Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 January 2024
Sec. Addictive Behaviors

Explicit and implicit effects of gaming content on social media on the behavior of young adults

Daisuke JitokuDaisuke Jitoku1Nanase KobayashiNanase Kobayashi1Yuka Fujimoto,,Yuka Fujimoto1,2,3Chenyu QianChenyu Qian1Shoko OkuzumiShoko Okuzumi1Shisei Tei,,,Shisei Tei4,5,6,7Daisuke MatsuyoshiDaisuke Matsuyoshi2Takehiro TamuraTakehiro Tamura1Hidehiko Takahashi,,Hidehiko Takahashi1,5,8Takefumi UenoTakefumi Ueno9Makiko Yamada,Makiko Yamada2,10Junya Fujino,,
Junya Fujino1,5,11*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Graduate School of Medical and Dental Sciences, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Institute for Quantum Life Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Nara Medical University, Nara, Japan
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
  • 5Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research, Showa University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 6Institute of Applied Brain Sciences, Waseda University, Saitama, Japan
  • 7School of Human and Social Sciences, Tokyo International University, Saitama, Japan
  • 8Center for Brain Integration Research, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 9Division of Clinical Research, National Hospital Organization, Hizen Psychiatric Medical Center, Saga, Japan
  • 10Department of Functional Brain Imaging, Institute for Quantum Medical Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan
  • 11Division of the Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, United States

Excessive gameplay can have negative effects on both mental and physical health, especially among young people. Nowadays, social media platforms are bombarding users with gaming-related content daily. Understanding the effect of this content on people’s behavior is essential to gain insight into problematic gaming habits. However, this issue is yet to be studied extensively. In this study, we examined how gaming-related content on social media affects young adults explicitly and implicitly. We studied 25 healthy young adults (average age 21.5 ± 2.2) who played online games casually and asked them to report their gaming desire. We also conducted an implicit association test (IAT) to measure their implicit attitudes toward gaming-related content. We also investigated the relationship between these measures and various psychological factors, such as personality traits, self-efficacy, impulsiveness, and cognitive flexibility. The results revealed that participants had a higher explicit gaming desire when exposed to gaming-related cues on social media than neutral cues. They also had a robust positive implicit attitude toward gaming-related content on social media. Explicit gaming desire was positively correlated with neuroticism levels. Furthermore, the IAT effect was negatively correlated with self-efficacy and cognitive flexibility levels. However, there were no significant correlations between explicit gaming desire/IAT effect and impulsiveness levels. These findings suggest that gaming-related content on social media can affect young adults’ behavior both explicitly and implicitly, highlighting the need for further research to prevent gaming addiction in vulnerable individuals.

1 Introduction

Online games have grown immensely popular over the past few decades, both as recreation and for specific applications such as mental, physical, and cognitive training (Sailer and Homner, 2020; Dever et al., 2022; Han et al., 2022; Aydin and Kuş, 2023). These games can promote relaxation by allowing participants to leave their regular duties, and experience fantastical environments (Sailer and Homner, 2020; Maldonado-Murciano et al., 2022). Despite these benefits, online gameplay can become an obsession that replaces human interactions, and it may even impair mental and physical health, which has attracted widespread public concern and clinical attention, particularly among young generations (King et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2020; Gülü et al., 2023).

Recently, social media, such as YouTube, Twitter, and Instagram, are increasingly used for communication, learning, and collaboration (Carr and Hayes, 2015; Kuss and Griffiths, 2017). In line with this trend, people are exposed to excessive gaming-related content on social media in daily life (Balakrishnan and Griffiths, 2017). They connect with their gaming community through social media platforms, enjoy watching others play games, and learn how to become better at games. A better understanding of the effects of gaming-related content on social media can offer additional insights into the psychological mechanisms of problematic game use and practical intervention strategies.

The psychocognitive model suggests that the addiction-related cues would trigger positive expectations, which drive the addictive behavior (Hyman et al., 2006; Starcke et al., 2018). Accordingly, a number of experimental studies have investigated reactions to gaming cues, which provide useful information for the pathogenesis of excessive gameplay (Liu et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Ma et al., 2019). However, most previous studies used pictures or videos of familiar games as stimuli, without contextual information; thus, reaction to context-relevant gaming content on social media remains largely unknown. A behavioral study using real-world gaming-related cues on social media should improve ecological validity and expand the findings of previous studies of gaming cue reactivity.

Previous studies have used explicit self-report to evaluate the effects of gaming cues. However, self-reported responses to addictive content predict only some variabilities in future behavior (Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Garrison et al., 2018). Additional variability in behavior might be explained by implicit processes inaccessible to conscious awareness but not captured by self-report (Falk et al., 2010; Garrison et al., 2018). Gaming-related content on social media are pervasive environmental cues that people attend to both consciously and unconsciously (Balakrishnan and Griffiths, 2017). Thus, the effects of gaming-related cues on social media should be assessed explicitly and implicitly to get a broader picture of them.

To date, several factors are known to influence gaming addiction. In particular, personality traits, self-efficacy, impulsivity, and cognitive flexibility (CF) are repeatedly associated with individual differences in vulnerability to problematic game use. Neuroticism was identified as an essential factor influencing problematic online gaming (Chu et al., 2022). Chung et al. (2020) showed significant differences in self-efficacy according to the extent of gaming. Individuals that are more impulsive spend more time playing video games (Gentile et al., 2012). Regarding CF, recent studies have found an association between reduced CF and the severity of gaming addiction in individuals with internet gaming disorder (IGD) (Weinstein et al., 2017; Yang X. et al., 2023). Furthermore, these factors have been reported to play important roles in cue reactivity. For example, Glautier et al. (2000) showed that the levels of neuroticism and extraversion were associated with alcohol cue reactivity. A strategy focused on self-efficacy reportedly reduces cue-induced craving for addictive behaviors such as smoking (Loeber et al., 2006; Ono et al., 2018). Previous studies found that impulsivity moderated the relationship between food cue reactivity and food intake (van den Akker et al., 2014). Cocaine-dependent individuals are reportedly vulnerable to impairments in CF and inhibition in the face of stress and cue reactivity (Milivojevic et al., 2017). Thus, investigating the relationship between cue reactivity and these factors is vital to detect individuals susceptible to gaming-related content on social media.

This study investigated the explicit and implicit effects of gaming-related content on social media among young adults. For explicit measures, self-reported cue-induced desire for gaming was estimated. Regarding implicit measures, the implicit association test (IAT) was employed based on previous studies (Ames et al., 2014; Snagowski et al., 2015; Roh et al., 2018). We hypothesized that gaming-related content on social media significantly influenced participants’ behavior both explicitly and implicitly and that levels of cue reactivity were significantly correlated with the abovementioned factors influencing gaming addiction and cue reactivity. The hypothetical framework diagram is depicted Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study’s hypothetical framework.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

The study enrolled 26 healthy volunteers aged between 19 and 25 who played online games casually. Only male participants were selected because of a sex difference in the mechanism for gaming addiction, and men have higher gaming addiction potential than women (Dong et al., 2018, 2019; Wang et al., 2022). One participant faced a technical error during data collection, so data obtained from 25 participants were analyzed. All participants played online games regularly for at least an hour weekly and did not meet the IGD criteria proposed by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition (DSM-5). They also did not have any psychiatric disorders, history of head trauma, serious medical or surgical illness, or substance abuse. Additionally, none of the participants were current smokers. To estimate the participants’ levels of gameplay, weekly gaming time and gaming history were assessed based on previous studies (Dong et al., 2018, 2019). In addition, based on previous studies (Dong et al., 2018, 2019), all participants completed the internet addiction test (Young, 1998; Ishii et al., 2023) to measure the degree of dependence on the internet. Predicted IQ was estimated using the Japanese version of the National Adult Reading Test short form (Matsuoka et al., 2006). Details are described in the Supplementary methods.

This study was approved by the institutional review board of Tokyo Medical and Dental University Hospital and was conducted in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association. After explaining the entire study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

2.2 Behavioral tasks

2.2.1 Explicit desire task

To estimate the participants’ explicit gaming desire, we designed the EDT based on the methodologies in the previous studies of substance and behavioral addiction (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2011; Courtney et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2017; Dong et al., 2019). Sixteen gaming-related videos such as persons enjoying a game, introducing a game, and teaching how to capture a game from social media platforms were selected. A game in each video was different from each other. The 16 games featured in the videos were all popular online games (shooting games, role-playing games, puzzle games, or sports games) in Japan. Sixteen neutral videos (nongaming videos, such as furniture-, hygiene-, travel-, and work-related videos) were also selected from social media platforms. Based on the previous studies (Tapert et al., 2003; Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2011; Chen Y. et al., 2018), they were chosen to match as much as possible each gaming-related video on complexity, content, design, luminance, color, action, and presence of faces. Details are described in Supplementary methods.

The videos were displayed pseudo-randomly, and after each video was presented, the participants were asked to rate the gaming desire from 1 (no desire) to 9 (extreme desire) (Figure 2). The experiment was conducted using E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The levels of participants’ explicit gaming desire were computed as follows: mean scores of the gaming desire in gaming-related videos–those in neutral ones. Higher scores denote higher levels of explicit gaming desire.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Explicit desire task (EDT). After each video was presented, participants were asked to rate the gaming desire from 1 (no desire) to 9 (extreme desire).

2.2.2 IAT

In this study, the IAT (Greenwald et al., 1998, 2003) was modified with gaming-related pictures on social media. Previous studies have revealed that the IAT reliably predicts addictive behaviors (Ames et al., 2014; Snagowski et al., 2015; Roh et al., 2018).

To assess individual differences in the levels of implicit positive attitudes toward gaming-related content on social media, the IAT evaluated the extent to which participants associated game vs. work (target categories) with positive vs. negative (attribute categories) (Figure 3). Four items were selected as the to-be-sorted stimuli for each concept and the present IAT comprised seven blocks according to the standard procedures (Greenwald et al., 1998, 2003; Fujino et al., 2017; Tei et al., 2018). Similar to the EDT, four gaming-related pictures were captured from the social media platforms for the target concept of game. The 4 games featured in the pictures were all popular online games (shooting games, role-playing games, puzzle games, or sports games) in Japan. For the target concept of work, four work-related pictures were also selected from social media platforms. They were selected to match each gaming-related picture on complexity, content, design, luminance, color, action, and presence of faces as much as possible. Stimuli from the categories positive/negative were happy, fun, attractive, and excited, and painful, sad, difficult, and boring, respectively. Participants were asked to classify stimuli that appeared at the lower portion of a computer screen into corresponding categories and paired attributes appearing in the upper left or right of the screen. They were told to be as accurate and as quickly as possible. The IAT was conducted using the E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Implicit association test (IAT). Here, participants are asked to classify stimuli that appear at the lower portion of a computer screen into corresponding categories and pair attributes appearing in the screen’s upper left or right corners.

The IAT effect scores were computed based on the improved scoring algorithm (Greenwald et al., 2003). High scores represented quicker associations of game–positive and work–negative (congruent condition) relative to game–negative and work–positive (incongruent condition); thus, higher scores indicate higher levels of implicit positive attitudes toward gaming-related content on social media. Details are described in Supplementary methods.

2.3 Familiarity ratings

Following the tasks, the participants were asked to rate the familiarity of each game presented in the EDT and IAT from 1 (very unfamiliar) to 9 (very familiar).

2.4 Psychological measures

To estimate personality traits, self-efficacy, impulsivity, and CF, we administered the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) (Costa and McCrae, 1992; Shimonaka et al., 1998), self-efficacy scale (Bandura, 2006; Ono et al., 2018), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, 11th version (BIS-11) (Patton et al., 1995; Someya et al., 2001), and CF scale (Martin and Rubin, 1995; Oshiro et al., 2016; Fujino et al., 2019; Sami et al., 2023), respectively. Higher scores indicate higher levels of each factor. Details are described in Supplementary methods.

2.5 Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). As mentioned above, explicit gaming desire and IAT effect were estimated. Correlational analyses between these measures and participants’ characteristics/psychological measures [age, IQ, internet addiction test, weekly gaming time, gaming history, NEO-FFI (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness), self-efficacy, BIS-11, and CF scale] were performed. First, we conducted Shapiro–Wilk tests to estimate the normality of measures (non-normality determined at p < 0.05). Following the previous studies (Solas et al., 2013; Ferreira et al., 2021), correlation analysis was performed using Pearson or Spearman based on the normality of variables. Results were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

3 Results

The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. According to the internet addiction test, no participants showed severe symptoms.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ demographics.

The participants exhibited higher explicit gaming desire by the gaming-related cues on social media than the neutral ones (game 4.09 ± 1.52, neutral 1.63 ± 0.87, p < 0.01). Regarding the IAT, the mean response latency for incongruent condition (game–negative and work–positive) was significantly longer than that for congruent condition (game–positive and work–negative) (congruent 610.2 ± 88.1 ms, incongruent 1090.4 ± 156.8 ms, p < 0.01), which indicated that the participants showed robust and positive implicit attitudes toward gaming-related cues on social media. Familiarity rating scores were not significantly correlated with an explicit gaming desire (Pearson’s r = 0.24, p = 0.24) or the IAT effect (Pearson’s r = 0.26, p = 0.21), respectively.

We did not find any significant correlations between explicit gaming desire/IAT effect and internet addiction tests or weekly gaming hours (Table 2). However, the explicit gaming desire positively correlated with the levels of neuroticism (Pearson’s r = 0.48, p = 0.02) (Figure 4A). In addition, the IAT effect negatively correlated with the self-efficacy (Pearson’s r = −0.41, p = 0.04) and the CF (Pearson’s r = −0.43, p = 0.03) scales, respectively (Figure 4B). That is, the participants with lower levels of self-efficacy or CF abilities showed higher IAT effect. There were no significant correlations between the explicit gaming desire and impulsivity levels (Pearson’s r = 0.36, p = 0.08) or the IAT effect and impulsivity levels (Pearson’s r = 0.26, p = 0.22). No other characteristics/psychological measures significantly correlated with the explicit gaming desire or the IAT effect. Details are described in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation coefficients between explicit gaming desire/IAT effect and characteristics/psychological measures.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlations of explicit gaming desire and IAT effect with psychological measures. (A) Correlations between explicit gaming desire and neuroticism (r = 0.48, p = 0.02). (B) Correlations between IAT effect and self-efficacy (r = −0.41, p = 0.04) and CF (r = −0.43, p = 0.03). CF, cognitive flexibility; IAT, implicit association test.

4 Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first experimental study to investigate both the explicit and implicit effects of gaming-related content on social media among young adults. The results extend the findings of previous studies on gaming cues and may provide additional information to practically address and prevent problematic game use.

As expected, gaming-related content on social media significantly influenced the participants’ behavior explicitly and implicitly. The participants showed higher explicit gaming desire by the gaming-related cues on social media than neutral ones. They exhibited robust, positive, and implicit attitudes toward gaming-related cues on social media. Previous studies have shown that substance-related cues in digital media influence addictive behaviors of the corresponding substance. For example, exposure to alcohol marketing in social and digital media has been repeatedly reported to be associated with alcohol purchase intentions, drinking patterns, or higher levels of alcohol consumption (Lobstein et al., 2017; Noel et al., 2020). According to Chen Y. et al. (2018), their participants, regardless of smoking status, showed high neural responses to e-cigarette advertising images from online resources and exhibited behaviorally that e-cigarette advertisements made them want to smoke more than neutral advertisements. In line with these previous studies on substance-related dependence, the current results suggest that real-world online gaming-related content can elicit gaming urges; thus, this issue deserves further examination to increase our understanding of the pathogenesis of gaming addiction.

The mean response latency for the incongruent condition (game-negative and work-positive) of the IAT was significantly longer than that of the congruent condition (game-positive and work-negative). This finding indicated that the participants possessed positive implicit attitudes toward gaming-related cues on social media. The IAT reveals unconscious or implicit attitudes, automatic preferences, and hidden biases in individuals by measuring the strength of associations between concepts and evaluations (Greenwald et al., 1998, 2003). The IAT has been utilized in various areas of psychological research, such as social cognition (Birmingham et al., 2015; Fujino et al., 2017). In recent years, it has also been employed in addiction research. For example, heavy drinkers will exhibit stronger positive implicit associations toward alcohol use on the IAT compared to light drinkers (Ames et al., 2014). A recent study adapted the IAT for smartphone and internet addictions, finding it a robust and valid tool to use with children and adolescents (Roh et al., 2018). This indicates that problematic use of the internet or smartphones shares common implicit characteristics with substance-use disorders.

The explicit desire for gaming positively correlated with the levels of neuroticism. Neuroticism is a personality trait characterized by the frequent experience of negative emotions and maladaptive coping strategies, including inadequate reactions to stressful situations and interpersonal issues (Suls and Martin, 2005; Fujino et al., 2016; Marciano et al., 2020). Previous studies have identified neuroticism as an essential factor that influences problematic online gaming (Chu et al., 2022). Highly neurotic people use online social networking platforms more frequently, likely to reduce social isolation (Hamburger and Ben-Artzi, 2000; Marciano et al., 2020). Neurotic individuals may find social interactions more rewarding online than in real life when they face difficulties in real social interactions (Amichai-Hamburger et al., 2002; Tei et al., 2020). Therefore, neurotic people may be more inclined to spend time playing online games (Chu et al., 2022). Together with these previous findings, the present results suggest that individuals with high neuroticism levels can be highly affected by gaming-related content on social media, and populations susceptible to gaming addiction should receive attention.

In this study, the IAT effect scores negatively correlated with the self-efficacy levels. Self-efficacy is defined as the belief or confidence in one’s ability to cope with demands in various contexts (Bandura, 2006; Ono et al., 2018; Du and Zhang, 2022). Studies revealed that self-efficacy is relevant to internet gaming addiction like other addictions such as smoking, alcoholism, and gambling. For example, Festl et al. (2013) demonstrated that high levels of gaming addiction are linked to weaker self-efficacy in broad generations. Internet self-efficacy, which is involved in regulating one’s internet usage, is one of the significant predictors of problematic internet use (Ceyhan and Ceyhan, 2008; Shi et al., 2011). Regarding cue reactivity, previous studies have shown that a strategy focused on self-efficacy effectively reduces cue-induced craving for addictive behaviors such as smoking (Loeber et al., 2006; Ono et al., 2018). Our findings align with those of previous studies and emphasize that self-efficacy is a critical protective factor for problematic game use.

Furthermore, the participants with lower CF scores showed higher IAT effects. It has been reported that patients with IGD exhibit various types of executive dysfunction (Lee et al., 2020). Specifically, recent studies have shown that both individuals with IGD and those at risk of IGD exhibit reduced CF (Weinstein et al., 2017; Yang X. et al., 2023). Inflexible cognitive processing may produce maladaptive emotions and cognitive biases that exacerbate IGD-related behaviors (Zhou et al., 2012; Chen L. et al., 2018; Tei and Fujino, 2023). For instance, low CF can lead to poor attentional shifting and impulsive control, thereby promoting IGD-related habit formation and dereliction of social or other responsibilities (Tei et al., 2017; Weinstein et al., 2017; Király et al., 2023). However, how these CF features are associated with cue reactivity in gaming addiction is still largely unknown. Our findings will provide an impetus for future neuroimaging studies using ecologically valid cues to better understand the neuropsychological mechanisms underlying this relationship.

Previous studies have suggested that there is a connection between cue reactivity and the severity of gaming addiction (Kim et al., 2018; Ma et al., 2019). However, in this study, we did not find any significant relationships between explicit gaming desire/IAT effect and either the scores of internet addiction test or weekly gaming hours. This may be because the study participants were casual gamers from non-clinical populations, and their game use was relatively low. Therefore, this study’s major limitation is that the results cannot be generalized to individuals with IGD, and future studies should include patients with this addiction.

We found no significant correlations between explicit gaming desire/IAT effect and impulsivity. However, impulsivity is reported to play a significant role in cue reactivity, especially in the context of addiction (Cunningham and Anastasio, 2014). Impulsive individuals often exhibit heightened responsivity to environmental cues, including those related to addictive behaviors. For example, high impulsivity has been noted in cocaine-dependent individuals who express high cocaine cue reactivity (Liu et al., 2011), and a similar relationship has been observed in those of behavioral addictions including IGD (Punia and Balodis, 2019; Şalvarlı and Griffiths, 2019). Impulsivity is linked to a preference for immediate rewards over delayed gratification (Liu et al., 2011; van den Akker et al., 2014). Thus, when exposed to cues associated with addictive behaviors, impulsive individuals may be more susceptible to the immediate pleasure or relief provided by engaging in the behavior. However, relationship between cue reactivity and impulsivity is extraordinarily complex (Cunningham and Anastasio, 2014; Punia and Balodis, 2019). Thus, continued research on this topic is essential to enhance our understanding of managing cue-induced cravings and reducing the likelihood of impulsive engagement in addictive behaviors.

The current findings have practical implications for young adults increasingly exposed to gaming-related content on social media. While such content can positively foster community building, provide information updates, enhance skills, and offer entertainment, excessive exposure can impair mental and physical health (Balakrishnan and Griffiths, 2017). In response, some game developers and platforms have implemented features or mechanisms to mitigate excessive gaming and promote healthier gaming habits (Yang Q. et al., 2023). Parents, educators, and mental health professionals also promote healthy gaming habits to address potential negative impacts. As the effects of gaming-related content on social media can vary among individuals, our findings contribute to the development of a more effective and balanced approach to preventing problematic gaming use.

4.1 Limitations

There are several limitations to this study. First, as mentioned earlier, our sample consisted solely of young casual gamers; specifically, it did not include clinical patients with IGD or individuals who had never played online games. Therefore, caution should be exercised when interpreting our results. However, considering that IGD often emerges during adolescence or young adulthood, when individuals have increased access to online games (Aydin and Kuş, 2023; Yang Q. et al., 2023), the insights obtained from our sample of young casual gamers should offer valuable indications for improved prediction and mitigation of triggers leading to the onset of IGD.

Second, although the games featured in the stimuli were all popular online games in Japan, the varying levels of interest in these games among participants may have influenced the results. The familiarity rating scores were not significantly correlated with the levels of explicit gaming desire or the IAT effect. Nevertheless, while familiarity is a common factor shaping gaming interests (Tan et al., 2016), it can vary among individuals. Some people may develop an interest in exploring new and unfamiliar games, while others may find comfort and satisfaction in revisiting familiar titles.

Third, despite our efforts to select neutral stimuli that closely matched each gaming-related stimulus in terms of complexity, content, design, luminance, color, action, and presence of faces, the use of naturalistic stimuli for the research purpose of incorporating real-world content from social media posed challenges in achieving complete matching across multiple parameters, such as foreground-background ratio and zooming speed. This issue should be further investigated in the future studies.

Fourth, we utilized the internet addiction test, a measure designed for assessing general internet use. Internet-game-related attitudes have been reported to be more strongly related to internet addiction than other categories related to problematic internet use, such as social interaction preferences (Lee et al., 2015; Roh et al., 2018). However, the internet addiction test results may not have necessarily reflected internet-game-related attitudes of the participants.

Fifth, because of the exploratory nature of this study, no correction was applied for multiple comparisons. Thus, our preliminary findings should be interpreted cautiously, and must be carefully explored and replicated in large-scale future research.

Sixth, for the psychological measures, we utilized self-reported questionnaires. It is common to worry about false bias when self-report measures such as the NEO-FFI are used. However, we took steps to minimize the risk of false bias. We explained to participants the purpose of the study and the importance of truthful responses for obtaining valid results. We also assured participants that their responses would remain anonymous and confidential. Further details are described in the Supplementary methods.

Seventh, the sample size was small and consisted of only males. Although the sample size was comparable to previous studies using IAT (Fujino et al., 2017; Tei et al., 2018), it was relatively small for studying individual differences.

Finally, this study was conducted using a cross-sectional design, which precludes any causal conclusions. Thus, future longitudinal studies should provide additional clues to enhance our understanding of problematic game use.

5 Conclusion

This study found that social media content related to gaming can influence on the behavior of young adults explicitly and implicitly. Additionally, the effects of such content can differ based on the levels of neuroticism, self-efficacy, and CF among individuals. Further research using real-world gaming-related cues in digital media can help us better understand the promotion of gaming habits and support individuals vulnerable to gaming addiction.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional review board of Tokyo Medical and Dental University Hospital. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

DJ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. NK: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. CQ: Data curation, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. SO: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. ST: Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. DM: Data curation, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. TT: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. HT: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. TU: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MY: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (23dk0307102h0003), grants-in-aid for Transformative Research Areas (23H04669) and Scientific Research C (21K07544, 23K06981) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (MEXT), JST Moonshot R&D Grant (JPMJMS2295-01, JPMJMS2295-11), and Intramural Research Grant (4-1) for Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders of NCNP. These agencies had no further role in the study design, the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, the writing of the report, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to extend their gratitude to the research team of Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Tokyo Medical and Dental University for their assistance in data acquisition.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1332462/full#supplementary-material

References

Ames, S. L., Grenard, J. L., He, Q., Stacy, A. W., Wong, S. W., Xiao, L., et al. (2014). Functional imaging of an alcohol-implicit association test (IAT). Addict. Biol. 19, 467–481. doi: 10.1111/adb.12071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Amichai-Hamburger, Y., Wainapel, G., and Fox, S. (2002). “On the internet no one knows I'm an introvert”: extroversion, neuroticism, and internet interaction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 5, 125–128. doi: 10.1089/109493102753770507

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aydin, M. K., and Kuş, M. (2023). Nomophobia and smartphone addiction amidst COVID-19 home confinement: the parallel mediating role of digital gaming and social media tools usage across secondary school students. Front. Psychol. 14:1175555. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1175555

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balakrishnan, J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social media addiction: what is the role of content in you tube? J. Behav. Addict. 6, 364–377. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.058

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2006). “Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales” in Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents. eds. F. Pajares and T. C. Urdan (Greenwich: Information Age Publishing), 307–337.

Google Scholar

Birmingham, E., Stanley, D., Nair, R., and Adolphs, R. (2015). Implicit social biases in people with autism. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1693–1705. doi: 10.1177/0956797615595607

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, C. T., and Hayes, R. A. (2015). Social media: defining, developing, and divining. Atlantic J. Commun. 23, 46–65. doi: 10.1080/15456870.2015.972282

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceyhan, A. A., and Ceyhan, E. (2008). Loneliness, depression, and computer self-efficacy as predictors of problematic internet use. Cyber Psychol. Behav. 11, 699–701. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Fowler, C. H., Papa, V. B., Lepping, R. J., Brucks, M. G., Fox, A. T., et al. (2018). Adolescents' behavioral and neural responses to e-cigarette advertising. Addict. Biol. 23, 761–771. doi: 10.1111/adb.12510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., Zhou, H., Gu, Y., Wang, S., Wang, J., Tian, L., et al. (2018). The neural correlates of implicit cognitive bias toward internet-related cues in internet addiction: an ERP study. Front. Psych. 9:421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00421

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, X., Wang, Q., Huang, W., Wang, Y., Yu, J., Zhou, Y., et al. (2022). Neuroticism and problematic online gaming: the mediating role of social anxiety and the moderating role of family socioeconomic status. Curr. Psychol. 42, 13497–13506. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02588-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, S. J., Jang, J. H., Lee, J. Y., Choi, A., Kim, B. M., Park, M. K., et al. (2020). Self-efficacy and clinical characteristics in casual gamers compared to excessive gaming users and non-gamers in young adults. J. Clin. Med. 9:2720. doi: 10.3390/jcm9092720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual for revised NEO personality inventory. Odessa, FL. Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Courtney, K. E., Ghahremani, D. G., London, E. D., and Ray, L. A. (2014). The association between cue-reactivity in the precuneus and level of dependence on nicotine and alcohol. Drug Alcohol Depend. 141, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, K. A., and Anastasio, N. C. (2014). Serotonin at the nexus of impulsivity and cue reactivity in cocaine addiction. Neuropharmacology 76, 460–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.06.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dever, D. A., Amon, M. J., Vrzakova, H., Wiedbusch, M. D., Cloude, E. B., and Azevedo, R. (2022). Capturing sequences of learners' self-regulatory interactions with instructional material during game-based learning using auto-recurrence quantification analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:813677. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.813677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., Wang, L., Du, X., and Potenza, M. N. (2018). Gender-related differences in neural responses to gaming cues before and after gaming: implications for gender-specific vulnerabilities to internet gaming disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 1203–1214. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., Wang, Z., Wang, Y., Du, X., and Potenza, M. N. (2019). Gender-related functional connectivity and craving during gaming and immediate abstinence during a mandatory break: implications for development and progression of internet gaming disorder. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, Z., and Zhang, X. (2022). Analysis of the mediating effects of self-efficacy and self-control between physical activity and internet addiction among Chinese college students. Front. Psychol. 13:1002830. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1002830

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T., Mann, T., Harrison, B., and Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Predicting persuasion-induced behavior change from the brain. J. Neurosci. 30, 8421–8424. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0063-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, S., Couto, B., Sousa, M., Vieira, R., Sousa, N., Pico-Perez, M., et al. (2021). Stress influences the effect of obsessive-compulsive symptoms on emotion regulation. Front. Psych. 11:594541. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.594541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Festl, R., Scharkow, M., and Quandt, T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction 108, 592–599. doi: 10.1111/add.12016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujino, J., Fujimoto, S., Kodaka, F., Camerer, C. F., Kawada, R., Tsurumi, K., et al. (2016). Neural mechanisms and personality correlates of the sunk cost effect. Sci. Rep. 6:33171. doi: 10.1038/srep33171

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujino, J., Tei, S., Itahashi, T., Aoki, Y., Ohta, H., Kubota, M., et al. (2019). Need for closure and cognitive flexibility in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a preliminary study. Psychiatry Res. 271, 247–252. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.11.057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujino, J., Tei, S., Jankowski, K. F., Kawada, R., Murai, T., and Takahashi, H. (2017). Role of spontaneous brain activity in explicit and implicit aspects of cognitive flexibility under socially conflicting situations: a resting-state fMRI study using fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations. Neuroscience 367, 60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.10.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrison, K. A., O'Malley, S. S., Gueorguieva, R., and Krishnan-Sarin, S. (2018). A fMRI study on the impact of advertising for flavored e-cigarettes on susceptible young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 186, 233–241. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentile, D. A., Swing, E. L., Lim, C. G., and Khoo, A. (2012). Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 1, 62–70. doi: 10.1037/a0026969

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glautier, S., Bankart, J., and Williams, A. (2000). Flavour conditioning and alcohol: a multilevel model of individual differences. Biol. Psychol. 52, 17–36. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00022-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gülü, M., Yagin, F. H., Gocer, I., Yapici, H., Ayyildiz, E., Clemente, F. M., et al. (2023). Exploring obesity, physical activity, and digital game addiction levels among adolescents: a study on machine learning-based prediction of digital game addiction. Front. Psychol. 14:1097145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1097145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamburger, Y. A., and Ben-Artzi, E. (2000). The relationship between extraversion and neuroticism and the different uses of the internet. Comput. Hum. Behav. 16, 441–449. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00017-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, T. S., Cho, H., Sung, D., and Park, M. H. (2022). A systematic review of the impact of COVID-19 on the game addiction of children and adolescents. Front. Psych. 13:976601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.976601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyman, S. E., Malenka, R. C., and Nestler, E. J. (2006). Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 565–598. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishii, S., Takagi, S., Kobayashi, N., Jitoku, D., Sugihara, G., and Takahashi, H. (2023). Hyperfocus symptom and internet addiction in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trait. Front. Psych. 14:1127777. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1127777

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., Ha, J., Chang, W. D., Park, W., Kim, L., and Im, C. H. (2018). Detection of craving for gaming in adolescents with internet gaming disorder using multimodal biosignals. Sensors 18:102. doi: 10.3390/s18010102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

King, D. L., Wölfling, K., and Potenza, M. N. (2020). Taking gaming disorder treatment to the next level. JAMA Psychiatry 77, 869–870. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.1270

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Király, O., Koncz, P., Griffiths, M. D., and Demetrovics, Z. (2023). Gaming disorder: a summary of its characteristics and aetiology. Compr. Psychiatry 122:152376. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2023.152376

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuss, D. J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social networking sites and addiction: ten lessons learned. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14:311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. H., Ko, C. H., and Chou, C. (2015). Re-visiting internet addiction among Taiwanese students: a cross-sectional comparison of students’ expectations, online gaming, and online social interaction. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 589–599. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9915-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Lee, D., Namkoong, K., and Jung, Y. C. (2020). Aberrant posterior superior temporal sulcus functional connectivity and executive dysfunction in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 9, 589–597. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S., Lane, S. D., Schmitz, J. M., Waters, A. J., Cunningham, K. A., and Moeller, F. G. (2011). Relationship between attentional bias to cocaine-related stimuli and impulsivity in cocaine-dependent subjects. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 37, 117–122. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.543204

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Yip, S. W., Zhang, J. T., Wang, L. J., Shen, Z. J., Liu, B., et al. (2017). Activation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during cue reactivity in internet gaming disorder. Addict. Biol. 22, 791–801. doi: 10.1111/adb.12338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobstein, T., Landon, J., Thornton, N., and Jernigan, D. (2017). The commercial use of digital media to market alcohol products: a narrative review. Addiction 112, 21–27. doi: 10.1111/add.13493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeber, S., Croissant, B., Heinz, A., Mann, K., and Flor, H. (2006). Cue exposure in the treatment of alcohol dependence: effects on drinking outcome, craving and self-efficacy. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 45, 515–529. doi: 10.1348/014466505X82586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S. S., Worhunsky, P. D., Xu, J. S., Yip, S. W., Zhou, N., Zhang, J. T., et al. (2019). Alterations in functional networks during cue-reactivity in internet gaming disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 8, 277–287. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.25

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldonado-Murciano, L., Guilera, G., Montag, C., and Pontes, H. M. (2022). Disordered gaming in esports: comparing professional and non-professional gamers. Addict. Behav. 132:107342. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marciano, L., Camerini, A. L., and Schulz, P. J. (2020). Neuroticism in the digital age: a meta-analysis. Comput. Human Behav. Rep. 2:100026. doi: 10.1016/j.chbr.2020.100026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, M. M., and Rubin, R. B. (1995). A new measure of cognitive flexibility. Psychol. Rep. 76, 623–626. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.623

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuoka, K., Uno, M., Kasai, K., Koyama, K., and Kim, Y. (2006). Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 60, 332–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01510.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milivojevic, V., Fox, H. C., Jayaram-Lindstrom, N., Hermes, G., and Sinha, R. (2017). Sex differences in guanfacine effects on stress-induced stroop performance in cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 179, 275–279. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.07.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noel, J. K., Sammartino, C. J., and Rosenthal, S. R. (2020). Exposure to digital alcohol marketing and alcohol use: a systematic review. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs Sup 19, 57–67. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2020.s19.57

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ono, M., Kochiyama, T., Fujino, J., Sozu, T., Kawada, R., Yokoyama, N., et al. (2018). Self-efficacy modulates the neural correlates of craving in male smokers and ex-smokers: an fMRI study. Addict. Biol. 23, 1179–1188. doi: 10.1111/adb.12555

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oshiro, K., Nagaoka, S., and Shimizu, E. (2016). Development and validation of the Japanese version of cognitive flexibility scale. BMC. Res. Notes 9, 275–278. doi: 10.1186/s13104-016-2070-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., and Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Punia, K., and Balodis, I. M. (2019). An overview of the neurobiology of impulsivity in gambling and gaming disorder. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 6, 209–218. doi: 10.1007/s40473-019-00190-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roh, D., Bhang, S. Y., Choi, J. S., Kweon, Y. S., Lee, S. K., and Potenza, M. N. (2018). The validation of implicit association test measures for smartphone and internet addiction in at-risk children and adolescents. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 79–87. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.02

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sailer, M., and Homner, L. (2020). The gamification of learning: a meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 32, 77–112. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09498-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Şalvarlı, Ş. İ., and Griffiths, M. D. (2019). The association between internet gaming disorder and impulsivity: a systematic review of literature. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 20, 92–118. doi: 10.1007/s11469-019-00126-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sami, H., Tei, S., Takahashi, H., and Fujino, J. (2023). Association of cognitive flexibility with neural activation during the theory of mind processing. Behav. Brain Res. 443:114332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2023.114332

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, J., Chen, Z., and Tian, M. (2011). Internet self-efficacy, the need for cognition, and sensation seeking as predictors of problematic use of the internet. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 231–234. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimonaka, Y., Nakazato, K., Gondo, Y., and Takayama, M. (1998). Construction and factorial validity of the Japanese NEO-PI-R. Jpn. J. Pers. 6, 138–147. doi: 10.2132/jjpjspp.6.2_138

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snagowski, J., Wegmann, E., Pekal, J., Laier, C., and Brand, M. (2015). Implicit associations in cybersex addiction: adaption of an implicit association test with pornographic pictures. Addict. Behav. 49, 7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solas, M., Francis, P. T., Franco, R., and Ramirez, M. J. (2013). CB2 receptor and amyloid pathology in frontal cortex of Alzheimer's disease patients. Neurobiol. Aging 34, 805–808. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Someya, T., Sakado, K., Seki, T., Kojima, M., Reist, C., Tang, S. W., et al. (2001). The Japanese version of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, 11th version (BIS-11): its reliability and validity. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 55, 111–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00796.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., and Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: a meta-analysis and methodological considerations. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 227–238. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.39

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suls, J., and Martin, R. (2005). The daily life of the garden-variety neurotic: reactivity, stressor exposure, mood spillover, and maladaptive coping. J. Pers. 73, 1485–1510. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00356.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, J. L., Goh, D. H. L., Ang, R. P., and Huan, V. S. (2016). Learning efficacy and user acceptance of a game-based social skills learning environment. Int. J. Child-Computer Interact. 9-10, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2016.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapert, S. F., Cheung, E. H., Brown, G. G., Frank, L. R., Paulus, M. P., Schweinsburg, A. D., et al. (2003). Neural response to alcohol stimuli in adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 727–735. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.727

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tei, S., and Fujino, J. (2023). Adverse effects of empathy and cognitive inflexibility on social trauma. Front. Psychol. 14:1090297. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1090297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tei, S., Fujino, J., Hashimoto, R. I., Itahashi, T., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., et al. (2018). Inflexible daily behaviour is associated with the ability to control an automatic reaction in autism spectrum disorder. Sci. Rep. 8:8082. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26465-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tei, S., Fujino, J., Kawada, R., Jankowski, K. F., Kauppi, J. P., van den Bos, W., et al. (2017). Collaborative roles of temporoparietal junction and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in different types of behavioural flexibility. Sci. Rep. 7:6415. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06662-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tei, S., Kauppi, J. P., Jankowski, K. F., Fujino, J., Monti, R. P., Tohka, J., et al. (2020). Brain and behavioral alterations in subjects with social anxiety dominated by empathic embarrassment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 4385–4391. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1918081117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Akker, K., Stewart, K., Antoniou, E. E., Palmberg, A., and Jansen, A. (2014). Food cue reactivity, obesity, and impulsivity: are they associated? Curr. Addict. Rep. 1, 301–308. doi: 10.1007/s40429-014-0038-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Kobiella, A., Bühler, M., Graf, C., Fehr, C., Mann, K., et al. (2011). Severity of dependence modulates smokers' neuronal cue reactivity and cigarette craving elicited by tobacco advertisement. Addict. Biol. 16, 166–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00207.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Zheng, H., Wang, M., Chen, S., Du, X., and Dong, G. H. (2022). Sex differences in neural substrates of risk taking: implications for sex-specific vulnerabilities to internet gaming disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 11, 778–795. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, T. L., and Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol. Bull. 132, 249–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, A., Livny, A., and Weizman, A. (2017). New developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75, 314–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X., Ebo, T. O., Wong, K., and Wang, X. (2023). Relationships between psychological flexibility and internet gaming disorder among adolescents: mediation effects of depression and maladaptive cognitions. PLoS One 18:e0281269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281269

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Q., Wang, H., Wu, H., Li, W., Zhang, Y., Yao, Y., et al. (2023). Effect of online game policy on smartphone game play time, addiction, and emotion in rural adolescents of China. BMC Psychiatry 23:814. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-05325-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. S. (1998). Caught in the net: How to recognize the signs of internet addiction-and a winning strategy for recovery. New York: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Hu, Y., Li, H., Zheng, H., Xiang, M., Wang, Z., et al. (2020). Altered brain activities associated with cue reactivity during forced break in subjects with internet gaming disorder. Addict. Behav. 102:106203. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106203

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Yuan, G., and Yao, J. (2012). Cognitive biases toward internet game-related pictures and executive deficits in individuals with an internet game addiction. PLoS One 7:e48961. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive flexibility, cue reactivity, gaming addiction, implicit association test, self-efficacy

Citation: Jitoku D, Kobayashi N, Fujimoto Y, Qian C, Okuzumi S, Tei S, Matsuyoshi D, Tamura T, Takahashi H, Ueno T, Yamada M and Fujino J (2024) Explicit and implicit effects of gaming content on social media on the behavior of young adults. Front. Psychol. 15:1332462. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1332462

Received: 03 November 2023; Accepted: 04 January 2024;
Published: 24 January 2024.

Edited by:

Álvaro García Del Castillo-López, Miguel Hernández University of Elche, Spain

Reviewed by:

Yuanyuan Zhang, Dalian Medical University, China
Deokjong Lee, Yonsei University Health System, Republic of Korea

Copyright © 2024 Jitoku, Kobayashi, Fujimoto, Qian, Okuzumi, Tei, Matsuyoshi, Tamura, Takahashi, Ueno, Yamada and Fujino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junya Fujino, jf15psyc@tmd.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.