain-Prairie regions.

Samples

Sample sizes ranged from 16 to 2,733 participants across studies (M=890, SD=811). The average age of participants in study samples varied from 17.0 to 61.5 (M=30.8, SD=11.1). Half of the samples (n=15) were relatively equal in terms of males and females, eight studies were exclusively or primarily male samples, and seven studies were exclusively or primarily female samples. Hardly any studies reported participants’ gender identities. In terms of sexual orientation, nine studies had samples that were exclusively or primarily (i.e., 82–100%) gay/lesbian-identified participants with the remaining participants identifying as bisexual; nine studies had samples of large majorities (i.e., 60–76%) of gay/lesbian participants with smaller representation of bisexual, queer, and other sexual minority identities; five studies had samples of relatively equal numbers of gay/lesbian and bisexual participants; three studies included participants with substantial representation of gay/lesbian, bisexual, and other sexual orientation identities; one study consisted of only bisexual participants; and three studies did not provide breakdowns for sexual orientation identities. Only 10 studies reported proportions of sexual orientation identities other than gay, lesbian, or bisexual, such as queer, pansexual, and mostly gay/lesbian. In terms of the racial/ethnic breakdowns of samples, about two-thirds of studies (n=19) consisted of samples that were largely White (i.e., 65–91% White participants). Less than half of the studies included samples with at least 10% Black/African Americans or at least 10% Hispanic/Latinx Americans. Only 17 studies reported the proportion of Asian American participants, and nine of these studies included at least 5% Asian Americans. Nine studies reported the proportion of Native American participants, which ranged from 1 to 3%. Very few studies reported participants who identified as multiracial/multiethnic. In addition, few studies reported other participant demographics, including gender identity, socioeconomic status, national origin, religious background, and ability/disability status. The years data were collected during studies ranged from 1987 to 2015 (M=2001, SD=8.0). In terms of birth cohorts included in samples, 20 studies included participants from Generation X (born 1965–1980), 17 studies included participants from the Baby Boom Generation (born 1946–1964), 15 studies included participants from the Millennial Generation or Generation Y (born 1981–1996), 12 studies included participants from the Silent Generation (born 1928–1945), and only one study included participants from Generation Z (born 1997–2012). These cohort names and birth years are based on definitions by the Pew Research Center (2015, 2019b).

Milestones Measured

Studies varied regarding which milestones were assessed. Four milestones were measured in the large majority studies: self-identifying as LGB+ (n=28), coming out to others (n=24), engaging in same-sex sexual activity (n=23), and becoming aware of queer attractions or desires (n=22). Other milestones assessed in some studies included questioning one’s sexual orientation (n=9), having a romantic relationship (n=8), experiencing queer fantasies (n=4), and experiencing feelings of differentness (n=2). Although most studies assessed coming out generally (e.g., “When did you first tell someone that you are LGB?”), other studies assessed coming out in specific social contexts, including parents (n=8), family members besides parents (n=6), family in general (n=2), friends (n=3), and individuals who are LGBTQ (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, or queer) (n=1).

Timing of Milestones

Supplementary Table 2 shows the timing of the eight most commonly measured milestones across the 30 studies. The meta-analysis results are shown in the last row. These results are based on the best available evidence from 10 studies representing 13,299 total participants from probability and non-probability national samples. Although there is diversity in the samples in terms of age, birth cohort, sex, sexual orientation, and race/ethnicity; males, gay/lesbian people, and White people were slightly overrepresented in the non-probability samples. Only one study included participants from Generation Z, which is understandable because it is the most recent birth cohort. The meta-analysis results show that on average attraction and questioning one’s orientation occurred around age 13, which was based on data from eight studies in the second tier of methodological rigor for attraction and one study in the top tier of methodological rigor for questioning. Self-identifying as LGB+ and engaging in sexual activity occurred around age 18, which were based on two studies in the top tier of evidence for self-identifying and eight studies in the second tier of methodological rigor for sexual activity. Having a romantic relationship occurred around age 21, which was based on eight studies in the second tier of evidence. Coming out as LGB+ to others in general occurred around age 19 to 20, which was based on two studies in the top tier of evidence. Coming out to parents and other family members occurred a bit later, around age 23, which is based on eight studies in the second tier of methodological rigor. All of the results showed substantial heterogeneity in the mean effect sizes, which is expected because the ages individuals reach milestones may likely vary by sex, gender, sexual orientation, birth cohort, race/ethnicity, and geographic location. The CIs indicate the ranges where the true mean ages would fall in 95% of comparable studies. For example, the average age that individuals self-identify as LGB+ was 17.8 (Supplementary Table 2). The true mean age likely falls in the range of 11.6 to 24.0 in 95% of studies with methods similar to Herek et al. (2010) and Pew Research Center (2013a), which were the studies used in that meta-analysis.

Sequences of Milestones

Based on the last row of Supplementary Table 2, meta-analysis results show the most common milestone sequence is (1) becoming aware of queer attractions, (2) questioning one’s orientation, (3) self-identifying as LGB+, (4) engaging in sexual activity, (5) coming out to others, and (6) having a romantic relationship. However, the results from the studies showed that LGB+ people experienced the milestones in many different sequences. Figure 2 shows the four most common sequences for the five most commonly measured milestones. Attraction was almost universally the first milestone experienced. Only one study found that a small group of participants (5% of the sample) had engaged in sexual activity prior to becoming aware of same-sex attractions (Maguen et al., 2002). Generally, the second and third milestones were either self-identifying as LGB+ or engaging in sexual activity. The latter milestones were typically coming out to others and one’s first romantic relationship. Findings also showed that for some individuals, certain milestones did not occur consecutively but rather concurrently. For example, for some people, awareness of their attractions occurred at the same age they engaged in sexual activity or came out to others. Similarly, at the age individuals self-identified as LGB+, some were also engaging in sexual activity, coming out, or initiating romantic relationships. And while some individuals were coming out as LGB+, they were also engaging in sexual activity or having a romantic relationship.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Common Sequences of LGB+ Identity Development Milestones.

Milestone Chronology by Sex, Race/Ethnicity, Sexual Orientation, and Birth Cohort

Milestone Comparisons by Sex

Among the studies that compared the timing of milestones by sex (male vs. female) in their analyses, results showed statistically significant differences for some milestones but not others. Eight studies found that males experienced awareness of attractions earlier than females (Fox, 1993; Savin-Williams and Diamond, 2000; D’Augelli, 2002; Floyd and Bakeman, 2006; D’Augelli et al., 2008; Hoenig, 2016; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a), and three studies found no significant differences (Herdt and Boxer, 1993; Rosario et al., 1996; Martos et al., 2015). Another study found that females experienced attraction later than males, except among Millennials in which there were no significant differences (Dunlap, 2016). Regarding self-identification as LGB+, nine studies showed that males self-identified earlier than females (Fox, 1993; Rosario et al., 1996; Savin-Williams and Diamond, 2000; D’Augelli, 2002; Floyd and Bakeman, 2006; Grov et al., 2006; Herek et al., 2010; Martos et al., 2015; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a), and three studies found no significant differences (Herdt and Boxer, 1993; Hoenig, 2016). Two other studies found that males generally self-identified earlier than females, but this also depended on their cohort, with no significant differences between males and females among Millennials (Dunlap, 2016) and among individuals whose sexual identity development started later in life (Calzo et al., 2011). For first sexual activity, seven studies found that males initiated sexual activity before females (Fox, 1993; Herdt and Boxer, 1993; Savin-Williams and Diamond, 2000; Floyd and Bakeman, 2006; Grov et al., 2006; Calzo et al., 2011; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a), and two studies found no significant difference (Rosario et al., 1996; Hoenig, 2016). Another study found that females had first sexual activity later than males, except among Millennials in which there were no significant differences (Dunlap, 2016). Two studies found that males had their first romantic relationship earlier than females (Fox, 1993; Martos et al., 2015), one study found that females had a romantic relationship before males (D’Augelli, 2002), and a fourth study found no significant differences (Dunlap, 2016). Regarding coming out to others, eight studies found no significant differences by sex for the disclosure milestone (Fox, 1993; Savin-Williams and Diamond, 2000; D’Augelli, 2002; Floyd and Bakeman, 2006; Grov et al., 2006; D’Augelli et al., 2008; Martos et al., 2015; Hoenig, 2016). Another study found no significant difference in coming out between males and females, except in the Silent Generation, when males came out earlier than females (Dunlap, 2016). And, another study found that among individuals whose sexual identity development started later in life, females came out earlier than males (Calzo et al., 2011). A final study showed that males came out earlier than females (Herek et al., 2010).

Supplementary Table 3 shows the results of meta-analyses of milestone ages for females and males based on data from six studies that provided milestone ages by sex; five of these studies are in the second tier of methodological rigor and one study is in the top tier (Fox, 1993; D’Augelli, 2002; Herek et al., 2010; Dunlap, 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a). As Supplementary Table 3 shows, on average, males experienced the attraction, self-identification, and sexual activity milestones earlier than females. However, males and females came out and had their first romantic relationship at about the same ages, though females were slightly earlier. The order of milestones was similar for males and females beginning with attraction, followed by self-identification, sexual activity, romantic relationship, and disclosure. Finally, despite starting later than males, females achieved all five of the main milestones in a shorter amount of time (15.3 to 21.9=6.6years), compared to males (12.9 to 22.3=9.4years).

Milestone Comparisons by Race/Ethnicity

Among the studies that compared the timing of milestones between racial/ethnic groups in their analyses, three studies found no statistically significant differences between groups for any milestone (Rosario et al., 1996; Herek et al., 2010; Martos et al., 2015). Nevertheless, four other studies found some significant differences for certain milestones but not others. In a study of male youth, Hispanic/Latino youth reported awareness of attractions earlier than Black and White youth, and Asian youth reported first sexual activity later than White, Black, and Hispanic/Latino youth (Dubé and Savin-Williams, 1999). Similarly, a study of young adults found that Hispanic/Latina women reported initial sexual activity earlier than White women, and Black men reported initial sexual activity earlier than Asian, White, and other racial/ethnic men (Grov et al., 2006). Another study of youth found no significant differences in milestone timing except that Black youth self-identified and came out later than Latinx youth (Hoenig, 2016). In a study of gay/lesbian women, there were no significant differences between Black and Hispanic/Latina women, but compared to women of color, White women were significantly later in questioning their orientation, self-identifying as gay/lesbian, coming out, and having a same-sex romantic relationship (Parks et al., 2004).

Supplementary Table 3 shows the results of meta-analyses of milestone ages by race/ethnicity based on data from five studies that provided age breakdowns by race/ethnicity; four of these studies are in the third tier of methodological rigor and one study is in the second tier (Dubé and Savin-Williams, 1999; Parks et al., 2004; Martos et al., 2015; Hoenig, 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017). Supplementary Table 3 shows that Hispanic/Latinx individuals experienced attractions earliest, followed by Asian and White individuals, with Black/African American individuals reporting attraction latest. Racial/ethnic differences for ages when individuals self-identified as LGB+ are limited, with Hispanic/Latinx and Asian people self-identifying slightly earlier than Black/African American and White people. Regarding initial sexual activity, Black/African American and Hispanic/Latinx people engaged in sexual activity slightly earlier, followed by White and then Asian individuals. Similarly, Hispanic/Latinx and Black/African American individuals reported having romantic relationships and coming out slightly earlier than Asian and White individuals. Regarding milestone sequences, all groups followed a sequence of attraction, sexual activity, self-identify, romantic relationship, and coming out. Despite starting latest, Black/African American people achieved all five of the milestones in the shortest amount of time (12.3 to 20.5=8.2years), followed by Hispanic/Latinx people (10.6 to 19.8=9.2), Asians (11.3 to 21.1=9.8), and White people (11.4 to 21.2=9.8).

Milestone Comparisons by Sexual Orientation

Among the seven studies that compared the timing of milestones between gay/lesbian and bisexual people in their analyses, most studies found that gay/lesbian people reached the milestones of attraction and self-identifying significantly earlier than bisexual people (Diamond, 1998; Maguen et al., 2002; Herek et al., 2010; Calzo et al., 2011; Martos et al., 2015; Hoenig, 2016; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a). Only one study examined first romantic relationship and found no significant sexual orientation differences (Martos et al., 2015). Few studies examined comparisons for sexual activity and coming out, and these results were more mixed and nuanced. For example, in two studies, gender played an interactive role. In one study, attraction was experienced earlier for gay men than bisexual men, with no differences between gay/lesbian women and bisexual women (Katz-Wise et al., 2017a). Similarly, another study found that sexual activity was earlier for gay men than bisexual men, with no differences among women; coming out was earlier for gay/lesbian women than bisexual women, with no difference among men (Maguen et al., 2002).

Supplementary Table 3 shows the results of meta-analyses of milestone ages by sexual orientation (bisexual and gay/lesbian) based on data from four studies; two of these studies are in the top tier of methodological rigor and two are in the second tier (Herek et al., 2010; Pew Research Center, 2013a; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a); however, none of these studies examined the relationship milestone. Gay/lesbian people experienced the milestones earlier than bisexual people—about 1 to 2years for attraction, self-identifying, and sexual activity. However, coming out to others occurred at about the same age for both groups. In terms of milestone sequencing, bisexual and gay/lesbian people experienced attraction, followed by sexual activity, self-identification, and coming out. Despite starting later, bisexual people completed the milestones in a shorter amount of time (16.9 to 21.6=4.7years) compared to gay/lesbian people (15.1 to 21.3=6.2).

Milestone Comparisons by Birth Cohort

Among the studies that compared the timing of milestones between birth cohorts, all found significant differences between cohorts for the milestones of self-identifying, coming out, and romantic relationship whereby older cohorts experienced milestones later in life compared to younger cohorts (Fox, 1993; Floyd and Stein, 2002; Floyd and Bakeman, 2006; Grov et al., 2006, 2018; Parks and Hughes, 2007; Drasin et al., 2008; Herek et al., 2010; Calzo et al., 2011; Pew Research Center, 2013a; Martos et al., 2015; Dunlap, 2016; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017). However, findings were less consistent for the milestones of attraction and sexual activity. Four studies did not find significant differences in age of first attraction, but this finding only applied to males (Drasin et al., 2008; Martos et al., 2015; Dunlap, 2016; Grov et al., 2018). Similarly, three studies did not find substantial differences between cohorts in age of initial sexual activity among males (Grov et al., 2006; Drasin et al., 2008; Dunlap, 2016). In addition, although studies found significant cohort differences in age of self-identifying, these differences were smaller in several studies compared to the milestones of first disclosure and romantic relationship (Drasin et al., 2008; Martos et al., 2015; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017).

Supplementary Table 3 shows the results of meta-analyses of milestone ages by birth cohort based on data from five studies; four of these studies are in the second tier of methodological rigor and one study is in the top tier (Fox, 1993; Pew Research Center, 2013a; Dunlap, 2016; Grov et al., 2018). The timing of milestones was earliest for Millennials, followed by Generation X, Baby Boomers, and the Silent Generation. Millennials also achieved all five of the milestones in the shortest amount of time (12.0 to 17.9=5.9years), followed by Generation X (12.7 to 20.1=7.4years), Baby Boomers (13.1 to 24.3=11.2years), and the Silent Generation (15.0 to 29.2=14.2years). Millennials also differed from the other birth cohorts in milestone sequence with sexual activity following self-identifying as LGB+, whereas sexual activity preceded self-identifying as LGB+ for Generation X and Baby Boomers, and the Silent Generation self-identified as LGB+ and engaged at sexual activity at the same age. The attraction milestone showed limited differences between Millennials, Generation X, and Baby Boomers occurring at age 12 to 13, whereas the Silent Generation experienced attraction at 15. The romantic relationship milestone shows the most differences between birth cohorts.

Discussion

The purpose of this article was to systematically review studies examining sexual orientation identity development milestones among LGB+ people. The findings will be discussed according to the research questions that drove the review.

Milestones Measured

Four milestones were measured in about 75% of studies (i.e., self-identifying, coming out generally, sexual activity, and awareness of attractions), another four milestones were measured in about 20–30% of studies (i.e., questioning one’s sexual orientation, romantic relationship, and coming out to parents and family members), and another set of four milestones were measured in only a handful of studies (i.e., sexual fantasies, feeling different, and coming out to friends and LGBTQ people). This indicates that researchers view certain milestones as more significant in the development of a LGB+ identity. Initial awareness of queer attractions or desires is typically experienced first, making it an important initial marker in sexuality development. However, there are multiple related yet different experiences that can be considered attraction/desire (Foster, 2009; Diamond, 2010). One component, proceptivity, encompasses urges to seek sexual activity. Another is arousability, the capacity to become interested in sexual activity after encountering erotic stimuli. Attraction can also be physical or romantic in nature, with romantic attraction involving desire for emotional intimacy, dating, or a romantic relationship. None of the studies reviewed operationalized attraction with these three dimensions.

Sexual activity was another milestone that was frequently measured with measurement limitations. First sexual activity is an important developmental event that has implications for health and psychosocial wellbeing. The large majority of studies assessed sexual activity with a general question (e.g., “How old were you when you first had a sexual encounter with someone of the same sex?”) rather than a series of items about various sexual acts (e.g., penile-vaginal intercourse, penile-anal intercourse, oral sex, and manual stimulation). Although a single general question to measure sexual activity is advantageous in terms of feasibility and respondent burden, individuals’ understandings of what acts constitute sex varies; for example, some individuals do not consider oral or manual stimulation of the genitals to be sex (Bogart et al., 2000; Peterson and Muehlenhard, 2007; Sanders et al., 2010). Use of a single general question about sexual activity allows for openness to interpretation by respondents who have various notions about what forms of contact count as sex.

Choosing an identity label that best fits one’s experiences and applying that label to one’s self is a self-defining moment and perhaps the most fundamental of the milestones in sexual orientation identity development. It should be noted that this milestone can occur at multiple points in development. Research shows that a substantial minority of people change their identity label over time (Ott et al., 2011; Mock and Eibach, 2012; Savin-Williams et al., 2012; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017). For example, some people who initially identify as bisexual later identify as gay/lesbian, mostly heterosexual, or completely heterosexual. Those who report being unsure of their identity often later identified as bisexual, gay/lesbian, or heterosexual. And some people who initially identified as mostly heterosexual later identified as bisexual or completely heterosexual. These findings show that there is fluidity in sexual orientation, though most individuals report stability in their self-identification. Researchers have also found new identity labels being used and variation in the meanings of identities, with the same identity label having different meanings from person to person (Rust, 2000; Walton et al., 2016; Galupo et al., 2017; Hall, 2019; Watson et al., 2020). This is unsurprising because sexual orientation identities are socially constructed.

The first time a person discloses their LGB+ identity to someone else is another important milestone given societal heteronormativity where people are presumed to be heterosexual unless otherwise stated. Coming out to others is not a one-time event, but rather a multitude of events occurring over the life course with various people (e.g., family members, friends, classmates, and co-workers) and settings (e.g., home, neighborhood, school, workplace, place of worship, and social media). Nonetheless, the first time someone discloses their LGB+ identity to a friend or family member may be particularly vulnerable for the person coming out, and the nature of reactions of others can significantly influence mental health and interpersonal relationships in positive and negative ways (Beals and Peplau, 2006; Rosario et al., 2009; Rothman et al., 2012; D’Amico et al., 2015; Ryan et al., 2015). The emotional and behavioral reactions of others to disclosure can vary considerably from affirmation to rejection.

Certain milestones (i.e., feeling different and questioning one’s sexual orientation) may have been less frequently measured because they are more ambiguous in nature and less of a distinct, salient event. It should also be noted that despite the pervasiveness of heteronormativity, not all LGB+ people may experience feelings of differentness and question their orientation. Traditionally, children have been raised under the presumption of heterosexuality because a heterosexual identity is the norm and ideal in United States society. However, recently emerged parenting practices challenge presumptions of heteronormativity among children and adolescents by forwarding sexual diversity as the social norm with multiple valued sexual identities, including LGB+ identities (Helsel and Harris-Smith, 2020; Planned Parenthood, 2020). Young people growing up with this socialization with an emerging LGB+ identity may not feel confused, uncertain, or out-of-place when LGB+ feelings arise.

Timing of Milestones

The meta-analysis results show that, on average, attraction and questioning one’s orientation occurred during early adolescence, whereas self-identifying as LGB+, sexual activity, and initially coming out occurred during late adolescence; coming out to parents and other family members and having a romantic relationship occurred during emerging adulthood. The general timing of these milestones occurring during adolescence and emerging adulthood is expected because adolescence is a time of self-discovery and self-definition, as well as a blossoming in sexual feelings and experiences following puberty (Hall and Rounds, 2013). Nonetheless, the broad CIs and SDs for mean milestone ages (Supplementary Tables 2 and 3) demonstrate that for some individuals, milestones were experienced as early as childhood or as late as middle adulthood.

When an individual reaches a milestone, the developmental context has significant implications for how they experience the milestone and its impact on their biopsychosocial trajectories (Hall et al., 2022). For example, a person who becomes aware of same-sex attractions and questions their orientation during early adolescence may have less self-knowledge and insight about sexuality or have access to fewer resources to help them process and make sense of their experience, compared to someone in late adolescence. And, for example, someone who comes out to their parents during early adolescence versus later, such as late adolescence or young adulthood, would likely have different experiences with varying developmental implications. One’s parents may be more accepting or rejecting of their child’s identity. Hostile parental reactions could have more negative effects on the child’s mental health, housing stability, and financial status if the child is 13years old compared to age 18 or 25, when children are generally more independent and psychologically autonomous from their parents. Conversely, coming out to parents at an earlier age with neutral or positive reactions may preclude stressors from remaining in the closet to one’s family until a later age (e.g., hiding one’s identity, feeling dishonest, and interpersonal distance) and be beneficial for the individual and the parent-child relationship. Stress related to hiding one’s LGB+ identity is associated with depressive symptoms (Hall, 2018).

Although some of the sexual orientation identity development milestones are unique to LGB+ people (e.g., coming out), others are experienced across the human condition regardless of sexual orientation (e.g., attraction). The average age of first sexual attraction found in our analysis for LGB+ people (i.e., 12.7years) corresponds with results from studies with the general population (Bancroft et al., 2003; Reynolds et al., 2003). The timing of attraction is presumably biologically based, with the onset of desire to seek sexual activity following gonadarche (i.e., pubertal changes involving the growth of the ovaries and testes and hormonal increases in estrogen and testosterone), which typically begins around age 11–12 (Wan et al., 2012; Chulani and Gordon, 2014; Diamond et al., 2015; Wohlfahrt-Veje et al., 2016). Nonetheless, the capacity for arousability can occur following adrenarche (i.e., pre-pubertal increases in adrenal androgen hormones), which begins during ages 5–8 (Rainey et al., 2002; Remer et al., 2005; Diamond et al., 2015).

Another common milestone is first sexual contact, which we found occurred on average at age 18.1. Studies with the general population have found that first sexual activity occurs around age 17 (Haydon et al., 2012; National Center for Health Statistics, 2017), which is slightly earlier. LGB+ individuals may initiate sexual activity slightly later than their heterosexual counterparts because they may be working through issues specific to their emerging LGB+ identity. For example, unlike heterosexual individuals, LGB+ people may need time to process their attractions considered abnormal or unacceptable by society, explore their sexuality, choose an identity label that fits their experiences, and confront sexual stigma. In addition, LGB+ sexual activity may be delayed because many young people have not received affirmative, accurate, or comprehensive education about LGB+ sexual activity. Indeed, researchers have found that sex education typically focuses on heterosexuality; ignores or stigmatizes LGB+ sexualities; and emphasizes abstinence, often abstinence until heterosexual marriage (McNeill, 2013; Estes, 2017; Hall et al., 2019).

Meta-analytic results of the age of one’s first LGB+ romantic relationship (i.e., 20.9) were significantly later than what studies have found for heterosexual individuals. Research with heterosexual samples shows that one’s first romantic relationship occurred around age 16.6–18 (Regan et al., 2004; Zimmer-Gembeck et al., 2004; Raffaelli, 2005; Smiler et al., 2011). The later onset of romantic relationship initiation for LGB+ people may be due to several factors. LGB+ individuals would likely not initiate a queer relationship until after they have self-identified as LGB+ and started coming out, which we found occurred at ages 17.8 and 19.6, respectively. Socio-historically, before the proliferation of LGBTQ community centers and organized groups as well as LGBTQ-specific online dating services and apps, older generations of LGB+ people often relied on LGBTQ bars and night clubs to meet prospective romantic partners, and these institutions had age restrictions of 18 or 21. Internalized stigma may also play a role whereby individuals with higher levels of internalized stigma may forgo LGB+ relationships because they have internalized societal views that queer relationships are immoral, abnormal, or unhealthy. Findings from one study showed that internalized stigma was associated with loneliness, lack of warmth and trust in interpersonal relationships, and inhibition in sexual desire (Frost and Meyer, 2009).

Sequences of Milestones

The results demonstrate that although there are some dominant patterns in milestone sequence, there is also substantial diversity in trajectories. Although theoretical models proposing common linear sequences of stages in LGB+ identity development (e.g., Cass, 1979, 1984, 1996; Coleman, 1982) were noteworthy efforts during their time to advance scholarship in this understudied area, our findings and related scholarship underscore that LGB+ identity development is more diverse, dynamic, and complex than the stage models postulated (Eliason and Schope, 2007; Savin-Williams, 2011; Morgan, 2012; Savin-Williams and Cohen, 2015). Our results indicate that LGB+ identity development, like many aspects of human development, can be understood as a cascade model in which early sexual experiences serve as bases for and influence future sexual experiences in the life course (Diamond et al., 2015). For instance, attraction appears to be a foundational milestone that influences subsequent milestones of self-identification and sexual activity. Further, self-identifying as LGB+ is undoubtedly a necessary precursor of coming out to others as LGB+; one’s patterns of attraction and self-identification guide the romantic relationships individuals pursue; and one’s first coming out experience likely shapes the timing and form of subsequent disclosure events. There are multiple cascading patterns in sexuality development. Theory about the conditions or factors that shape particular sequences of milestones needs elaboration. Milestone sequences are undoubtedly influenced by biological, psychological, social, and cultural factors. Additional research could illuminate the combinations of variables that shape particular trajectories.

Milestone Chronology by Sex, Race/Ethnicity, Sexual Orientation, and Birth Cohort

Findings indicated that, on average, males reached the milestones of attraction, self-identification, and sexual activity earlier than females; however, fewer differences were found in these milestones among Millennials. This finding may be explained by shifts in societal views about sexuality and/or females beginning puberty earlier than in past decades. From a sociocultural perspective, older birth cohorts (i.e., the Silent Generation, Baby Boomers, and Gen X) may have been highly influenced by gender role expectations. For men, a strong sex drive and pursuit of sexual encounters were valued in United States society as signs of robust masculinity, whereas women may have been influenced by messages that their sexuality should be minimized, sexually active young women were “sluts,” and sexual activity outside of monogamous committed relationships or marriage was unacceptable. National data show more sexually liberal and sex-positive views among Millennials than prior generations (Twenge et al., 2015). From a biological perspective, the average age that females begin puberty is earlier among Millennials than prior cohorts (Steingraber, 2007), which may explain the fewer differences by sex in initial milestones among Millennials. Generally, males and females came out and had their first romantic relationships at about the same ages, and females reached the main milestones in a shorter amount of time, compared to males. The more prolonged process for males could be due to males having higher levels of internalized stigma, which may delay coming out and pursuing a LGB+ relationship despite early experiences of queer attractions and encounters. Studies have found slightly higher levels of internalized stigma among gay and bisexual men (Mohr and Fassinger, 2000; Herek et al., 2009; Barnes and Meyer, 2012).

We found fewer differences in milestone timing between racial/ethnic groups. Nonetheless, Hispanic/Latinx people reached all of the main milestones first, except for initial sexual encounter, which Black/African American people experienced earliest. We also found that White people reached most of the milestones later than people of color, especially when compared with Black and Hispanic/Latinx people. These findings are consistent with evidence from the general population showing earlier sexual activity among Hispanic/Latinx and Black youth, as well as later sexual activity among Asian and White young people (O’Sullivan et al., 2007; Zimmer-Gembeck and Helfand, 2008). These differences may be due to differences in socialization about sexuality between racial/ethnic groups and the earlier onset of puberty for Black and Hispanic/Latinx youth (Ward and Wyatt, 1994; Sun et al., 2002; Meneses et al., 2006; Butts and Seifer, 2010).

Bisexual people generally reached most milestones slightly later compared to gay/lesbian people. This may be due to several factors. Given the sociocultural pressure of heteronormativity, bisexual peoples’ attractions to multiple genders, and their capacity to engage in sexual behaviors and relationships that may be viewed as heterosexual, there may be more denial, minimization, or uncertainty about their bisexual sexuality than for gay/lesbian people. In addition, because bisexuality as a legitimate sexual orientation has historically been questioned, with views that bisexuality is a transitional step between heterosexuality and homosexuality and that very few people are truly bisexual, people with an emerging bisexual identity may feel more confusion and self-doubt about their identity than people with monosexual orientations (Brown, 2002; Roberts et al., 2015; Monro et al., 2017). Longitudinal research indicates that bisexual people can experience more fluctuations in their attractions over time compared to gay/lesbian people (Diamond, 2008), which may also contribute to confusion and feeling uncertain about their bisexual identity. Bisexual people also face prejudice and discrimination from both the heterosexual community and gay/lesbian community, often related to invalidation, mistrust, and hypersexualization of a bisexual identity (Roberts et al., 2015). Such experiences likely contribute to a special form of internalized stigma (e.g., internalized binegativity/biphobia) that may also delay their sexual identity development.

Our results showed that the older the birth cohort, the later the milestones were reached. And, the older the cohort, the longer the duration between the milestones of attraction and romantic relationship. We also found that Millennials had a different milestone sequence than prior cohorts where Millennials self-identified as LGB+ following attraction, whereas other cohorts engaged in sexual activity before self-identifying. Further, Millennials engaged in sexual activity and came out at about the same age. Collectively, these findings demonstrate the influence of cohort and period effects on LGB+ identity development. Public opinion data show that Americans’ attitudes about homosexuality and LGB+ issues have improved over time (Saad, 2012; Hall and Rodgers, 2019; Pew Research Center, 2019a; Flores, 2020). Correspondingly, evidence shows that younger cohorts of Americans report more accepting attitudes about LGB+ people and issues, compared to older cohorts (Avery et al., 2007; Andersen and Fetner, 2008). More positive social climates undoubtedly facilitate individuals feeling comfortable exploring their sexuality, self-identifying as LGB+, and living openly as LGB+. Future research with Generation Z, the most recent cohort, may find that milestone timing is even earlier than Millennials, though with a similar sequence.

Strengths and Limitations of the Review

This review used a rigorous approach to identify relevant studies by searching multiple databases using an expert-informed search string and screening over 3,200 records. Search records were independently screened by two screeners based on a priori inclusion criteria. The studies reviewed included formally published sources (e.g., peer-reviewed journal articles and book chapters) and several gray literature sources (i.e., unpublished dissertations and a think tank report) to minimize publication bias. Nonetheless, unpublished research may be underrepresented in this review. Although the bibliographic databases searched were large and directly relevant to the research questions, searching additional databases may have led to the discovery of other studies. In hindsight, additional search terms referring to some of the specific milestones (e.g., coming out) could have been used, which may have revealed additional pertinent studies. Meta-analysis results were based on the best available evidence among the studies; however, few studies possessed a high level of methodological rigor regarding internal and external validity. Therefore, caution should be taken when generalizing results because certain meta-analysis results are based on two rigorous studies and other results are based on several studies with notable limitations. Another complicating factor in the review was the inconsistent measurement of the milestones. By presenting the methodological characteristics of the studies in Supplementary Table 1 and the results in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3, readers are able to assess the methodological rigor and trustworthiness of the findings.

Methodological Considerations of the Studies Reviewed

Systematic reviews not only summarize what is substantively known about a topic but also provide a critical appraisal of the state of the research on a topic. Based on our appraisal of the methodological characteristics of the studies, we identified several strengths among the studies. A substantial portion of the studies used large, national samples, and most studies had relatively large sample sizes. These sampling strengths are beneficial for the generalizability of the findings because large and national samples are more likely to be representative of the US population. The studies were generally balanced in terms of sex, with many having comparable numbers of males and females, as well as some studies that exclusively focused on men or women. Historically, LGB+ identity research has tended to focus on gay men, White gay men in particular, more so than other subgroups of the queer community (Eliason and Schope, 2007; Morgan, 2012). Studies also included sufficient representation of various birth cohorts aside from Generation Z, as well as participants of various ages.

Conversely, several prominent methodological limitations were identified among the studies. All of the studies measured milestones using retrospective, cross-sectional methods. Although such methods are highly feasible, they are not ideal for capturing events and experiences that unfold during development, particularly for respondents in middle or older adulthood who are recalling milestones that often occurred during adolescence or young adulthood. Recall error is a notable concern, especially for participants recalling events that occurred many years or even decades ago. Only a few studies used mixed methods to capture milestones and their contexts. Although quantitative methods are essential for capturing milestone chronology, qualitative methods can capture rich, in-depth data about the context and meaning of milestone experiences. Sampling issues and the representation of various subgroups within the LGB+ population were limited for some studies. Many samples were largely or primarily White, with limited representation of people of color. Numerous studies focused on gay/lesbian individuals, followed by bisexual individuals, with limited inclusion of other sexual minorities (e.g., queer and pansexual people). There was also limited measurement of important demographics, including gender identity, socioeconomic status, national origin, immigrant/citizenship status, ability/disability status, and religious background.

Recommendations for Future Research

We anticipate that research on LGB+ identity development will continue to expand. In order to build upon and address gaps and limitations in the extant literature, we present six recommendations for future research. These recommendations are based on the critical appraisal of methodological characteristics and findings from the studies reviewed.

First, future research should improve sampling methods. Historically, LGB+ people have been a hard-to-reach, vulnerable, and hidden population for researchers (Meyer and Wilson, 2009; Ellard-Gray et al., 2015), although inclusion of this population has improved in recent decades. Nationally representative samples of LGB+ people that reflect the diversity of America are needed to produce generalizable knowledge. Online-, telephone-, and address-based sampling methods can facilitate samples that are national and more representative. Nonetheless, well-designed and well-executed non-probability community-based samples are still needed. Community-based studies can explore in-depth experiences from segments of the LGB+ community who are particularly hard-to-reach and/or have been underrepresented in extant research, including sexual minorities other than gays/lesbians (e.g., queer, bisexual, and pansexual people), transgender and nonbinary people, people of color, people living in rural areas, people living in the South and Mountain-Prairie region, and Generation Z. Community venue-based sampling, time-space sampling, respondent-driven sampling, adaptive sampling, and follow-back methods may be useful approaches in reaching these groups (Corliss et al., 2009a; Meyer and Wilson, 2009), as well as strategies to overcome barriers to participation (Elze, 2009; Rodríguez Rust, 2009; Ellard-Gray et al., 2015).

Second, more research is needed on intersectionality. The queer community is a heterogeneous population group and one’s sexual orientation identity intersects with many other identities and social locations (e.g., race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sex, gender identity, ability/disability status, immigrant/citizenship status, religious/spiritual orientation, and age group), which are associated with various social systems of privilege and oppression (e.g., racism, classism, sexism, cisgenderism, ableism, nativism, religism, and ageism). An intersectional approach can be incorporated in the conceptualization, sampling, data collection methods, and data analysis of a study (Bowleg, 2008; Warner, 2008; Cole, 2009).

Third, future research must use more rigorous study designs. Single or multiple cohort prospective longitudinal studies with appropriate time intervals to capture milestones would be the most rigorous designs, yet also the most resource-intensive. Cross-sequential studies are more feasible than longitudinal studies and more rigorous than cross-sectional studies. Another rigorous alternative that has been hardly used in LGB+ research is the life history/event calendar design. Life history/event calendar methods were developed to address concerns about recall error in traditional retrospective interviews, surveys, and questionnaires in the social/behavioral sciences (Freedman et al., 1988; Caspi et al., 1996; Glasner and van der Vaart, 2009). Rather than asking discrete questions about milestones via an interview or a self-administered questionnaire (e.g., “How old were you when you first identified as LGB?”), calendar methods use visual aids, cognitive recall strategies, and interactive data collection to minimize recall error (Belli, 1998; Axinn et al., 1999; Belli and Callegaro, 2009; Glasner and van der Vaart, 2009). Calendar methods can yield quantitative and qualitative retrospective life-course data and have several advantages over traditional retrospective methods (e.g., recall accuracy and data completeness) and over prospective longitudinal methods (e.g., no attrition and no testing effects). Though calendar methods provide advantages regarding feasibility and validity and are well-suited for addressing critical gaps in knowledge in this area, only a handful of studies have used calendar methods with LGB+ populations (Fisher, 2012, 2013; Fisher et al., 2014; Goldbach and Gibbs, 2015, 2017).

Fourth, regardless of the number and timing of data collection points, future studies should use more mixed-methods designs. Quantitative methods can capture important variables, including the timing, sequencing, durations between, and frequency of milestones. Despite the descriptive utility of mapping chronological signposts in the identity development process, exclusive use of quantitative methods misses important information about the context and meaning of milestones. Indeed, sexual orientation identity development involves diverse, complex, and dynamic developmental experiences and processes unfolding in psychological and social contexts over time. Qualitative methods can provide important data about the meaning of sexual orientation identities across individuals, groups, places, and times; ways individuals explore, process, and integrate their sexual orientation; and psychosocial contexts of milestones, such as individuals’ psychological reactions to milestones, ways social and cultural forces influence their experiences, changes in interpersonal relationships due to milestone experiences, ways individuals cope with difficult milestone experiences, and experiences that lead to adaptive psychosocial functioning and resilience.

Fifth, researchers should improve the measurement and reporting of milestones. Although dependent on the purpose of a study, researchers should endeavor to measure as many of the key milestones as possible to develop more comprehensive knowledge on LGB+ identity development trajectories. Measurement concerns about the construct validity of certain milestones, as discussed previously for attraction and sexual activity, can be addressed by using specific, multi-item measures. Given that LGB+ young people are disproportionate targets for sexual abuse and sexual assault (Saewyc et al., 2006; Friedman et al., 2011; Rothman et al., 2011), questions about sexual activity may need to distinguish between consensual and non-consensual encounters. Questions about coming out can ask about the very first time someone comes out to others as LGB+, as well as disclosure to specific people (e.g., parents, siblings, and friends) and social settings (e.g., school, workplace, neighborhood, and religious/spiritual community). Researchers could also allow respondents to indicate if they were outed (i.e., their identity was disclosed to someone else without their consent) or if their identity was inadvertently discovered by someone else. Although milestone research typically captures the first time a milestone occurs, a substantial minority of LGB+ people experience fluidity in their identity. Therefore, measures can be crafted so that they can capture milestones experienced more than once (e.g., someone initially identifies as lesbian, but later questions their identity and self-identifies as bisexual). In reporting the timing of milestones, researchers should continue to report means and standard deviations, but also medians and ranges. Medians are robust against outliers and skewed distributions, which occur in developmental data. Similarly, although standard deviations are useful statistics to understand the spread of the data, ranges in terms of minimum and maximum values convey the total span of the data.

Finally, although a number of researchers have investigated associations between milestone variables and psychosocial outcomes (e.g., Floyd and Stein, 2002; Friedman et al., 2008; Katz-Wise et al., 2017a; Rendina et al., 2019), more research is needed to examine how LGB+ identity development relates to mental, behavioral, and social outcomes. As described earlier in this paper, LGB+ people face disproportionately high rates of depression, anxiety, suicidal ideation and behavior, substance abuse, sexually transmitted infections, and disordered eating. Milestone variables may be directly and indirectly related to important psychosocial outcomes (e.g., mental health, substance use, life satisfaction, interpersonal wellbeing, relationship satisfaction, and self-esteem). In addition, key mediating and moderating variables need to be identified, which could inform the development of interventions. These mediating or moderating variables could include individual variables (e.g., age, race/ethnicity, gender, and religiosity), psychological variables (e.g., self-acceptance, internalized stigma, personal authenticity, identity centrality/prominence, and identity integration), interpersonal variables (e.g., outness, family reactions, social support, and involvement with the LGBTQ community), and sociocultural environmental variables (e.g., urbanicity/rurality, socio-political climate, and policy landscape). Such research should not rely on simplistic and reductionist analytic methods, but rather advanced methods, such as general structural equation modeling, latent profile analysis, latent curve modeling, regression modeling with interactions, machine learning for qualitative analysis, hierarchical linear modeling, and mixed methods. Understanding complex phenomena often requires complex methods.

Conclusion

This systematic review and meta-analysis demonstrate the complexity of sexual orientation identity development for LGB+ people—an important dimension of human development involving many milestones that are cascading over time, shaped by biopsychosocial factors, and illustrative of common and unique developmental trajectories. Additional research is needed to further explicate these developmental journeys, which have significant implications for the wellbeing and lives of LGB+ people.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author Contributions

WH conceptualized the study and led the study design. WH, HD, and NP analyzed the data, interpreted the results, and wrote and edited parts of the manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript as submitted.

Funding

Research reported in this article was supported by the National Institute of Minority Health and Health Disparities of the National Institutes of Health under award number R01MD015109-01A1.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753954/full#supplementary-material

References

Andersen, R., and Fetner, T. (2008). Cohort differences in tolerance of homosexuality attitudinal change in Canada and the United States, 1981–2000. Public Opin. Q. 72, 311–330. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfn017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avery, A., Chase, J., Johansson, L., Litvak, S., Montero, D., and Wydra, M. (2007). America’s changing attitudes toward homosexuality, civil unions, and same-gender marriage: 1977–2004. Soc. Work 52, 71–79. doi: 10.1093/sw/52.1.71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axinn, W. G., Pearce, L. D., and Ghimire, D. (1999). Innovations in life history calendar applications. Soc. Sci. Res. 28, 243–264. doi: 10.1006/ssre.1998.0641

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baams, L., Grossman, A. H., and Russell, S. T. (2015). Minority stress and mechanisms of risk for depression and suicidal ideation among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth. Dev. Psychol. 51, 688–696. doi: 10.1037/a0038994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baiocco, R., Salvati, M., Carone, N., Ioverno, S., Nappa, M. R., and Pistella, J. (2018). Identità positiva in persone lesbiche, gay e bisessuali_ un contributo alla validazione italiana della [Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Positive Identity Measure (LGB-PIM)]. J. Psychol. 45, 953–978. doi: 10.1421/93010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsam, K. F., Rothblum, E. D., and Beauchaine, T. P. (2005). Victimization over the life span: a comparison of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and heterosexual siblings. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73:477. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bancroft, J. H., Herbenick, D. L., and Reynolds, M. A. (2003). “Masturbation as a marker of sexual development: two studies 50 years apart,” in Sexual Development in Childhood. ed. J. Bancroft (United States: Indiana University Press), 156–185.

Google Scholar

Barnes, D. M., and Meyer, I. H. (2012). Religious affiliation, internalized homophobia, and mental health in lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 82:505. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01185.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beals, K. P., and Peplau, L. A. (2006). Disclosure patterns within social networks of gay men and lesbians. J. Homosex. 51, 101–120. doi: 10.1300/J082v51n02_06

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belli, R. F. (1998). The structure of autobiographical memory and the event history calendar: potential improvements in the quality of retrospective reports in surveys. Memory 6, 383–406. doi: 10.1080/741942610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belli, R. F., and Callegaro, M. (2009). “The emergence of calendar interviewing: A theoretical and empirical rationale,” in Calendar and Time Diary Methods in Life Course Research. eds. R. F. Belli, F. P. Stafford, and D. F. Alwin (United States: Sage), 31–52.

Google Scholar

Bieber, I. (1962). Homosexuality—A Psychoanalytic Study of Male Homosexuals. United States: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bieber, I. (1967). “Homosexuality,” in Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. eds. A. M. Freedman and H. Kaplan (United States: Williams and Wilkins), 963–976.

Google Scholar

Bieber, I. (1969). Homosexuality. Am. J. Nurs. 69, 2637–2641.

Google Scholar

Bogart, L. M., Cecil, H., Wagstaff, D. A., Pinkerton, S. D., and Abramson, P. R. (2000). Is it “sex”? College students’ interpretations of sexual behavior terminology. J. Sex Res. 37, 108–116. doi: 10.1080/00224490009552027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. United States: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Bowleg, L. (2008). When Black+ lesbian+ woman≠ Black lesbian woman: The methodological challenges of qualitative and quantitative intersectionality research. Sex Roles 59, 312–325. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9400-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. (2002). A proposed model of bisexual identity development that elaborates on experiential differences of women and men. J. Bisexual. 2, 67–91. doi: 10.1300/J159v02n04_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butts, S. F., and Seifer, D. B. (2010). Racial and ethnic differences in reproductive potential across the life cycle. Fertil. Steril. 93, 681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2009.10.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calzo, J. P., Antonucci, T. C., Mays, V. M., and Cochran, S. D. (2011). Retrospective recall of sexual orientation identity development among gay, lesbian, and bisexual adults. Dev. Psychol. 47:1658. doi: 10.1037/a0025508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Thornton, A., Freedman, D., Amell, J. W., Harrington, H., et al. (1996). The life history calendar: a research and clinical assessment method for collecting retrospective event-history data. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 6, 101–114. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1234-988X(199607)6:2<101::AID-MPR156>3.3.CO;2-E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cass, V. C. (1979). Homosexuality identity formation: A theoretical model. J. Homosex. 4, 219–235.

Google Scholar

Cass, V. C. (1984). Homosexual identity formation: testing a theoretical model. J. Sex Res. 20, 143–167.

Google Scholar

Cass, V. (1996). “Sexual orientation identity formation: A Western phenomenon,” in Textbook of Homosexuality and Mental Health. eds. R. P. Cabaj and T. S. Stein (Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association), 227–251.

Google Scholar

Choi, S. K., and Meyer, I. H. (2016). LGBT Aging: A Review of Research Findings, Needs, and Policy Implications. United States: Williams Institute.

Google Scholar

Chulani, V. L., and Gordon, L. P. (2014). Adolescent growth and development. Prim. Care 41, 465–487. doi: 10.1016/j.pop.2014.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. Am. Psychol. 64, 170–180. doi: 10.1037/a0014564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, E. (1982). Developmental stages of the coming out process. J. Homosex. 7, 31–43.

Google Scholar

Corliss, H. L., Cochran, S. D., and Mays, V. M. (2009a). “Sampling approaches to studying mental health concerns in the lesbian, gay, and bisexual community,” in Handbook of Research with Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Populations. eds. W. Meezan and J. I. Martin (United Kingdom: Routledge), 131–158.

Google Scholar

Corliss, H. L., Cochran, S. D., Mays, V. M., Greenland, S., and Seeman, T. E. (2009b). Age of minority sexual orientation development and risk of childhood maltreatment and suicide attempts in women. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 79, 511–521. doi: 10.1037/a0017163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Amico, E., Julien, D., Tremblay, N., and Chartrand, E. (2015). Gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths coming out to their parents: parental reactions and youths’ outcomes. J. GLBT Fam. Stud. 11, 411–437. doi: 10.1080/1550428X.2014.981627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R. (2002). Mental health problems among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths ages 14 to 21. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 7, 433–456. doi: 10.1177/1359104502007003010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R. (2003). Lesbian and bisexual female youths aged 14 to 21: developmental challenges and victimization experiences. J. Lesbian Stud. 7, 9–29. doi: 10.1300/j155v07n04_02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R. (2006). “Developmental and contextual factors and mental health among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths,” in Sexual Orientation and Mental Health: Examining Identity and Development in Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual People. eds. A. M. Omoto and H. S. Kurtzman (United States: American Psychological Association), 37–53.

Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R., Grossman, A. H., and Starks, M. T. (2008). Gender atypicality and sexual orientation development among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth: Prevalence, sex differences, and parental responses. J. Gay and Lesbian Ment. 12, 121–143. doi: 10.1300/J529v12n01_08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’augelli, A. R., and Hershberger, S. L. (1993). Lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth in community settings: personal challenges and mental health problems. Am. J. Community Psychol. 21, 421–448. doi: 10.1007/BF00942151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Augelli, A. R., Hershberger, S. L., and Pilkington, N. W. (1998). Lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth and their families: disclosure of sexual orientation and its consequences. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 68, 361–371. doi: 10.1037/h0080345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M. (1998). Development of sexual orientation among adolescent and young adult women. Dev. Psychol. 34, 1085–1094. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M. (2003). Was it a phase? Young women’s relinquishment of lesbian/bisexual identities over a 5-year period. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:352. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M. (2005). A new view of lesbian subtypes: stable versus fluid identity trajectories over an 8-year period. Psychol. Women Q. 29, 119–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00174.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M. (2008). Female bisexuality from adolescence to adulthood: results from a 10-year longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 44:5. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M. (2010). “Sexual desire,” in The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. 4th Edn. eds. I. B. Weiner and W. E. Craighead (United States: John Wiley and Sons), 1566–1567.

Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M., Bonner, S. B., and Dickenson, J. (2015). “The development of sexuality,” in Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science. Vol. 3. 7th Edn. eds. M. E. Lamb and R. M. Lerner (United States: John Wiley and Sons), 888–931.

Google Scholar

Dirkes, J., Hughes, T., Ramirez-Valles, J., Johnson, T., and Bostwick, W. (2016). Sexual identity development: relationship with lifetime suicidal ideation in sexual minority women. J. Clin. Nurs. 25, 3545–3556. doi: 10.1111/jocn.13313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drasin, H., Beals, K. P., Elliott, M. N., Lever, J., Klein, D. J., and Schuster, M. A. (2008). Age cohort differences in the developmental milestones of gay men. J. Homosex. 54, 381–399. doi: 10.1080/00918360801991372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drescher, J. (2015). Out of DSM: depathologizing homosexuality. Behav. Sci. 5, 565–575. doi: 10.3390/bs5040565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubé, E. M. (1997). Sexual Identity and Intimacy Development among Two Cohorts of Gay and Bisexual Men. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.

Google Scholar

Dubé, E. M. (2000). The role of sexual behavior in the identification process of gay and bisexual males. J. Sex Res. 37, 123–132. doi: 10.1080/00224490009552029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubé, E. M., and Savin-Williams, R. C. (1999). Sexual identity development among ethnic sexual-minority male youths. Dev. Psychol. 35, 1389–1398. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlap, A. (2016). Changes in coming out milestones across five age cohorts. J. Gay Lesbian Soc. Serv. 28, 20–38. doi: 10.1080/10538720.2016.1124351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eliason, M. J., and Schope, R. (2007). “Shifting sands or solid foundation? Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender identity formation,” in The Health of Sexual Minorities. eds. I. H. Meyer and M. E. Northridge (Netherlands: Springer), 3–26.

Google Scholar

Ellard-Gray, A., Jeffrey, N. K., Choubak, M., and Crann, S. E. (2015). Finding the hidden participant: solutions for recruiting hidden, hard-to-reach, and vulnerable populations. Int J Qual Methods 14:1609406915621420. doi: 10.1177/1609406915621420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elze, D. E. (2009). “Strategies for recruiting and protecting gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender youths in the research process,” in Handbook of Research with Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Populations. eds. W. Meezan and J. I. Martin (United Kingdom: Routledge), 60–88.

Google Scholar

Estes, M. L. (2017). “If there’s one benefit, you’re not going to get pregnant”: The sexual miseducation of gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals. Sex Roles 77, 615–627. doi: 10.1007/s11199-017-0749-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everett, B. G. (2013). Sexual orientation disparities in sexually transmitted infections: examining the intersection between sexual identity and sexual behavior. Arch. Sex. Behav. 42, 225–236. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9902-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. M. (2012). Assessing developmental trajectories of sexual minority youth: discrepant findings from a life history calendar and a self-administered survey. J. LGBT Youth 9, 114–135. doi: 10.1080/19361653.2012.649643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. M. (2013). Queering data collection: using the life history calendar method with sexual-minority youth. J. Soc. Serv. Res. 39, 306–321. doi: 10.1080/01488376.2013.766554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, C. M., Lee, M. G., and Boudreau, M. E. (2014). Using mixed-method assessment to advance adolescent HIV prevention. Clin. Nurs. Res. 23, 490–513. doi: 10.1177/1054773813503968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flores, A. R. (2020). “Attitudes toward LGBT rights: political tolerance and egalitarian values in the United States,” in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. ed. W. R. Thompson (United Kingdom: Oxford University Press)

Google Scholar

Floyd, F. J., and Bakeman, R. (2006). Coming-out across the life course: implications of age and historical context. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35, 287–296. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9022-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floyd, F. J., and Stein, T. S. (2002). Sexual orientation identity formation among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: multiple patterns of milestone experiences. J. Res. Adolesc. 12, 167–191. doi: 10.1111/1532-7795.00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, C. A. (2009). “Arousal and attraction,” in Encyclopedia of Human Relationships. eds. H. T. Reis and S. Sprecher (United States: Sage Publications), 110–113.

Google Scholar

Fox, R. C. (1993). Coming out Bisexual: Identity, Behavior, and Sexual Orientation Self-Disclosure. California Institute of Integral Studies. California: Institute of Integral Studies

Google Scholar

Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I., Bryan, A. E., Jen, S., Goldsen, J., Kim, H. J., and Muraco, A. (2017). The unfolding of LGBT lives: key events associated with health and well-being in later life. Gerontologist 57(suppl_1), S15–S29. doi:doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. I., Kim, H. J., Barkan, S. E., Muraco, A., and Hoy-Ellis, C. P. (2013). Health disparities among lesbian, gay, and bisexual older adults: results from a population-based study. Am. J. Public Health 103, 1802–1809. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedman, D., Thornton, A., Camburn, D., Alwin, D., and Young-DeMarco, L. (1988). The life history calendar: A technique for collecting retrospective data. Sociol. Methodol. 18, 37–68. doi: 10.2307/271044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1905). “Three essays on the theory of sexuality and trans,” in The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. Vol. 7. ed. J. Strachey (United Kingdom: Hogarth Press), 123–245.

Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1935). “Anonymous (letter to an American mother),” in The Letters of Sigmund Freud. ed. E. Freud (United Kingdom: Hogarth Press), 423–424.

Google Scholar

Friedman, M. S., Marshal, M. P., Guadamuz, T. E., Wei, C., Wong, C. F., Saewyc, E. M., et al. (2011). A meta-analysis of disparities in childhood sexual abuse, parental physical abuse, and peer victimization among sexual minority and sexual nonminority individuals. Am. J. Public Health 101, 1481–1494. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.190009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, M. S., Marshal, M. P., Stall, R., Cheong, J., and Wright, E. R. (2008). Gay-related development, early abuse and adult health outcomes among gay males. AIDS Behav. 12, 891–902. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9319-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, M. S., Marshal, M. P., Wright, E. R., and Stall, R. (2006). Early Gay-Related Development and its Relationship to Long-Term Health Outcomes among Gay and Bisexual Urban Males. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Public Health Association. Boston: American Public Health Association.

Google Scholar

Frost, D. M., and Meyer, I. H. (2009). Internalized homophobia and relationship quality among lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals. J. Couns. Psychol. 56, 97–109. doi: 10.1037/a0012844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galupo, M. P., Ramirez, J. L., and Pulice-Farrow, L. (2017). “Regardless of their gender”: descriptions of sexual identity among bisexual, pansexual, and queer identified individuals. J. Bisexual. 17, 108–124. doi: 10.1080/15299716.2016.1228491

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasner, T., and Van der Vaart, W. (2009). Applications of calendar instruments in social surveys: A review. Qual. Quant. 43, 333–349. doi: 10.1007/s11135-007-9129-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldbach, J. T., and Gibbs, J. (2015). Strategies employed by sexual minority adolescents to cope with minority stress. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 2:297. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldbach, J. T., and Gibbs, J. J. (2017). A developmentally informed adaptation of minority stress for sexual minority adolescents. J. Adolesc. 55, 36–50. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzales, G., and Henning-Smith, C. (2017). Health disparities by sexual orientation: results and implications from the Behavioral risk factor surveillance system. J. Community Health 42, 1163–1172. doi: 10.1007/s10900-017-0366-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, A. H., Foss, A. H., and D’Augelli, A. R. (2014). Puberty: maturation, timing and adjustment, and sexual identity developmental milestones among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth. J. LGBT Youth 11, 107–124. doi: 10.1080/19361653.2014.846068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grov, C., Bimbi, D. S., Nanín, J. E., and Parsons, J. T. (2006). Race, ethnicity, gender, and generational factors associated with the coming-out process among gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals. J. Sex Res. 43, 115–121. doi: 10.1080/00224490609552306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grov, C., Rendina, H. J., and Parsons, J. T. (2018). Birth cohort differences in sexual identity development milestones among HIV-negative gay and bisexual men in the United States. J. Sex Res. 55, 984–994. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2017.1375451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, W. J. (2018). Psychosocial risk and protective factors for depression among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and queer youth: A systematic review. J. Homosex. 65, 263–316. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2017.1317467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, W. J. (2019). “Sexual orientation,” in Encyclopedia of Social Work. ed. C. Franklin (United States: National Association of Social Workers Press and Oxford University Press)

Google Scholar

Hall, W. J., Jones, B. L., Witkemper, K. D., Collins, T. L., and Rodgers, G. K. (2019). State policy on school-based sex education: A content analysis focused on sexual behaviors, relationships, and identities. Am. J. Health Behav. 43, 506–519. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.43.3.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, W. J., Lanier, P., Jenson, J. M., and Fraser, M. W. (2022). “A multisystems risk and resilience approach to social policy for children, youth, and families,” in Social Policy for Children and Families: A Risk and Resilience Perspective. 4th Edn. eds. W. J. Hall, P. Lanier, J. M. Jenson, and M. W. Fraser (United States: Sage), 1–26.

Google Scholar

Hall, W. J., and Rodgers, G. K. (2019). Teachers’ attitudes toward homosexuality and the lesbian, gay, bisexual, and queer community in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 22, 23–41. doi: 10.1007/s11218-018-9463-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, W. J., and Rounds, K. A. (2013). “Adolescent health in the public health social work section of the American public health association,” in Handbook for Public Health Social Work. eds. R. H. Keefe and E. T. Jurkowski (Netherlands: Springer), 59–80.

Google Scholar

Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., and Ebert, D. D. (2019). Doing meta-analysis in R: A hands-on guide. Available at: http://bookdown.org/MathiasHarrer/Doing_Meta_Analysis_in_R/ (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Haydon, A. A., Herring, A. H., Prinstein, M. J., and Halpern, C. T. (2012). Beyond age at first sex: patterns of emerging sexual behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. J. Adolesc. Health 50, 456–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helsel, C. B., and Harris-Smith, J. (2020). The ABCs of Diversity: Helping Kids (and ourselves!) Embrace our Differences. United States: Chalice Press.

Google Scholar

Herdt, G. H., and Boxer, A. (1993). Children of Horizons: How Gay and Lesbian Teens Are Leading a New Way out of the Closet. United States: Beacon Press.

Google Scholar

Herek, G. M., Gillis, J. R., and Cogan, J. C. (2009). Internalized stigma among sexual minority adults: insights from a social psychological perspective. J. Couns. Psychol. 56:32. doi: 10.1037/a0014672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herek, G. M., Norton, A. T., Allen, T. J., and Sims, C. L. (2010). Demographic, psychological, and social characteristics of self-identified lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in a US probability sample. Sexuality Research and Social Policy 7, 176–200. doi: 10.1007/s13178-010-0017-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Li, T., and Deeks, J. J. (2019). “Choosing effect measures and computing estimates of effect,” in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Version 6.0). eds. J. P. T. Higgins, J. Thomas, J. Chandler, M. Cumpston, T. Li, and M. J. Page, et al. (United States: John Wiley and Sons)

Google Scholar

Hoenig, J. M. (2016). Sexual Identity Milestone Attainment: Understanding Differences among Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Young People. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona.

Google Scholar

Hooker, E. (1957). The adjustment of the male overt homosexual. J. Proj. Tech. 21, 18–31. doi: 10.1080/08853126.1957.10380742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Human Rights Watch. (2009). Together, Apart: Organizing around Sexual Orientation and Gender Identity Worldwide. New York: Human Rights Watch New York.

Google Scholar

Kamody, R. C., Grilo, C. M., and Udo, T. (2020). Disparities in DSM-5 defined eating disorders by sexual orientation among US adults. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 278–287. doi: 10.1002/eat.23193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz-Wise, S. L., Rosario, M., Calzo, J. P., Scherer, E. A., Sarda, V., and Austin, S. B. (2017a). Associations of timing of sexual orientation developmental milestones and other sexual minority stressors with internalizing mental health symptoms among sexual minority young adults. Arch. Sex. Behav. 46, 1441–1452. doi: 10.1007/s10508-017-0964-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz-Wise, S. L., Rosario, M., Calzo, J. P., Scherer, E. A., Sarda, V., and Austin, S. B. (2017b). Endorsement and timing of sexual orientation developmental milestones among sexual minority young adults in the growing up today study. J. Sex Res. 54, 172–185. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1170757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenneady, D. A., and Oswalt, S. B. (2014). Is Cass’s model of homosexual identity formation relevant to today’s society? Am. J. Sexual. Edu. 9, 229–246. doi: 10.1080/15546128.2014.900465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kertzner, R. M., Meyer, I. H., Frost, D. M., and Stirratt, M. J. (2009). Social and psychological weil-being in lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals: The effects of race, gender, age, and sexual identity. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 79, 500–510. doi: 10.1037/a0016848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., and Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. United States: W.B. Saunders.

Google Scholar

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., and Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. United States: W.B. Saunders.

Google Scholar

Lewes, K. (1988). The Psychoanalytic Theory of Male Homosexuality. United States: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Lipsey, M. W., and Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis. United States: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Maguen, S., Floyd, F. J., Bakeman, R., and Armistead, L. (2002). Developmental milestones and disclosure of sexual orientation among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 23, 219–233. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00105-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshal, M. P., Dietz, L. J., Friedman, M. S., Stall, R., Smith, H. A., McGinley, J., et al. (2011). Suicidality and depression disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual youth: A meta-analytic review. J. Adolesc. Health 49, 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshal, M. P., Friedman, M. S., Stall, R., King, K. M., Miles, J., Gold, M. A., et al. (2008). Sexual orientation and adolescent substance use: a meta-analysis and methodological review. Addiction 103, 546–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02149.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martos, A. J., Nezhad, S., and Meyer, I. H. (2015). Variations in sexual identity milestones among lesbians, gay men, and bisexuals. Sexual. Res. Soc. Policy 12, 24–33. doi: 10.1007/s13178-014-0167-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNeill, T. (2013). Sex education and the promotion of heteronormativity. Sexualities 16, 826–846. doi: 10.1177/1363460713497216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meneses, L. M., Orrell-Valente, J. K., Guendelman, S. R., Oman, D., and Irwin, C. E. Jr. (2006). Racial/ethnic differences in mother-daughter communication about sex. J. Adolesc. Health 39, 128–131. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, I. H., and Wilson, P. A. (2009). Sampling lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. J. Couns. Psychol. 56, 23–31. doi: 10.1037/a0014587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. M., and Luk, J. W. (2019). A systematic review of sexual orientation disparities in disordered eating and weight-related behaviors among adolescents and young adults: Toward a developmental model. Adol. Res. Rev. 4, 187–208. doi: 10.1007/s40894-018-0079-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mock, S. E., and Eibach, R. P. (2012). Stability and change in sexual orientation identity over a 10-year period in adulthood. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41, 641–648. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9761-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4, 172–185. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1170757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, J., and Fassinger, R. (2000). Measuring dimensions of lesbian and gay male experience. Measure. Evaluation Counsel. Dev. 33, 66–90.

Google Scholar

Mondimore, F. M. (1996). A Natural History of Homosexuality. United States: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Google Scholar

Monro, S., Hines, S., and Osborne, A. (2017). Is bisexuality invisible? A review of sexualities scholarship 1970–2015. Sociol. Rev. 65, 663–681. doi: 10.1177/0038026117695488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, E. M. (2012). Contemporary issues in sexual orientation and identity development in emerging adulthood. Emerg. Adulthood 1, 52–66. doi: 10.1177/2F2167696812469187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. F., Balsam, K. F., and Rothblum, E. D. (2002). Lesbian and bisexual mothers and nonmothers: demographics and the coming-out process. J. Fam. Psychol. 16:144. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.16.2.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mustanski, B., Kuper, L., and Greene, G. J. (2014). “Development of sexual orientation and identity. In APA handbook of sexuality and psychology,” in Person-Based Approaches. Vol. 1. eds. D. L. Tolman and L. M. Diamond (United States: American Psychological Association), 597–628.

Google Scholar

National Center for Health Statistics. (2017). Key statistics from the National Survey of Family Growth. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nsfg/key_statistics/s.htm#sexualactivity (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

O’Sullivan, L. F., Cheng, M. M., Harris, K. M., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). I wanna hold your hand: The progression of social, romantic and sexual events in adolescent relationships. Perspect. Sex. Reprod. Health 39, 100–107. doi: 10.1363/3910007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Operario, D., Gamarel, K. E., Grin, B. M., Lee, J. H., Kahler, C. W., Marshall, B. D., et al. (2015). Sexual minority health disparities in adult men and women in the United States: National Health and nutrition examination survey, 2001–2010. Am. J. Public Health 105, e27–e34. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ott, M. Q., Corliss, H. L., Wypij, D., Rosario, M., and Austin, S. B. (2011). Stability and change in self-reported sexual orientation identity in young people: application of mobility metrics. Arch. Sex. Behav. 40, 519–532. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9691-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parks, C. A., and Hughes, T. L. (2007). Age differences in lesbian1 identity development and drinking. Subst. Use Misuse 42, 361–380. doi: 10.1080/10826080601142097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parks, C. A., Hughes, T. L., and Matthews, A. K. (2004). Race/ethnicity and sexual orientation: intersecting identities. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 10:241. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.3.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, Z. D., and Muehlenhard, C. L. (2007). What is sex and why does it matter? A motivational approach to exploring individuals' definitions of sex. J. Sex Res. 44, 256–268. doi: 10.1080/00224490701443932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrocchi, N., Pistella, J., Salvati, M., Carone, N., Laghi, F., and Baiocco, R. (2020). I embrace my LGB identity: self-reassurance, social safeness, and the distinctive relevance of authenticity to well-being in Italian lesbians, gay men, and bisexual people. Sexual. Res. Soc. Policy 17, 75–86. doi: 10.1007/s13178-018-0373-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petticrew, M., and Roberts, H. (2006). Systematic Reviews in the Social Sciences: A Practical Guide. United States: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Pew Research Center. (2013a). A Survey of LGBT Americans. United States: Pew Research Center.

Google Scholar

Pew Research Center. (2013b). The Global Divide on Homosexuality. United States: Pew Research Center.

Google Scholar

Pew Research Center. (2015). The Whys and Hows of Generations Research. United States: Pew Research Center.

Google Scholar

Pew Research Center. (2019a). Changing attitudes on gay marriage. Available at: http://www.pewforum.org/fact-sheet/changing-attitudes-on-gay-marriage/ (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Pew Research Center. (2019b). Defining generations: Where Millennials end and Generation Z begins. Available at: http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/01/17/where-millennials-end-and-generation-z-begins/ (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Planned Parenthood. (2020). What should I teach my high school-aged teen about identity? Available at: https://www.plannedparenthood.org/learn/parents/high-school/what-should-i-teach-my-high-school-aged-teen-about-identity (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Puckett, J. A., Horne, S. G., Surace, F., Carter, A., Noffsinger-Frazier, N., Shulman, J., et al. (2017). Predictors of sexual minority youth’s reported suicide attempts and mental health. J. Homosex. 64, 697–715. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2016.1196999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rado, S. (1940). A critical examination of the concept of bisexuality. Psychosom. Med. 2, 459–467. doi: 10.1097/00006842-194010000-00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rado, S. (1949). “An adaptational view of sexual behavior,” in Psychosexual Development in Health and Disease. eds. P. H. Hoch and J. Zubin (New York: Grune and Stratton), 159–189.

Google Scholar

Raffaelli, M. (2005). Adolescent dating experiences described by Latino college students. J. Adolesc. 28, 559–572. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raifman, J., Charlton, B. M., Arrington-Sanders, R., Chan, P. A., Rusley, J., Mayer, K. H., et al. (2020). Sexual orientation and suicide attempt disparities among US adolescents: 2009–2017. Pediatrics 145:20191658. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-1658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainey, W. E., Carr, B. R., Sasano, H., Suzuki, T., and Mason, J. I. (2002). Dissecting human adrenal androgen production. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 13, 234–239. doi: 10.1016/S1043-2760(02)00609-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Regan, P. C., Durvasula, R., Howell, L., Ureño, O., and Rea, M. (2004). Gender, ethnicity, and the developmental timing of first sexual and romantic experiences. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 32, 667–676. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2004.32.7.667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Remer, T., Boye, K. R., Hartmann, M. F., and Wudy, S. A. (2005). Urinary markers of adrenarche: reference values in healthy subjects, aged 3–18 years. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 90, 2015–2021. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rendina, H. J., Carter, J. A., Wahl, L., Millar, B. M., and Parsons, J. T. (2019). Trajectories of sexual identity development and psychological well-being for highly sexually active gay and bisexual men: A latent growth curve analysis. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 6:64. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, A. L., and Hanjorgiris, W. F. (2000). “Coming out: lesbian, gay, and bisexual identity development,” in Handbook of Counseling and Psychotherapy with Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Clients. eds. R. M. Perez, K. A. DeBord, and K. J. Bieschke (United States: American Psychological Association), 35–55.

Google Scholar

Reynolds, M. A., Herbenick, D. L., and Bancroft, J. H. (2003). “The nature of childhood sexual experiences: two studies 50 years apart,” in Sexual Development in Childhood. ed. J. Bancroft (United States: Indiana University Press), 134–155.

Google Scholar

Rice, C. E., Vasilenko, S. A., Fish, J. N., and Lanza, S. T. (2019). Sexual minority health disparities: An examination of age-related trends across adulthood in a national cross-sectional sample. Ann. Epidemiol. 31, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2019.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riggle, E. D., Mohr, J. J., Rostosky, S. S., Fingerhut, A. W., and Balsam, K. F. (2014). A multifactor lesbian, gay, and bisexual positive identity measure (LGB-PIM). Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 1, 398–411. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riggle, E. D., Rostosky, S. S., Black, W. W., and Rosenkrantz, D. E. (2017). Outness, concealment, and authenticity: associations with LGB individuals’ psychological distress and well-being. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 4, 54–62. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, T. S., Horne, S. G., and Hoyt, W. T. (2015). Between a gay and a straight place: bisexual individuals’ experiences with monosexism. J. Bisexual. 15, 554–569. doi: 10.1080/15299716.2015.1111183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez Rust, P. C. (2009). “No more lip service: how to really include bisexuals in research on sexuality,” in Handbook of Research with Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Populations. eds. W. Meezan and J. I. Martin (United Kingdom: Routledge), 100–130.

Google Scholar

Rosario, M., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Hunter, J., Exner, T. M., Gwadz, M., and Keller, A. M. (1996). The psychosexual development of urban lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths. J. Sex Res. 33, 113–126. doi: 10.1080/00224499609551823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., and Hunter, J. (2009). Disclosure of sexual orientation and subsequent substance use and abuse among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: critical role of disclosure reactions. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 23, 175–184. doi: 10.1037/a0014284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., and Hunter, J. (2011). Different patterns of sexual identity development over time: implications for the psychological adjustment of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths. J. Sex Res. 48, 3–15. doi: 10.1080/00224490903331067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. E., Salway, T., Tarasoff, L. A., MacKay, J. M., Hawkins, B. W., and Fehr, C. P. (2018). Prevalence of depression and anxiety among bisexual people compared to gay, lesbian, and heterosexual individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Sex Res. 55, 435–456. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2017.1387755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rostosky, S. S., Cardom, R. D., Hammer, J. H., and Riggle, E. D. (2018). LGB positive identity and psychological well-being. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 5, 482–489. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, E. F., Exner, D., and Baughman, A. L. (2011). The prevalence of sexual assault against people who identify as gay, lesbian, or bisexual in the United States: A systematic review. Trauma Violence Abuse 12, 55–66. doi: 10.1177/1524838010390707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, E. F., Sullivan, M., Keyes, S., and Boehmer, U. (2012). Parents' supportive reactions to sexual orientation disclosure associated with better health: results from a population-based survey of LGB adults in Massachusetts. J. Homosex. 59, 186–200. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2012.648878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rust, P. C. (2000). Two many and not enough: The meanings of bisexual identities. J. Bisexual. 1, 31–68. doi: 10.1300/J159v01n01_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, W. S., Legate, N., and Weinstein, N. (2015). Coming out as lesbian, gay, or bisexual: The lasting impact of initial disclosure experiences. Self Identity 14, 549–569. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2015.1029516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saad, L. (2012). U.S. acceptance of gay/lesbian relations is the new normal. Available at: https://news.gallup.com/poll/154634/acceptance-gay-lesbian-relations-new-normal.aspx (Accessed October 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

Saewyc, E. M., Skay, C. L., Pettingell, S. L., Reis, E. A., Bearinger, L., Resnick, M., et al. (2006). Hazards of stigma: the sexual and physical abuse of gay, lesbian, and bisexual adolescents in the United States and Canada. Child Welfare 85, 195–213.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Meca, J., and Marín-Martínez, F. (2008). Confidence intervals for the overall effect size in random-effects meta-analysis. Psychol. Methods 13:31. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.13.1.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, S. A., Hill, B. J., Yarber, W. L., Graham, C. A., Crosby, R. A., and Milhausen, R. R. (2010). Misclassification bias: diversity in conceptualisations about having ‘had sex’. Sex. Health 7, 31–34. doi: 10.1071/SH09068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C. (1995). An exploratory study of pubertal maturation timing and self-esteem among gay and bisexual male youths. Dev. Psychol. 31, 56–64. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.1.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C. (1998). “…and Then I Became Gay”: Young Men’s Stories. New York: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C. (2011). “Identity development among sexual minority youth,” in Handbook of Identity Theory and Research. eds. S. J. Schwartz, V. L. Vigoles, and K. Luyckx (Netherlands: Springer), 671–689.

Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C. (2019). “Developmental trajectories and milestones of sexual-minority youth,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Sexual Development: Childhood and Adolescence. eds. S. Lamb and J. Gilbert (United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press), 156–179.

Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C., and Cohen, K. M. (2015). Developmental trajectories and milestones of lesbian, gay, and bisexual young people. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 27, 357–366. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2015.1093465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C., and Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual identity trajectories among sexual-minority youths: gender comparisons. Arch. Sex. Behav. 29, 607–627. doi: 10.1023/A:1002058505138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savin-Williams, R. C., Joyner, K., and Rieger, G. (2012). Prevalence and stability of self-reported sexual orientation identity during young adulthood. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41, 103–110. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9913-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrimshaw, E. W., Rosario, M., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., and Scharf-Matlick, A. A. (2006). Test–retest reliability of self-reported sexual behavior, sexual orientation, and psychosexual milestones among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35, 220–229. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-9006-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuler, M. S., Rice, C. E., Evans-Polce, R. J., and Collins, R. L. (2018). Disparities in substance use behaviors and disorders among adult sexual minorities by age, gender, and sexual identity. Drug Alcohol Depend. 189, 139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smiler, A. P., Frankel, L. B., and Savin-Williams, R. C. (2011). From kissing to coitus? Sex-of-partner differences in the sexual milestone achievement of young men. J. Adolesc. 34, 727–735. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Socarides, C. (1968). The Overt Homosexual. New York: Grune and Stratton.

Google Scholar

Socarides, C. W. (1978). The sexual deviations and the diagnostic manual. Am. J. Psychother. 32, 414–426. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1978.32.3.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steingraber, S. (2007). The falling age of puberty in U.S. girls. Breast Cancer Fund; October 9, 2007.

Google Scholar

Strutz, K. L., Herring, A. H., and Halpern, C. T. (2015). Health disparities among young adult sexual minorities in the US. Am. J. Prev. Med. 48, 76–88. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, S. S., Schubert, C. M., Chumlea, W. C., Roche, A. F., Kulin, H. E., Lee, P. A., et al. (2002). National estimates of the timing of sexual maturation and racial differences among US children. Pediatrics 110, 911–919. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.5.911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suurmond, R., van Rhee, H., and Hak, T. (2017). Introduction, comparison, and validation of meta-essentials: A free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 8, 537–553. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M., Sherman, R. A., and Wells, B. E. (2015). Changes in American adults’ sexual behavior and attitudes, 1972–2012. Arch. Sex. Behav. 44, 2273–2285. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0540-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Rhee, H., Suurmond, R., and Hak, T. (2015). User Manual for Meta-Essentials: Workbooks for Meta-Analysis (Version 1.4). Netherlands: Erasmus Research Institute of Management.

Google Scholar

Walton, M. T., Lykins, A. D., and Bhullar, N. (2016). Beyond heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual: A diversity in sexual identity expression. Arch. Sex. Behav. 45, 1591–1597. doi: 10.1007/s10508-016-0778-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, W., Deng, X., Archer, K. J., and Sun, S. S. (2012). Pubertal pathways and the relationship to anthropometric changes in childhood: The Fels longitudinal study. Open J. Pedia. 2:201222020. doi: 10.4236/ojped.2012.22020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, L. M., and Wyatt, G. E. (1994). The effects of childhood sexual messages on African-American and white women's adolescent sexual behavior. Psychol. Women Q. 18, 183–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb00450.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner, L. R. (2008). A best practices guide to intersectional approaches in psychological research. Sex Roles 59, 454–463. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9504-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, R. J., Wheldon, C. W., and Puhl, R. M. (2020). Evidence of diverse identities in a large national sample of sexual and gender minority adolescents. J. Res. Adolesc. 30, 431–442. doi: 10.1111/jora.12488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wohlfahrt-Veje, C., Mouritsen, A., Hagen, C. P., Tinggaard, J., Mieritz, M. G., Boas, M., et al. (2016). Pubertal onset in boys and girls is influenced by pubertal timing of both parents. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 101, 2667–2674. doi: 10.1210/jc.2016-1073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., and Helfand, M. (2008). Ten years of longitudinal research on US adolescent sexual behavior: developmental correlates of sexual intercourse, and the importance of age, gender and ethnic background. Dev. Rev. 28, 153–224. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Siebenbruner, J., and Collins, W. A. (2004). A prospective study of intraindividual and peer influences on adolescents' heterosexual romantic and sexual behavior. Arch. Sex. Behav. 33, 381–394. doi: 10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028891.16654.2c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sexual orientation, identity development, milestones, gay, lesbian, bisexual, queer, sexual minority

Citation: Hall WJ, Dawes HC and Plocek N (2021) Sexual Orientation Identity Development Milestones Among Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Queer People: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:753954. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753954

Received: 05 August 2021; Accepted: 15 September 2021;
Published: 21 October 2021.

Edited by:

Marco Salvati, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Elizabeth Morgan, Springfield College, United States
Karina Gattamorta, University of Miami, United States
Yachao Li, The College of New Jersey, United States

Copyright © 2021 Hall, Dawes and Plocek. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: William J. Hall, wjhall@email.unc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.