Skip to main content

CURRICULUM, INSTRUCTION, AND PEDAGOGY article

Front. Psychol., 17 January 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Outcomes: From the Perspective of Interpersonal Emotions View all 14 articles

To Be Expressive or Not: The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Learning

  • School of Educational Science, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China

Understanding the role of teachers’ facial expressions in students’ learning is helpful to improve online teaching. Therefore, this study explored the effects of teacher’s facial expressions on students’ learning through analyzing three groups of video lectures. Participants were 78 students enrolled in three groups: one with an enhanced-expression teacher, one with a conventional-expression teacher, and one with the teacher’s audio only. ANOVA was used to explore whether video lectures instructed by the enhanced-expression teacher were better than those instructed by the conventional-expression teacher and the audio-only teacher for facilitating students’ learning, and what is the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning. The results showed that the video lecture by the enhanced-expression teacher was better than those with the conventional-expression teacher and with the audio-only for facilitating students’ social presence, arousal level, and long-term learning. Interestingly, it was found that the teacher’s emotions could relieve students’ cognitive load. These results explained the inconsistency of existing studies by exploring the mechanism of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning. It also provides teachers with practical guidance for video lecture design.

Introduction

Video lectures are effective for delivering knowledge and have attracted much attention from practitioners and researchers. During the COVID-19 epidemic, video lecture-based learning has made it possible for students to learn at home without face-to-face interaction, avoiding the risk of virus transmission. However, how to minimize students’ negative emotions, such as isolation, anxiety, and loneliness during online learning is one of the biggest challenges faced by teachers and researchers (Zembylas et al., 2008; Reupert et al., 2009; Regan et al., 2012; Xing et al., 2019). Studies support that teachers’ emotions play a key role in students’ learning. For example, Zembylas et al. (2008) found that teachers’ emotional support is essential to relieve students’ negative emotions. Reupert et al. (2009) examined the importance of teachers’ presence and emotions for students’ online learning. Fanselow (2018) found that teachers’ emotions are associated with students’ attention, memory, and motivation. Wang et al. (2019) supported that teachers’ emotions had a positive effect on students’ satisfaction and learning. Additionally, students’ emotions depend heavily on the teacher’s teaching design (Frenzel et al., 2018; Mainhard et al., 2018). It is how teaching behaviors are interpreted by students that determines students’ learning rather than the learning materials themselves (Marsh and Bailey, 1993). The Emotional Response Theory also supports that the connection between teacher communication and students’ learning is regulated by the students’ responses to the teacher’s emotions (Mottet et al., 2006).

Although it is supported that teachers’ emotions are essential to students’ learning, teachers have not realized the role of their emotions in students’ learning in video lectures. Some teachers tend to demonstrate relatively enhanced emotions, while others prefer to be conventional. Some teachers even feel that their expressions should be serious, as education itself is a serious matter (Wang et al., 2019). Hence, an understanding of the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning can provide teachers with practical guidance as to how to behave in video lectures.

The role of emotions in education has recently attracted researchers’ attention, and it is now widely accepted that teachers’ emotions are essential to students’ learning. For example, Neill (1989) found that the teacher’s emotions facilitated students’ learning interest and performance. Similarly, Theonas et al. (2008) supported the belief that teachers’ emotions expressed through facial expressions would improve students’ learning. Wang et al. (2019) analyzed the role of teachers’ facial expressions in students’ learning and found that teachers’ expressions improved students’ learning. Keller and Becker (2020) explored the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ responses and found that teachers’ emotions were related to students’ emotions. Although studies support that teachers’ emotions are essential to students’ learning, there are still some studies which have found that the presence of the teacher had no significant effects on students’ learning (Homer et al., 2008; Kizilcec et al., 2014). That is, the effects of teachers’ presence and emotions on students’ learning are still to be further explored. Exploring the role of teachers’ presence and emotions in students’ perceived social presence, emotions, cognitive load, and learning can improve our understanding of the internal mechanism of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning. It would also be helpful for effective video lecture design.

Literature Review

Emotions

Emotions are involved in every aspect of our lives, and they are key elements in education (Lacave et al., 2020). Emotion itself is difficult to define. It is an experience of a subject’s attitude toward an object mediated by the central nervous system (Kleinginna and Kleinginna, 1981). Scherer (2009) proposed that emotions can be defined as cognitive, motivational, and physiological processes. They influence the way people think, behave, and process social information (Forgas and Eich, 2012). Despite the complex structure of emotions, currently emotions are often divided into two categories: positive and negative emotions (Watson et al., 1988). Positive emotions are often associated with optimism, happiness, and confidence, while negative emotions are associated with isolation, anxiety, and tension. It is supported that positive emotions are often associated with a better competence to pick relevant from irrelevant information, resulting in better performance in cognitive skills (Moridis and Economides, 2009). On the other hand, negative emotions are often associated with information analysis and judgment making (Isen et al., 1985). It is also supported that students’ emotions affect their attention, cognition, and social behaviors (Isen et al., 1987; Chen and Sun, 2012). Lacave et al. (2020) also found that students’ emotions are essential to their learning. Although emotions are essential to students’ cognitive process, the internal mechanism of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning is still to be further explored. Kizilcec et al. (2015) supported that most students favor lectures with the teacher’s presence, while some studies have proposed that teachers’ emotions harmed students’ learning (Homer et al., 2008). That is, there is no consensus on the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning. Hence, exploring the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning can further explain the mechanism of emotions in students’ learning. It is proposed that facial expressions can reflect a person’s emotions in a natural state (Ekman et al., 1978; Keller and Becker, 2020). Thus, teachers’ and students’ emotions were analyzed through facial expressions in this study.

The Role of Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Perceived Social Presence, Emotions, Cognitive Load, and Learning

Although teachers’ emotions are essential to students’ learning (Wang et al., 2019), the specific role of teachers’ emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning have yet to be explored.

The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Perceived Social Presence

Social presence refers to the extent to which a student is perceived as a “real person” in communications supported by media (Gunawardena, 1995). It is the extent to which a student authentically engages in learning interaction with others to reduce the distance between them. Social presence is determinant of students’ online learning assessment (Edwards, 2021). Affective expressions, open communication, and group collaboration are the three factors that determine students’ social presence. The affective expressions represent the extent to which participants share their personal details and emotions. Open communication refers to the extent to which group members trust in group communication and share ideas freely. Group coherence refers to the extent to which group members communicate frequently and effectively, and their readiness to influence and be influenced by others (Byrne, 2021). The perceived social presence determines students’ perceived distance during online learning. Additionally, this perceived distance between students and teachers in the online environment is a considerable challenge for online learning (Barratt and Duran, 2021). The community of inquiry theory supports that teachers’ expressions have effects on students’ perceived presence and online learning (Borup et al., 2012). The emotion expressed by the teacher’s expressions in lectures could shorten this distance (Wang et al., 2019). However, Homer et al. (2008) found no significant differences between the social presence of students who learned in the video or no-video conditions. Thus, exploring the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ perceived social presence would be valuable for improving our understanding of online teaching.

The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Arousal Level

Arousal level is a person’s physiological and psychological awakening state. It is a measure of a person’s perceived energy level, ranging from low (calming) to high (exciting). Arousal has been examined to be associated with cognition, psychology, and learning (Bjalkebring et al., 2015; Hoogerheide et al., 2019). Hence, exploring the effects of a teacher’s emotions expressed by facial expressions could improve our understanding of the effects of teachers’ emotions on students’ learning in video lectures. Arousal levels can be assessed through the change of facial action units (Senechal et al., 2012). A two-dimensional space constructed by Ressel (1980) determined emotions according to two aspects: arousal (inactive-active) and valence (unpleasant-pleasant). The results were validated with facial expression stimuli (Calbi et al., 2017; Schneider et al., 2020).

Teachers’ emotions have been shown to be associated with students’ attention, memory, and motivation (Fanselow, 2018). It has even been found that emotions demonstrate stronger predictive power for students’ learning performance than motivation and cognition (Ruiz et al., 2016; Iskrenovic-Momcilovic, 2018). Robinson (2013) found that teachers’ emotions had a positive effect on students’ satisfaction and learning. Pekrun (2014) also found that students’ perceived emotions during computer-supported learning were essential to their learning performance. Furthermore, students’ emotions greatly depend on the teacher’s emotions (Frenzel et al., 2018; Mainhard et al., 2018). Although it is supported that teachers’ emotions determine students’ learning, the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ arousal level needs to be explored to explain the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ video-lecture-based learning.

The Role of Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Cognitive Load

Intrinsic and extraneous load are two essential aspects of cognitive load (Sweller et al., 2019). Intrinsic cognitive load stems from the difficulty of the learning content, while extraneous cognitive load is generated from the poor design of the learning materials. Since a learner’s cognitive resources in the information processing system are limited, Mayer and Moreno (2003) proposed that the cognitive load from the learning media is the biggest challenge of multimedia-supported education. Homer et al. (2008) proposed that, compared with students in the group without the teacher’s video, those in the group with the teacher’s video experienced a relatively higher cognitive load. Students may feel relaxed when teachers demonstrate positive emotions, while they may feel frustrated when the teacher seems negative. Students may have difficulty when they struggle to understand the teacher’s emotions and the reasons behind those emotions (Schutz and DeCuir, 2002). That is, the teacher’s emotions may improve students’ cognitive load. However, Kizilcec et al. (2015) reported that most students tend to learn from lectures with the teacher’s presence, and supported that the teacher’s presence was essential to students’ video-lecture learning. Wang et al. (2019) also supported that teachers’ emotions have effects on students’ learning and satisfaction. That is, the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ cognitive load is still to be further explored as exploring the role is essential to explain the mechanism of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning, and can provide practical guidance to improve students’ video-lecture learning.

The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Learning

Social learning theory proposes that learning is a process of observing, analyzing, and imitating others’ emotions, attitudes, and behaviors (Bandura, 1997). This theory considers environmental and cognitive factors as two major elements that influence learning. It is proposed that emotions evoked in the learning can influence the learning process in turn. Robinson (2013) explored the interrelationship of emotion and cognition in online courses, and found that emotions can influence students’ motivation and observation. Kay (2008) analyzed the relationship between emotions and students’ learning and found that positive emotions can improve students’ learning. However, Homer et al. (2008) and Kizilcec et al. (2014) found no significant difference between the learning performance of students who learned through lectures with and without the teacher’s presence. That is, there is no consensus on the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning.

Research Questions

Although studies support that teachers’ emotions are essential and can influence students’ emotions, the internal mechanism of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning is still to be further explored. To fill this gap and to provide practical guidance for video-lecture teaching, the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning were explored in this study with quasi-experimental research.

Specifically, this study aimed to explore the following research questions:

RQ1: What is the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence?

RQ2: What is the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ arousal level?

RQ3: What is the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ cognitive load?

RQ4: What is the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ learning?

Materials and Methods

Study Design

A quasi-experiment was conducted to analyze the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning. Participants were randomly assigned into three groups and students of the three groups learned with video lectures instructed by a teacher with different levels of emotions. In the first group, students learned with the video lecture instructed by an enhanced-expression teacher; those in the second group learned with the video lecture instructed by the same teacher with conventional expression; and students in the third group learned with the teacher’s audio of the lecture only. Then students’ perceived social presence, emotions, cognitive load, and learning data were collected to analyze the mechanism of the teacher’s emotions in the students’ learning.

Participants and Materials

In this study, participants were students recruited from a Chinese university through the social network software, QQ. They were normal in terms of their social communication and had no previous learning experience of micro-course design. The fact that the learning process would be recorded was communicated to all participants before the experiment. Participants’ background information including gender, age, and educational level was collected. The experiment schedules were defined during the enrollment. Of the 89 students who registered, 78 were available for the experiment. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 24 years old (M = 20.4, SD = 1.93) with 15 males and 63 females.

Participants were randomly assigned to three learning groups. There were 26 students (5 males and 21 females) in each group. The teacher of the three video lectures was a female teacher with more than 4 years of online teaching experience.

The topic of the three video lectures was micro-course design. The content and teaching design of the three lectures were the same. In the conventional-expression video lecture the teacher was informed to teach in her usual way. In the enhanced-expression video lecture, she was encouraged to express her emotions as clearly as she could. The audio of the audio-only lecture was taken from the enhanced-expression lecture.

Measurements

Perceived Social Presence

To explore the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence, students’ perceived social presence was investigated after the learning session with a questionnaire adapted from Kim and Biocca (1997). Perceived social presence was assessed using six items (as shown in Table 1). Each question was assessed with a 5-point Likert scale (1 “totally disagree,” 5 “totally agree”). There was a reverse question in the social presence scale and its score was reversed before the data analysis. Hence, a higher score indicated a higher perceived social presence. The Cronbach’s reliability (alpha) for perceived social presence was 0.89 which is considered acceptable (Nunnally, 1978).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The six-item questionnaire for the measurement of perceived social presence.

Emotions and Arousal Level

Facial expressions analysis based on FaceReader is more reliable compared with traditional investigation and observation (Sun and Chen, 2016). FaceReader was constructed according to Ekman’s basic emotion theory. In FaceReader, emotions and arousal levels are calculated based on movement analysis of 20 facial action units (Ekman et al., 1978). It has been found to be reliable and effective for assessing a person’s emotions (Drozdova, 2014). Given the reliability of FaceReader, it was used in this study to assess the teacher’s and students’ emotions. The three video lectures were imported into FaceReader to explore the teacher’s emotions. Students’ learning processes were recorded with the camera in the computer, and FaceReader was used to analyze the recorded videos to determine the students’ arousal levels.

Cognitive Load

To explore the role of the teacher’s emotions in the students’ cognitive load, students’ perceived social presence and cognitive load were investigated after the learning session with a questionnaire adapted from Leppink et al. (2013). Cognitive load was assessed with six questions, including two on intrinsic load, two on extraneous load, and two on germane load (as shown in Table 2). Each question was assessed with a 5-point Likert scale (1-“totally disagree,” 5-“totally agree”). The Cronbach’s reliability for the cognitive load scale was 0.90. The reliability for each sub-scale (intrinsic load, extraneous load, and germane load) was calculated to be 0.87, 0.91, and 0.85, respectively, which is considered to be acceptable (Nunnally, 1978).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The six-item questionnaire for the measurement of cognitive load.

Learning Performance

Before the learning, students’ knowledge of this topic was assessed with a pre-test. After the learning, students’ learning performance was assessed with short-term and long-term recall of the learning content. There were 10 questions on short-term and long-term recall which were different from the pre-test. The scores of the pre- and post-tests were between 0 and 10. Items such as “The advantages of micro-courses” and “How to design micro-courses” were included in the test. The pre- and post-tests were examined by two experts in educational technology to ensure their validity for assessing students’ knowledge of micro-course design.

Procedure

There were three stages in this experiment. In the first stage, participants were taken into the classroom one by one. Researchers communicated with them to make sure that their social communication skills were normal, and they had not learned micro-course design before. At the same time, they were administered a pretest on micro-courses. In addition, they received training on how to operate the video lecture. In the second stage, they participated in learning. Students were randomly assigned to one of the video lectures. Each participant learned with the video lecture individually. To explore the effect of the teacher’s emotions on the students’ learning, participants’ learning processes were recorded with the laptop camera. The experiment scene is shown in Figure 1. Each student viewed and tried to learn the content. In the third stage, there was a short-term recall posttest on the knowledge after students had finished learning. Their perceived social presence and cognitive load were also investigated. Finally, five participants in each group were interviewed about their learning experience. Two weeks later, a long-term recall posttest was administered to participants through emails. Those who finished the long-term recall posttest were rewarded with cash bonuses. All participants submitted the long-term recall posttest.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Emotional analysis of a participant.

Results

To explore the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ learning, the two-way ANOVA was used to analyze students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning, using SPSS 19.0. The FaceReader analysis result indicated that there were six facial expressions in the enhanced-expression video lecture, three in the conventional-expression video lecture, and no expression result in the audio-only video lecture.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Perceived Social Presence

Students’ perceived social presence was analyzed to explore the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence. The Shapiro-Wilk test supported that we could not refuse the hypothesis that students’ perceived social presence was normally distributed (w1 = 0.94, p1 = 0.11). The two-way ANOVA results indicated that the teacher’s emotions had a significant effect on the students’ perceived social presence [F(2, 75) = 53.0, MSE = 112.62, p < 0.001]. Specifically, the Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that students’ perceived social presence in the enhanced-expression teacher group was higher than that in the conventional-expression teacher group (p < 0.001) and the audio-only lecture group (p < 0.001) (as shown in Figure 2). Students in the conventional-expression teacher group demonstrated a higher perceived social presence than students in the audio-only lecture group (p < 0.001). RQ1 was thus answered. It was found that students in the enhanced-expression teacher group perceived higher social presence compared with those in the conventional-expression teacher group and the audio-only group. In addition, compared with students in the audio-only group, those in the conventional-expression teacher group perceived higher social presence. That is, the teacher’s presence and emotions improved students’ perceived social presence.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Participants’ perceived social presence in the three groups. The 3.69 and 4.62 represent the Mean difference, ***represents that the mean difference is significant at the 0.001 level.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Arousal Level

Students’ arousal level was analyzed to explore the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ emotions. The Shapiro-Wilk test supported that students’ arousal was normally distributed (w1 = 0.89, p1 = 0.18). The two-way ANOVA results indicated that the teacher’s emotions had a significant effect on students’ arousal level [F(2, 75) = 103.1, MSE = 0.682, p < 0.001]. Additionally, according to the Bonferroni multiple comparison tests, students’ arousal in the enhanced-expression teacher group was significantly higher than that in the conventional-expression teacher group (p < 0.001). Also, students’ arousal in the conventional-expression teacher group was significantly higher than that in the audio-only lecture group (p < 0.001). The result answered RQ2. It was found that the students in the enhanced-expression teacher group demonstrated higher arousal level compared with those in the conventional-expression teacher group and in the audio-only lecture group. That is, the teacher’s presence and emotions improved the students’ arousal levels.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Cognitive Load

The Shapiro-Wilk test supported that students’ cognitive load was normally distributed (w1 = 0.95, p1 = 0.19). A significant difference in students’ cognitive load among the three groups was found [F(2, 75) = 3.29, MSE = 13.94, p = 0.04] through the two-way ANOVA result. Specifically, the Bonferroni multiple comparison tests indicated that students in the enhanced-expression teacher group (p = 0.03) and in the audio-only lecture group (p = 0.04) manifested significantly lower cognitive load compared with those in the conventional-expression teacher group. No significant difference was found in students’ cognitive load in the enhanced-expression group or in the audio-only lecture group (p = 0.71). Additionally, there was no significant difference in students’ intrinsic load and germane load among the three groups. Students in the enhanced-expression teacher group (p = 0.04) and audio-only lecture group (p = 0.01) reported significantly lower extraneous load compared with those in the conventional-expression teacher group. Table 3 demonstrates the post hoc test result of the three groups’ cognitive load. The result answered RQ3. Students in the enhanced-expression teacher group and the audio-only lecture group manifested lower cognitive load compared with those in the conventional-expression teacher group. That is, the teacher’s presence improved students’ cognitive load, but the teacher’s emotions reduced students’ cognitive load.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Post hoc test of the three groups’ cognitive load.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Learning

The Shapiro-Wilk test supported that we could not refuse the hypothesis that students’ pre-test performance was normally distributed (w1 = 0.96, p1 = 0.32). Two-way ANOVA supported that no significant difference was found in the pretest knowledge of the three groups [F(2, 75) = 0.08, MSE = 0.12, p = 0.93]. The Shapiro-Wilk test supported that students’ short-term and mid-term recall were distributed normally (w1 = 0.93, p1 = 0.69). No significant difference was found in the three groups’ short-term recall according to the ANOVA result [F(2, 75) = 0.13, MSE = 0.17, p = 0.74].

While a significant difference was found in the long-term recall of the three groups [F(2, 75) = 16.42, MSE = 25.17, p < 0.001], specifically, students reported higher long-term recall in the enhanced-expression teacher group than those in the conventional-expression teacher group (p < 0.001) and the audio-only lecture group (p < 0.001) through Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test. No significant difference was found in the long-term recall of the conventional-expression teacher group and the audio-only lecture group (p = 0.50). The average scores of the three groups in the pre-test, short-term recall, and long-term recall are reported in Figure 3. RQ4 was therefore answered. That is, compared with students in the conventional-expression teacher group and the audio-only lecture group, those in the enhanced-expression teacher group reported higher long-term recall. It can therefore be stated that the teacher’s presence and emotions improved students’ learning.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Pretest and posttest outcomes of the three groups.

Discussion and Conclusion

The role of a teacher’s emotions in students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, cognitive load, and learning performance was explored. The analysis results supported that the teacher’s emotions facilitated students’ perceived social presence, arousal level, and learning performance. Interestingly, it was found that the teacher’s emotions could relieve students’ cognitive load. Implications of this study are discussed as follows.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Perceived Social Presence

Students in the enhanced-expression teacher group reported a higher social presence than students in the conventional-expression teacher and audio-only groups. Compared with students in the audio-only lecture group, those in the conventional-expression teacher group demonstrated a significantly higher social presence. This finding is consistent with Wang et al. (2019) who found that teachers’ emotions facilitated students’ learning satisfaction. This result can also be explained by the transactional distance theory. This theory supported that students’ perceived communication could influence their perceived distance with the learning community during online learning (Moore and Kearsley, 1996). The distance between students and the teacher is one of the biggest challenges of online learning. Thus, it is essential to shorten the distance between students and the teacher during online learning or video-lecture learning. It was found that the teacher’s enhanced expressions could facilitate students’ perceived social presence, which is essential to shorten the distance between students and the teacher. Hence, it is suggested that teachers could enhance their expressions during video lecture material production.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Arousal Level

It was found that the teacher’s presence and emotions improved students’ arousal level. This finding is supported by Baylor and Kim (2009) who showed that teachers’ emotions expressed through expressions could improve students’ perceptions and learning. It can also be explained by the emotion contagion theory in which the emotional contagion is an automatic, unconscious process of emotional transmission between individuals, an automatic tendency to imitate and synchronize facial expressions and movements, resulting in the production of the same emotion (Liu et al., 2019). The result is also supported by Becker et al. (2014) who found that the teacher’s emotions were determinant of students’ emotions, which could further improve their learning engagement. Thus, it is necessary for teachers to be expressive during video-lecture teaching.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Cognitive Load

The results indicated that the teacher’s presence increased students’ extraneous load, but the teacher’s emotions reduced students’ extraneous load. This result gives teachers practical suggestions for designing effective teaching materials. On one hand, it is consistent with Homer et al. (2008) who proposed that, compared with students in the no-video group, those in the video group experienced a higher cognitive load. On the other hand, it gives teachers practical suggestions to deal with this problem. The teacher’s enhanced expressions during teaching acted as an assisting signal regarding the learning content which could relieve students’ cognitive load. The interview results also supported this finding. Several students proposed that the teacher’s facial expressions during teaching could improve their understanding of the learning content and make it more impressive. This result explained the internal mechanism of why teachers’ enhanced expressions facilitate students’ learning engagement during video lecture learning.

The Role of the Teacher’s Emotions in Students’ Learning Performance

It was found that the teacher’s presence and emotions had no significant effects on students’ short-term recall. This may have resulted from the low difficulty level of the short-term recall and students’ deep memory of knowledge as they had just finished learning. In contrast, the teacher’s emotions improved the students’ long-term recall. The interview data helped to explain this result. Several students proposed that the teacher’s emotions during teaching acted as an indicator, meaning that the knowledge left a greater impression on them. This result is supported by Wang et al. (2019). Compared with Wang et al. (2019), the current study further explored the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ social presence and cognitive load, which can explain the internal mechanism of the role of the teacher’s emotions in students’ learning. This result does not mean that the more expressive the teacher is the better, because the cognitive theory proposes that signals that are consistent with the learning content promote student learning (Mayer, 2005). That is, not all rich expressions are useful for students’ learning. It is the emotional support improving students’ understanding of the learning content that is useful for learning. Hence, we can conclude that the teacher’s heightened level of facial expressions during teaching is helpful for students’ content understanding.

This study gives researchers and practitioners practical guidance to improve video-lecture teaching. On one hand, teachers could heighten their facial expressions while teaching to improve students’ learning, as teachers’ improved expression of their emotions facilitates students’ social presence, arousal level, and learning. On the other hand, although it is supported that teacher presence would improve students’ cognitive load, the teacher’s enhanced expressions could relieve students’ cognitive load.

There are some limitations of this study that should be noted. The number of participants in this study was limited, which influences the generalizability of the results. Secondly, the teacher in this study is a teacher who is not so expressive in her natural state. Thus, the result may only apply to such teachers. In the future, more diverse methods can be considered during the analysis to further explore the role of teachers’ emotions in students’ learning.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Nanjing Normal University. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the Project of National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 62107023), the Humanities and Social Sciences Program of the Ministry of Education (No. 21YJC880076), and the Digital Research Institute of the People’s Education Publishing House project (No. RJA0121007).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank all participants who contributed to this study.

References

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Freeman and Company, doi: 10.5860/choice.35-1826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barratt, J. M., and Duran, F. (2021). Does psychological capital and social support impact engagement and burnout in online distance learning students? Int. Higher Educ. 12:100821. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.100821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baylor, A. L., and Kim, S. (2009). Designing nonverbal communication for pedagogical agents: when less is more. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, E. S., Goetz, T., Morger, V., and Ranellucci, J. (2014). The importance of teachers’ emotions and instructional behavior for their students’ emotions–An experience sampling analysis. Teach. Teach. Educ. 43, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjalkebring, P., Västfjäll, D., and Johansson, B. E. A. (2015). Happiness and arousal: framing happiness as arousing results in lower happiness ratings for older adults. Front. Psychol. 6:706. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borup, J., West, R. E., and Graham, C. R. (2012). Improving online social presence through asynchronous video. Int. Higher Educ. 15, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2011.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, V. L. (2021). “You might as well just all agree with each other:”an initial study of cyberbullying victims’ social presence in online discussions. Comput. Educ. 167:104174. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2021.104174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calbi, M., Heimann, K., Barratt, D., Siri, F., Umiltà, M. A., and Gallese, V. (2017). How context influences our perception of emotional faces: a behavioral study on the Kuleshov effect. Front. Psychol. 8:1684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C.-M., and Sun, Y.-C. (2012). Assessing the effects of different multimedia materials on emotions and learning performance for visual and verbal style learners. Comput. Educ. 59, 1273–1285. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drozdova, N. (2014). Measuring Emotions in Marketing and Consumer Behavior: Is Face Reader an Applicable Tool? Unpublished Master dissertation. Bergen: Norwegian School of Economics.

Google Scholar

Edwards, O. V. (2021). The Impact of the social context on value and expectancy beliefs in an online graduate program: the role of social presence. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 37, 333–345. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., and Hager, J. (1978). The Facial Action Coding System (FACS): A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Action. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S. (2018). Emotion, motivation and function. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 105–109. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P., and Eich, E. E. (2012). “Affective influences on cognition: mood congruence, mood dependence, and mood effects on processing strategies,” in Experimental Psychology, Handbook of Psychology, Vol. 4, eds A. F. Healy, R. W. Proctor, and I. B. Weiner (New York, NY: Wiley), 61–82.

Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Taxer, J. L., Schwab, C., and Kuhbandner, C. (2018). Independent and joint effects of teacher enthusiasm and motivation on student motivation and experiences: a field experiment. Motiv. Emot. 43, 255–265. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9738-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunawardena, C. N. (1995). Social presence theory and implications for interaction and collaborative learning in computer conferences. Int. J. Educ. Telecommun. 1, 147–166.

Google Scholar

Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., and Blake, L. (2008). The effects of video on cognitive load and social presence in multimedia-learning. Comput. Hum. Behav. 24, 786–797. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoogerheide, V., Renkl, A., Fiorella, L., Paas, F., and van Gog, T. (2019). Enhancing example-based learning: teaching on video increases arousal and improves problem-solving performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 45–56. doi: 10.1037/edu0000272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., and Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 52, 1122–1131. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., and Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1413–1426. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iskrenovic-Momcilovic, O. (2018). Learning a programming language. Int. J. Elect. Eng. Educ. 55, 324–333. doi: 10.1177/0020720918773975

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, R. H. (2008). Exploring the relationship between emotions and the acquisition of computer knowledge. Comput. Educ. 50, 1269–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2006.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, M. M., and Becker, E. S. (2020). Teachers’ emotions and emotional authenticity: do they matter to students’ emotional responses in the classroom? Teach. Teach. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2020.1834380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, T., and Biocca, F. (1997). Telepresence via television: two dimensions of telepresence may have different connections to memory and persuasion. J. Comput. Med. Commun. 3:JCMC325. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.1997.tb00073.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kizilcec, R. F., Bailenson, J. N., and Gomez, C. J. (2015). The instructor’s face in video instruction: evidence from two large-scale field studies. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 724–739. doi: 10.1037/edu0000013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kizilcec, R. F., Papadopoulos, K., and Sritanyaratana, L. (2014). “Showing face in video instruction: effects on information retention, visual attention, and affect,” in Paper Presented at the Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, New York; NY, doi: 10.1145/2556288.2557207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinginna, P. R., and Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motiv. Emot. 5, 345–379. doi: 10.1007/BF00992553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacave, C., Velázquez-Iturbide, J. Á, Paredes-Velasco, M., and Molina, A. I. (2020). Analyzing the influence of a visualization system on students’ emotions: an empirical case study. Comput. Educ. 149:103817. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103817

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leppink, J., Paas, F., Van der Vleuten, C. P. M., Van Gog, T., and Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav. Res. Methods 45, 1058–1072. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0334-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X.-Y., Chi, N.-W., and Gremler, D. D. (2019). Emotion cycles in services: emotional contagion and emotional labor effects. J. Service Res. 22, 285–300. doi: 10.1177/1094670519835309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mainhard, T., Oudman, S., Hornstra, L., Bosker, R. J., and Goetz, T. (2018). Student emotions in class: the relative importance of teachers and their interpersonal relations with students. Learn. Instr. 53, 109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and Bailey, M. (1993). Multidimensionality of students’ evaluations of teaching effectiveness: a profile analysis. J. High. Educ. 64, 1–18.

Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning. Santa Barbaras, CA: University of California, 31–48. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511816819.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E., and Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ. Psychol. 38, 43–52. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, M. G., and Kearsley, G. G. (1996). Distance Education. A Systems View. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Google Scholar

Moridis, C., and Economides, A. A. (2009). Mood recognition during online self-assessment test. IEEE Trans. Learn. Technol. 2, 50–61. doi: 10.1109/TLT.2009.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottet, T. P., Richmond, V. P., and McCroskey, J. C. (2006). Handbook of Instructional Communication: Rhetorical and Relational Perspectives. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, doi: 10.4324/9781315189864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neill, S. R. S. J. (1989). The effects of facial expression and posture on children’s reported responses to teacher nonverbal communication. Br. Educ. Res. J. 15, 195–204. doi: 10.1080/0141192890150207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric Methods. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, doi: 10.1007/978-94-010-0462-6_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2014). “Emotions and learning. Educational practices series-24,” in UNESCO International Bureau of Education, Geneva.

Google Scholar

Regan, K., Evmenova, A., Baker, P., Jerome, M. K., Spencer, V., Lawson, H., et al. (2012). Experiences of instructors in online learning environments: identifying and regulating emotions. Int. Higher Educ. 15, 204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2011.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ressel, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reupert, A., Maybery, D., Patrick, K., and Chittleborough, P. (2009). The importance of being human: instructors’ personal presence in distance programs. Int. J. Teach. Learn. Higher Educ. 21, 47–56.

Google Scholar

Robinson, K. (2013). The interrelationship of emotion and cognition when students undertake collaborative group work online: an interdisciplinary approach. Comput. Educ. 62, 298–307. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz, S., Charleer, S., Urretavizcaya, M., Klerkx, J., Fernández-Castro, I., and Duval, E. (2016). “Supporting learning by considering emotions: tracking and visualisation. A case study,” in Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Learning Analytics & Knowledge - LAK, New York, NY: ACM, 254–263. doi: 10.1145/2883851.2883888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: evidence for the component process model. Cogn. Emot. 23, 1307–1351.

Google Scholar

Schneider, J. N., Brick, T. R., and Dziobek, I. (2020). Distance to the neutral face predicts arousal ratings of dynamic facial expressions in individuals with and without autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 11:577494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutz, P. A., and DeCuir, J. T. (2002). Inquiry on emotions in education. Educ. Psychol. 37, 125–134. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3702_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senechal, T., Rapp, V., Salam, H., Seguier, R., Bailly, K., and Prevost, L. (2012). Facial action recognition combining heterogeneous features via multikernel learning. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybernet. Part B 42, 993–1005. doi: 10.1109/TSMCB.2012.2193567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, J. C.-Y., and Chen, A. Y.-Z. (2016). Effects of integrating dynamic concept maps with Interactive Response System on elementary school students’ motivation and learning outcome: the case of anti-phishing education. Comput. Educ. 102, 117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2016.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 31, 261–292. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theonas, G., Hobbs, D., and Rigas, D. (2008). Employing Virtual Lecturers’ facial expressions in virtual educational environments. Int. J. Vir. Real. 7, 31–44.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Liu, Q., Chen, W., Wang, Q., and Stein, D. (2019). Effects of instructor’s facial expressions on students’ learning with video lectures. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 50, 1381–1395. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, W., Tang, H., and Pei, B. (2019). Beyond positive and negative emotions: looking into the role of achievement emotions in discussion forums of MOOCs. Int. Higher Educ. 43:100690. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2019.100690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zembylas, M., Theodorou, M., and Pavlakis, A. (2008). The role of emotions in the experience of online learning: challenges and opportunities. Educ. Med. Int. 45, 107–117. doi: 10.1080/09523980802107237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: teacher’s emotions, students’ learning, enhanced-expression, social presence, arousal level, cognitive load

Citation: Wang Y (2022) To Be Expressive or Not: The Role of Teachers’ Emotions in Students’ Learning. Front. Psychol. 12:737310. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.737310

Received: 06 July 2021; Accepted: 05 November 2021;
Published: 17 January 2022.

Edited by:

Zhongling Pi, Shaanxi Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Jingxiu Huang, South China Normal University, China
Kun Yu, University of Technology Sydney, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yang Wang, wangyang@nnu.edu.cn; orcid.org/0000-0002-4125-9436

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.