ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 April 2025

Sec. Cognitive Science

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1516587

This article is part of the Research TopicRisk and Protective Factors, Family Environment and (A)Typical Neurodevelopmental Outcomes - Volume IIView all 4 articles

Joint attention and exogenous attention allocation during mother-infant interaction at 12 months associate with 24-month vocabulary composition

  • 1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 2Developmental Psychobiology Lab, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy

Introduction: Early attentional processes are inherently linked with early parent-infant interactions and play a critical role in shaping cognitive and linguistic development. This study explored how specific early attention mechanisms-namely, exogenous attention allocation and joint attention initiation-during mother-infant interactions at 12 months may influence language development at 24 months.

Methods: A sample of 46 typically developing children was observed at 12 months during mother-infant interactions obtained through remote videotaping. Quantitative measures of exogenous attention allocation to external auditory stimuli and joint attention initiation by the infant were obtained through micro-analytical coding. Language outcomes were assessed at 24 months, with a focus on vocabulary composition (i.e., percentage of predicates).

Results: Findings showed significant negative associations between early life exogenous attention allocation and later vocabulary composition (i.e., predicate percentage). This association was modulated by joint attention initiation: infants displaying lower levels of joint attention initiation showed a negative association between exogenous attention allocation and language development.

Discussion: The findings are suggestive of a complex relationship among different forms of early attention skills and language development in the first 2 years of life.

Introduction

The transition to the third year of life represents a pivotal turning point for language development. Children's vocabulary tends to experience a significant increase at the end of the second year of life and many children start to combine different words (Rantalainen et al., 2021). This phase is often accompanied by a substantial increase in predicates production. Nonetheless, this developmental milestone contributes to individual differences that are only partly explained. In the present study, we explored how early forms of attention at 12 months (i.e., exogenous attention allocation and joint attention initiation) might modulate individual differences in 24 months language development in typically developing children.

Children's vocabulary shows a dramatic increase in the second year of life, as infants go from learning their first words around their first birthday to an average of 300 words by their second birthday, with significant individual variability (Frank et al., 2017). Moreover, a common pattern can be found in the qualitative composition of this first vocabulary. Namely, in the very first phases of language development a larger proportion of words learned are related to first social interaction and everyday life routines whereas as the vocabulary grows a larger proportion of words learned are object names and subsequently, as combinatory abilities emerge, action (such as verbs and adjectives) and function words tend to augment. This pattern has been reported in different languages (Caselli et al., 1999; Choi and Gopnik, 1995; Maital et al., 2000; Tardif et al., 1999). A restricted percentage of predicates has also been proposed as an early marker of language delay in Italian speaking children (Camaioni and Longobardi, 1995; D'Odorico et al., 2001) and the number of verbs that children produce at age two has been shown to be a better predictor of later grammatical skills than the number of nouns (Hadley et al., 2016). Nevertheless, limited research as focused on predictors of vocabulary composition or on potential mechanisms of predicates learning (Horvath et al., 2021; Horvath and Arunachalam, 2019).

Children learn language in environments that are both rich with information and yet ambiguous, where every label can have multiple possible referents (Raz et al., 2019). We know that the parents' ability to provide labels contingently with the child's attentional focus to a specific object can support learning (Suanda et al., 2016; Yurovsky et al., 2013) and early interventions focused on parent-infant interactions capitalize on this core assumption (Buschmann et al., 2015; Fong et al., 2012; Roberts and Kaiser, 2015). Consistent with expectations, numerous studies have documented links between children's attentional abilities and their language development. This association appears to hold true across various attentional skills (Salley et al., 2013).

Attention allocation is guided by endogenous mechanisms entailing the ability to actively maintaining focus on a stimulus (sustained attention) and exogenous mechanisms leading to shifts to environmental stimuli or distractors (distractibility) (Colombo, 2001; Colombo and Cheatham, 2006). Endogenous attention allocation develops early in the first year of life but continues to improve in the second half of the first year, enhancing visual engagement, and attention allocation control (Lansink et al., 2000; Reynolds and Romano, 2016).

Higher levels of sustained attention in 9-month-olds have been linked with better language outcomes 6 months later (Yu et al., 2019). Consistently, 12-month-old infants showing frequent exogenous attention allocation to external stimuli have been described as achieving less optimal language development 6 months later (Testa et al., 2023). Notably, prior investigations in this domain have been confined to controlled laboratory environments. Consequently, the influence of early exogenous attention allocation within naturalistic, interactive social contexts involving infants and their caregivers remains largely unexplored. To bridge this knowledge gap, future research should elucidate how this facet of attention relates to subsequent language development when assessed in ecologically valid settings.

Furthermore, between 9 and 12 months, infants show significant advancements in social attention development, including joint attention engagement i.e., the ability to share their attentional focus on external stimuli with the caregivers (Bradley, 2023). In this developmental window infants are sensitive to other's gaze direction and show gradual increases in gaze- and point-following, responding to joint attention cues (Tang et al., 2024) and further expanding their ability to initiate joint attention, using their own gaze and other social cues to intentionally share attention with others (Stephenson et al., 2021). Links between joint attention and language development have been described, too (Tomasello and Todd, 1983) and social-pragmatic cues remain important sources of information about word meanings even for adults. For instance, infants learn to use the adult's gaze to acquire new words using this cue to develop word-to-world mappings (Hollich et al., 2000). Coherently, significant associations have been reported between language development and child ability to both respond to joint-attention (Delgado et al., 2002; Morales et al., 2000) and initiate joint attention by sending specific bids in the shape of gaze triangulation and early communicative behaviors (e.g., pointing; Bavin et al., 2008; Brooks and Meltzoff, 2008; McGillion et al., 2017). As previously highlighted, evidence of a link between early life exogenous attention allocation and later cognitive and language development is present in the literature (Colombo and Mitchell, 2009; Hendry et al., 2019; Wass, 2015). Although multiple studies have shown that children are active agents in their environment and their abilities and motivations greatly impact learning mechanisms (Thelen, 1995), previous research has only partly focused on the role played by children's initiation of joint attention in influencing later language outcomes.

Underlying causes of individual variability in language development are still partly unknown, as many studies on language development predictors only explain a small portion of the difference in vocabulary size (Reilly et al., 2018). Moreover, studies often investigate the main effect of potential predictors and don't account for interaction effects between predictors; conversely recent studies have highlighted the presence of complex and cumulative effects of risk factors in later development (Eadie et al., 2022). For instance, the presence of multiple risk factors has been shown to increase the probability of language delays in the general population (Hayiou-Thomas et al., 2021). However, limited research has investigated potential interactive effects between attention related measures and later language development. This dearth of evidence is regrettable considering that language development (as development in general) is shaped by diverse, interconnected, interdependent mechanisms, and its nature should prompt us to embrace complexity (D'souza et al., 2017). These dynamic interconnected effects are even more relevant in the case of attentional risk and protective factors as attentional mechanisms can both support one another (e.g., Fisher, 2019) or act in competition (e.g., Lee and Schumacher, 2024). For instance, limited studies have investigated the potential moderating effect of joint attention on the link between early individual cognitive differences and later language development (e.g., Canfield and Saudino, 2016; Salley et al., 2013) with contrasting results. For instance, recent studies have shown that infant sustained attention in the context of joint attention, but not joint attention itself, seem to be a stronger predictor of later vocabulary size, suggesting that joint attention may not just co-occur with infant sustained attention but may play a key supportive role (Yu et al., 2019). As such good joint attention skills may increase the protective effect of sustained attention or reduce the reported negative effect of frequent exogenous attention allocation on later development. On the other hand, research has shown that individual susceptibility to environmental cues in early life (i.e., environmental sensitivity—Bahrick et al., 2018; Moyano et al., 2023) can make individuals more malleable to both its positive and negative influences, essentially making them more responsive to both risk and protective factors (Greven et al., 2019). As such exogenous attention allocation—framed as an index of environmental sensitivity i.e., heightened openness to the environment—may act as a mediator in word learning enhancing or reducing the reported positive effects of joint attention engagement.

Thus, the goals of the present study were: (a) to investigate associations between 12-month exogenous attention allocation (to social auditory stimuli), joint attention initiation—measured in real-life social settings—and language development at 24 months assessing both vocabulary dimension and composition; (b) to explore the presence of interactive/modulating effects between 12-month infants' exogenous attention allocation and initiation of joint attention in predicting later language development.

Methods

Participants

Forty-six children and their families participated in the study as part of a spin-off follow-up of a longitudinal study on the association between early environmental exposures and child cognitive and emotional developmental trajectories—masked for peer review—. Families were recruited at birth from 10 local Neonatal Units in—masked for peer review—. All children were born at term, from healthy pregnancies, and had no neurological or sensory deficit diagnoses. Two waves of data collection have been selected for the purposes of the present study: 12-month-age (T1) and 24-month-age (T2). The study was approved by the—masked for peer review—Ethics Committees. All families provided informed consent prior to participating in the study.

Procedures and measures

Observational measures of attention

At infants' 12-month-age (a relevant developmental window where the first word production typically emerges), mothers and infants participated to a video-recorded face-to-face interaction via teleconferencing. Mothers were instructed to position their child in a highchair and sit a short distance away to facilitate interaction. They were advised not to use toys or pacifiers. The device used for the connection was to be set horizontally, providing a clear view of both partners' torsos and faces, with the screen set to blank. The procedure comprised five consecutive episodes (see masked reference for blind review). Initially, they engaged in unstructured face-to-face interaction for 2 min. Next, researchers introduced a series of three sounds, presented one at a time. These sounds included both socially meaningful utterances like “hello” and “how nice,” as well as neutral sounds like flowing water and a mixer. Each sound had the same duration. While the sounds played (30 s), mothers were instructed to maintain neutral expressions and avoid talking to their children. The request made to the caregiver aimed to maintain the child's motivational state, encouraging them to re-engage the adult interactive partner that has stopped the interaction abruptly. This approach aligns with previous studies on infants' responses to brief interruptions and manipulations of face-to-face interactions, which highlight the importance of sustaining engagement in social exchanges (Mesman et al., 2009; Provenzi et al., 2016). After each sound exposure, a play resumption period of 30 s allowed for free interaction again. This exposure-reprise sequence was repeated four times, with the order of sounds varied between mother-child pairs to control for order effects. By employing a blank screen on the device used for the connection, the study focused solely on how mothers and children responded to the auditory stimuli within the context of their ongoing interaction. Consistent with the goals of the present study, socially meaningful exposure episodes have been selected for data analysis.

Evaluation of vocabulary development

The mothers filled in an Italian adaptation of the McArthur Bates Communication Development Inventory (CDI; Fenson et al., 1993), the “Il primo vocabolario del bambino” Words and Action/Gestures short form (Caselli et al., 2015). The questionnaire is normed on a sample of typically developing Italian children between the ages of 8 and 30 months. The Words and Action/Gestures short form assesses the onset of communication skills, between 8 and 24 months of the infants' life. The first section of the questionnaire comprise a list of 100 words; the parent is asked to indicate if the child understands and/or produces each word. Qualitative vocabulary composition was obtained for four major word categories based on operational procedures provided by Bates et al. (1994): (a) Social words—containing sound effects and animal sounds, names for people, and games and routines (b) Nouns—including semantic categories with a clear naming function: animals, vehicles, toys, food and drinks, clothing, body parts, household objects, and furniture and house rooms (c) Predicates—containing two semantic categories: action words (verbs) and descriptive words (adjectives) (d) Closed-class words—including pronouns, prepositions, question words, quantifiers and articles, and connecting words.

Data reduction

Observational measures of attention

Exposure episodes were micro-analytically coded. Exogenous attention allocation (EAA) was coded as the proportion of time infant's face and/or gaze was clearly directed toward the auditory stimulus source, as an index of time spent looking at the distracting stimulus. Conversely, the proportion of time infant's face and/or gaze was directed to the mother was coded as Social Attention to mother (SAM). Joint attention initiation (JAI) was coded as the frequency of occurrence of the following sequence of three infant gaze behaviors within 2 s of each other: (1) orienting to the auditory stimulus source; (2) orienting to the mother; (3) orienting back to the auditory stimulus source. This allowed us to highlight instances in which the infants used their gaze to signal an attentional shift, triangulating their gaze between the stimulus and the interactive partner. A subset of randomly selected videos (10%) was independently coded by two coders and the inter-rater agreement was above 95% for all tested dyads.

Evaluation of vocabulary development

From the CDI questionnaire we computed the child's vocabulary size as the number of words the parent reported the child to use (CDI production). Composition percentages were computed for the four word-categories: social, nouns, predicates, and closed class words.

Statistical power and sample size estimation

Sample size was estimated based on the final regression model (planned with three predictors and one interaction effect) and setting parameters as follows: f2 = 0.30, α = 0.05, β = 0.20. A total sample size of n = 45 was estimated.

Plan of analyses

Associations between EAA, SAM, JAI, and later language measures were tested via Pearson bivariate correlations. Correction for multiple comparisons (Benjamini-Hochberg false discovery rate procedure) was applied. A regression model was used to assess potential significant interactive effects between EAA and JAI on later language outcomes for all attention-language associations that survived multiple comparison correction. Regression analyses will include control for the potential confounding effect of infants' sex. All analyses were conducted with Jamovi 2.5 for Windows 11.

Results

Characteristics of the sample

The socio-demographic sample description is reported in Table 1. Of the 46 children, 21 were females (46%). Children and their caregiver participated in two waves of data collection: time-point (1) around 12-month-age (M = 12.40; SD = 0.34) and time-point (2) around 24-month-age (M = 24.15; SD = 0.67). Descriptive analyses of variables measured at the 12- and 24-months data collection points are presented in Table 2.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic descriptive statistics.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for T1 (12 months) and T2 (24 months) data.

Vocabulary composition

Looking at our sample vocabulary composition, we observed significant positive associations between the total vocabulary size (number of words produced) and percentage of nouns [Pearson's r(44) = 0.350, p = 0.017] and predicates [Pearson's r(44) = 0.878, p < 0.001]. Conversely negative associations emerged between vocabulary size and percentage of social words [Pearson's r(44) = −0.812, p < 0.001] and closed class words [Pearson's r(44) = −0.303, p = 0.041]. This trend was confirmed observing the composition distributions (see Figure 1A) in 2 sub-groups of children split by vocabulary size (lower 1–50 words n = 15 vs. higher 50–100 words n = 31).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Twenty-four months vocabulary distribution (social, nouns, predicates, and closed class words) in two sub-groups of children split by vocabulary size (lower 1–50 words, n = 15 vs. higher 50–100 words, n = 31); (B) Correlation heatmap reporting Pearson's r values for the associations between 12-month behavioral measures and 24-month vocabulary; EAA, exogenous attention allocation; SAM, social attention to mother; JAI, joint attention initiation; CDI, McArthur Bates communication development inventory.

Attention skills at 12 months and vocabulary development at 24 months

Significant negative correlations emerged for the percentage of time that the infant spent looking toward the social sound source (EAA) during the exposure episode with overall vocabulary size [Pearson's r(44) = −0.323, p = 0.028] and percentage of predicates [Pearson's r(44) = −0.342, p = 0.020; Figure 1B]. Only the latter correlation test survived correction for multiple comparisons (Supplementary material S1). The complete correlation table for all measures collected is presented in Supplementary material S2.

A regression model investigating effects of EAA and JAI abilities on later language (controlling for potential confounding effects of sex) was tested with percentage of predicates as the dependent variable and assessing interaction effects between EAA and JAI (mean centered). The model was statistically significant F(4, 41) = 3.12, p = 0.025 and explained 23.3% of the variance. The results highlight a significant negative effect of EAA [β = −0.416, t(44) = −2.65, p = 0.011] with no significant effects of JAI [β = 0.089, t(44) = 0.537, p = 0.594] and sex [β = −0.287, t(44) = −1.05, p = 0.299]. Furthermore, a trend for a significant interaction effect between EAA and JAI emerged [β = 0.260, t(43) = 1.99, p = 0.054]. Interaction was inspected with simple effects post-hoc analysis (Figure 2A) and significant slopes further specified with the Johnson-Neyman procedure (Figure 2B). In infants exhibiting greater JAI the negative association between EAA and percentage of predicates was weaker (Figure 2). Simple effects post-hoc analyses confirms this observation: the regression slopes for −1 SD, F(1, 41) = 11.71, p < 0.001, and mean levels of JAI, F(1, 41) = 7.01, p = 0.011, showed statistically significant associations between EAA and predicates percentage, whereas the +1 SD JAI level slope did not, F(1, 41) = 0.54, p = 0.466. Conversely, simple slopes based on EAA resulted in non-statistically significant regression slopes between JAI and percentage of predicated for mean and low EAA levels [−1 SD, F(1, 41) = 0.50, p = 0.482; Mean, F(1, 41) = 0.29. p = 0.594] and a slope showing a trend for a positive association between JAI and percentage of predicates only for higher levels on EAA [+1 SD F(1, 41) = 3.98, p = 0.053].

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Regression slopes for EAA (exogenous attention allocation) during exposure – CDI (Communication Development Inventory) predicates % association at three different Triangulation values (-1SD, mean, +1SD) (B) and Johnson-Neyman graph for interaction inspection.

Discussion

The present study aimed at exploring how early forms of attention at 12 months (i.e., exogenous attention allocation and joint attention initiation) might modulate individual differences in 24 months language development (i.e., vocabulary) in typically developing children.

Higher vocabulary size at 24 months—and specifically more advanced vocabulary composition as signaled by the percentage of predicates—was negatively associated with the amount of time infants' exogenous attention allocation was attracted by external social auditory stimuli during the interaction with their primary caregiver. These findings are consistent with studies previously conducted in more experimental settings (Salley et al., 2013; Testa et al., 2023) highlighting negative associations between distractibility and language development. Moreover, the results can add to the current literature on the importance of early attention allocation in impacting later cognitive development at different levels (Hendry et al., 2019). Although orienting to new external stimuli is certainly an important skill, supporting environmental exploration and learning, it can be hypothesized that the role of this ability could be modulated by different contexts (e.g., interactive vs. individual context) and in relation to different developmental outcomes (e.g., socio-cognitive vs. communicative development). Moreover, if we frame exogenous attention allocation as an individual feature potentially related to the concept of environmental sensitivity (Greven et al., 2019), the present results could suggest that higher levels of sensitivity to the environment may expose infants to higher distractibility and be linked to less optimal language outcomes already at 24 months.

Furthermore, interactive effects between exogenous attention allocation and joint attention initiation emerged. On one hand, infants that exhibited less joint attention initiation bids showed a stronger association between such distractibility and language development compared to counterparts that produced higher levels of joint attention initiation. As such, joint attention initiation ability appears to emerge as a potential protective factor in the previously presented developmental association between attention and language. A potential explaining hypothesis for this finding may be that the child's ability to actively bring the adults' attention to a distracting event (i.e., by initiating joint attention episodes) could attenuate the negative effects of distractibility by integrating the distracting event in the interaction. If confirmed, these findings could stress the importance of assessing more complex and interactive effects in early development cascades and the relevant active role of the child on his/her own cognitive development (Thelen, 1995). On the other hand, only infants with higher exogenous attention allocation tended to experience positive associations between joint attention initiation and later language. A potential explaining hypothesis for this finding, could be that being more open and responsive to the environment can increase the positive effect of joint attention initiation episodes—for instance creating more opportunities for concurrent maternal language input—on later early vocabulary acquisition. If confirmed this finding could support evidence on the moderating effects of environmental sensitivity on other risk and protective factors (Greven et al., 2019).

The present study has limitations. Firstly, the sample is relatively small and homogeneous, recruited in an urban middle to high-income area, thus the findings require further replication in larger and more diverse cohorts, ideally including also families from different socio-economic and cultural backgrounds. Nonetheless, consistent with the available literature in Italian speaking children (Caselli et al., 1999; D'Odorico et al., 2001), bigger vocabulary size at 24 months featured a larger predominance of predicates. This suggests that—despite the sample size of our study might be relatively small—the representativeness of language data is preserved. Second, despite its longitudinal nature, the present study is observational and the relationships among variables can only be considered in in terms of associations rather than predictions. Furthermore, the study is focused on the link between early attentional abilities and language; we acknowledge the complex and multi-faceted mechanisms underlying communicative and linguistic development and that other factors may intervene and further contribute to the described association framework. Finally, whereas the 12-month measures have been collected observing mother-infant interactive behaviors, vocabulary was tested via parental questionnaire. The CDI is a widely used tool in language development research; notwithstanding, a combination of parent-reported and direct testing would have allowed a broader assessment of language abilities.

Conclusions

The present study aimed at testing early attention abilities in an interactive social context and assessing not only direct links but also interaction effects between these early abilities and later language. The results of the present study could thus be relevant in expanding our knowledge of early developmental cascades in language acquisition and in supporting the current literature on potential targets for screening and intervention.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article are available in the Zenodo repository and linked by doi: 10.5281/zenodo.12633606. Further enquiries should be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comitato Etico Policlinico San Matteo (Pavia). The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

EC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SG: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LV: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LP: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was partly supported by grants of the Italian Ministry of Health Ricerca Corrente 2022–2024 to the IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the families and children who participated in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1516587/full#supplementary-material

References

Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J. T., and Soska, K. C. (2018). The multisensory attention assessment protocol (MAAP): characterizing individual differences in multisensory attention skills in infants and children and relations with language and cognition. Dev. Psychol. 54, 2207–2225. doi: 10.1037/dev0000594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., et al. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. J. Child Lang. 21, 85–123. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900008680

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bavin, E. L., Prior, M., Reilly, S., Bretherton, L., Williams, J., Eadie, P., et al. (2008). The early language in victoria study: predicting vocabulary at age one and two years from gesture and object use. J. Child Lang. 35, 687–701. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, H. (2023). Qualitative and quantitative measures of joint attention development in the first year of life: a scoping review. Infant Child Dev. 32:e2422. doi: 10.1002/icd.2422

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, R., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. J. Child Lang. 35, 207–220. doi: 10.1017/S030500090700829X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschmann, A., Multhauf, B., Hasselhorn, M., and Pietz, J. (2015). Long-term effects of a parent-based language intervention on language outcomes and working memory for late-talking toddlers. J. Early Intervent. 37, 175–189. doi: 10.1177/1053815115609384

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camaioni, L., and Longobardi, E. (1995). Nature and stability of individual differences in early lexical development of Italian-speaking children. First Lang. 15, 203–218. doi: 10.1177/014272379501504405

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Canfield, C. F., and Saudino, K. J. (2016). The influence of infant characteristics and attention to social cues on early vocabulary. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 150, 112–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caselli, C., Casadio, P., and Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from first words to grammar in English and Italian. J. Child Lang. 26, 69–111. doi: 10.1017/S0305000998003687

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caselli, M. C., Bello, A., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., and Pasqualetti, P. (2015). Il Primo Vocabolario del Bambino: Gesti, Parole e Frasi. Valori di riferimento fra 8 e 36 mesi delle Forme complete e delle Forme brevi del questionario MacArthur-Bates CDI. Milano: Franco Angeli.

Google Scholar

Choi, S., and Gopnik, A. (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a cross-linguistic study. J. Child Lang. 22, 497–529. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900009934

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 337–367. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.337

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, J., and Cheatham, C. L. (2006). The emergence and basis of endogenous attention in infancy and early childhood. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 34, 283–322. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2407(06)80010-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, J., and Mitchell, D. W. (2009). Infant visual habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 92, 225–234. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, C. E. F., Mundy, P., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., and Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development. J. Speech Lang. Hearing Res. 45, 715–719. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/057)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Odorico, L., Carubbi, S., Salerni, N., and Calvo, V. (2001). Vocabulary development in Italian children: a longitudinal evaluation of quantitative and qualitative aspects. J. Child Lang. 28, 351–372. doi: 10.1017/S0305000901004676

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D'souza, D., D'souza, H., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Precursors to language development in typically and atypically developing infants and toddlers: the importance of embracing complexity. J. Child Lang. 44, 591–627. doi: 10.1017/S030500091700006X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eadie, P., Levickis, P., Mckean, C., Westrupp, E., Bavin, E., Ware, R., et al. (2022). Developing preschool language surveillance models—cumulative and clustering patterns of early life factors in the early language in Victoria Study Cohort. Front. Pediatr. 10:826817. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.826817

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., and Hartung, J. P. (1993). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual. New York, NY: Brookes.

Google Scholar

Fisher, A. V. (2019). Selective sustained attention: a developmental foundation for cognition. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 29, 248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, N. W. Y., Ho, S. K. Y., So, B. J. W., and Lian, W. B. (2012). Evaluation of the Hanen it takes two to talk intervention programme. Proc. Singapore Healthc. 21, 251–256. doi: 10.1177/201010581202100406

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., and Marchman, V. A. (2017). Wordbank: an open repository for developmental vocabulary data. J. Child Lang. 44, 677–694. doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000209

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greven, C. U., Lionetti, F., Booth, C., Aron, E. N., Fox, E., Schendan, H. E., et al. (2019). Sensory processing sensitivity in the context of environmental sensitivity: a critical review and development of research agenda. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 98, 287–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadley, P. A., Rispoli, M., Hsua, N., Nippold, M., and Hoffman, L. (2016). Toddlers' verb lexicon diversity and grammatical outcomes. Lang. Speech Hearing Serv. Sch. 47, 44–58. doi: 10.1044/2015_LSHSS-15-0018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Smith-Woolley, E., and Dale, P. S. (2021). Breadth versus depth: cumulative risk model and continuous measure prediction of poor language and reading outcomes at 12. Dev. Sci. 24:e12998. doi: 10.1111/desc.12998

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendry, A., Johnson, M. H., and Holmboe, K. (2019). Early development of visual attention: change, stability, and longitudinal associations. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 1, 251–275. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., et al. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 65, 1–135. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, S., and Arunachalam, S. (2019). Optimal contexts for verb learning. Perspect. ASHA Spec. Interest Groups 4, 1239–1249. doi: 10.1044/2019_PERSP-19-00088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, S., Kueser, J., Kelly, J., and Borovsky, A. (2021). Difference or delay? Syntax, semantics, and verb vocabulary development in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Lang. Learn. Dev. 18, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1977645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansink, J. M., Mintz, S., and Richards, J. E. (2000). The distribution of infant attention during object examination. Dev. Sci. 3, 163–170. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y., and Schumacher, E. H. (2024). Cognitive flexibility in and out of the laboratory: task switching, sustained attention, and mind wandering. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 59:101434. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2024.101434

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maital, S. L., Dromi, E., Sagi, A., and Bornstein, M. H. (2000). The Hebrew communicative development inventory: language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. J. Child Lang. 27, 43–67. doi: 10.1017/S0305000999004006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGillion, M., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M., dePaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., et al. (2017). What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Dev. 88, 156–166. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 29, 120–162. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E. F., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R., et al. (2000). Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 21, 283–298. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(99)00040-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyano, S., Rico-Picó, J., Conejero, Á., Hoyo, Á., Ballesteros-Duperón, M., de los, Á., et al. (2023). Influence of the environment on the early development of attentional control. Infant Behav. Dev. 71:101842. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101842

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Giusti, L., and Montirosso, R. (2016). Do infants exhibit significant cortisol reactivity to the face-to-face still-face paradigm? A narrative review and meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 42, 34–55. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2016.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rantalainen, K., Paavola-Ruotsalainen, L., and Kunnari, S. (2021). Maternal responsiveness and directiveness in speech to 2-year-olds: relationships with children's concurrent and later vocabulary. First Lang. doi: 10.1177/01427237211049585

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, H. K., Abney, D. H., Crandall, D., Yu, C., and Smith, L. B. (2019). “How do infants start learning object names in a sea of clutter?” in Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society (U.S.). Conference 2019, 521–526.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., Cook, F., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Cahir, P., Eadie, P., et al. (2018). Cohort profile: the early language in victoria study (ELVS). Int. J. Epidemiol. 47, 11–20. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, G. D., and Romano, A. C. (2016). The development of attention systems and working memory in infancy. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:15. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, M. Y., and Kaiser, A. P. (2015). Early intervention for toddlers with language delays: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 135, 686–693. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-2134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salley, B., Panneton, R. K., and Colombo, J. (2013). Separable attentional predictors of language outcome. Infancy 18, 462–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00138.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephenson, L. J., Edwards, S. G., and Bayliss, A. P. (2021). From gaze perception to social cognition: the shared-attention system. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 553–576. doi: 10.1177/1745691620953773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suanda, S. H., Smith, L. B., and Yu, C. (2016). The multisensory nature of verbal discourse in parent–toddler interactions. Dev. Neuropsychol. 41, 324–341. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1256403

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y., Gonzalez, M. R., and Deák, G. O. (2024). The slow emergence of gaze- and point-following: a longitudinal study of infants from 4 to 12 months. Dev. Sci. 27:e13457. doi: 10.1111/desc.13457

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tardif, T., Gelman, S. A., and Xu, F. (1999). Putting the “Noun Bias” in context: a comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Dev. 70, 620–635. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Testa, K., McNew, M. E., Todd, J. T., Eschman, B., and Bahrick, L. E. (2023). Infant distractibility from social events mediates the relation between maternal responsiveness and infant language outcomes. Infant Behav. Dev. 71:101840. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: a new synthesis. Am. Psychol. 50, 79–95. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., and Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Lang. 4, 197–211. doi: 10.1177/014272378300401202

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wass, S. V. (2015). Applying cognitive training to target executive functions during early development. Child Neuropsychol. 21, 150–166. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.882888

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, C., Suanda, S. H., and Smith, L. B. (2019). Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months. Dev. Sci. 22:e12735. doi: 10.1111/desc.12735

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yurovsky, D., Smith, L. B., and Yu, C. (2013). Statistical word learning at scale: the baby's view is better. Dev. Sci. 16, 959–966. doi: 10.1111/desc.12036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: distractibility, infancy, joint attention, language, vocabulary composition distractibility, vocabulary composition

Citation: Capelli E, Grumi S, Vercellino L and Provenzi L (2025) Joint attention and exogenous attention allocation during mother-infant interaction at 12 months associate with 24-month vocabulary composition. Front. Psychol. 16:1516587. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1516587

Received: 24 October 2024; Accepted: 24 March 2025;
Published: 17 April 2025.

Edited by:

Anna Gui, University of Essex, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Priya Silverstein, Ashland University, United States
Madeleine Bruce, Duke University, United States

Copyright © 2025 Capelli, Grumi, Vercellino and Provenzi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Capelli, ZWxlbmEuY2FwZWxsaUB1bmlwdi5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.